Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study

Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study
Using a Novel Camera Technology to Examine
Neural Differences in Self-projection of
Self versus Other Perspectives

Peggy L. 英石. Jacques1, Martin A. Conway2, Matthew W. Lowder1,
and Roberto Cabeza1

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

F

t

.

.

.

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

抽象的

■ Self-projection, the capacity to re-experience the personal
past and to mentally infer another personʼs perspective, 有
been linked to medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). 尤其,
ventral mPFC is associated with inferences about oneʼs own self,
whereas dorsal mPFC is associated with inferences about an-
other individual. In the present fMRI study, we examined self-
projection using a novel camera technology, which employs a
sensor and timer to automatically take hundreds of photographs
when worn, in order to create dynamic visuospatial cues taken
from a first-person perspective. This allowed us to ask partici-
pants to self-project into the personal past or into the life of an-
other person. We predicted that self-projection to the personal
past would elicit greater activity in ventral mPFC, whereas self-
projection of another perspective would rely on dorsal mPFC.

There were three main findings supporting this prediction. 第一的,
we found that self-projection to the personal past recruited greater
ventral mPFC, whereas observing another personʼs perspective
recruited dorsal mPFC. 第二, activity in ventral versus dorsal
mPFC was sensitive to parametric modulation on each trial by
the ability to relive the personal past or to understand anotherʼs
看法, 分别. 第三, task-related functional connec-
tivity analysis revealed that ventral mPFC contributed to the me-
dial temporal lobe network linked to memory processes, 然而
dorsal mPFC contributed to the fronto-parietal network linked to
controlled processes. 总共, these results suggest that ventral–
dorsal subregions of the anterior midline are functionally disso-
ciable and may differentially contribute to self-projection of self
versus other.

介绍

Self-projection is the capacity that allows us to shift our
perspective from the present moment to alternative tem-
poral and mental locations (米切尔, 2009; 巴克纳 &
卡罗尔, 2007; Suddendorf & Corballis, 2007). The neural
correlates supporting temporal simulations of the personal
past during autobiographical memory (是) retrieval over-
lap with those supporting mental simulations of another
personʼs perspective during Theory of Mind (ToM) 任务
(for meta-analysis, see Spreng, 三月, & Kim, 2009). 如何-
曾经, few functional neuroimaging studies have manipu-
lated self-projection within the same individual (虽然
see Rabin, Gilboa, 愚蠢的, 三月, & Rosenbaum, 2010; Spreng
& Grady, 2010) while also using naturalistic stimuli. 在里面
present fMRI study, we examined temporal and mental
forms of self-projection using a novel camera technology
to create dynamic visuospatial cues which provided the
opportunity to step into the personal past or another indi-
vidualʼs perspective.

1杜克大学, 2University of Leeds, 英国

Functional neuroimaging studies have linked self-
projection to medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; 米切尔,
2009). Medial PFC is involved in abstract forms of mental-
izing (Amodio & Frith, 2006; 加拉格尔 & Frith, 2003) 这样的
as integrating social information about the stable disposi-
tions of others and the self across time ( Van Overwalle,
2009). Subregions within mPFC, 然而, may differen-
tially contribute to self-projection. Ventral mPFC is sensitive
to the ability to re-experience the self in time during AM
(Levine et al., 2004; Maguire & Mummery, 1999), and focal
lesions that overlap here impair the subjective experience
of re-experiencing the personal past ( Wheeler & 愚蠢的,
2003). 而且, resting-state functional connectivity has
shown that subregions of mPFC comprise separable net-
作品. Ventral mPFC is linked to the hippocampus and
other regions associated with the medial temporal lobe
(MTL) 网络 (Kahn, Andrews-Hanna, Vincent, 斯奈德,
& 巴克纳, 2008; Vincent et al., 2007), a system of brain
regions important for internally directed processes, 这样的
as memory. 相比之下, dorsal mPFC is linked to dorso-
lateral PFC and lateral parietal cortices associated with the
fronto-parietal network (Vincent, Kahn, 斯奈德, Raichle, &
巴克纳, 2008), a system of brain regions associated with

© 2011 麻省理工学院

认知神经科学杂志 23:6, PP. 1275–1284

controlled processes. Ventral mPFC has been associated
with inferences about oneʼs own self, whereas dorsal mPFC
has been associated with inferences about another indi-
vidual (Krueger, Barbey, & Grafman, 2009; 米切尔, 2009;
Van Overwalle, 2009). 然而, there is considerable
debate regarding the functional specialization of mPFC
with respect to self versus other processing (Northoff
等人。, 2006).

The goal of the present fMRI study was to examine self-
projection of self versus other elicited by naturalistic stim-
uli within the same individuals by employing a novel
camera technology. We tested three main predictions re-
garding the role of mPFC, which were based on the evi-
dence regarding ventral and dorsal subregions in self
versus other processing (Krueger et al., 2009; 米切尔,
2009; Van Overwalle, 2009). 第一的, ventral mPFC will be pref-
erentially recruited during self-projection of self, 然而
dorsal mPFC will be recruited to a greater extent for self-
projection of other. 第二, ventral versus dorsal mPFC will
be sensitive to variability in the ability to re-experience the
personal past and to understand anotherʼs perspective.
第三, ventral versus dorsal mPFC will be functionally con-

nected to separable neural networks contributing to self-
projection of self versus other.

方法

参加者

The study recruited 23 参与者 (12 女性; 意思是
age = 23.7, 标准差= 3.6) who were healthy, right-handed,
and without history of neurological or psychiatric episodes.
Participants gave written informed consent for a protocol
approved by the Duke University Institutional Review
Board.

程序

SenseCam

SenseCam ( http://research.microsoft.com/en-us/um/
cambridge/projects/sensecam/ ) is a small wearable digital
camera that has electronic sensors (例如, 光, heat, motion)
which can automatically and silently trigger thousands of
photographs in a single day (see Figure 1A). This differs

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

.

t

.

/

F


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

数字 1. SenseCam. (A) The SenseCam is a small wearable device that takes photographs automatically, without input from the user. (乙) SenseCam
images acquired during a trip to the ice-cream shop. (C) The experimental design depicting the study conditions.

1276

认知神经科学杂志

体积 23, 数字 6

considerably from the normal way in which we can use a
camera to generate retrieval cues (英石. Jacques, 鲁宾, Labar,
& Cabeza, 2008; Cabeza et al., 2004) because it does not
disrupt the ongoing experience of events through the act
of taking a photograph (also see Levine et al., 2004). 一些
photographs taken from a particular event (例如, eating ice-
cream; see Figure 1B) can be consecutively viewed to cre-
ate a dynamic visuospatial cue (www.youtube.com/watch?
v=sr1i-sICafs). The SenseCam lens also maximizes the
field of view to better capture the perspective of the wear-
er by incorporating a wide-angle (fish-eye) lens. 总共,
the SenseCam captures dynamic images taken from the
first-person perspective, which provide highly effective
cues for self-projection to alternative self versus other
perspectives.

Prospective Collection

Retrieval cues were prospectively collected, such that par-
ticipants were asked to keep a record of their lives prior
to the scanning session by wearing the SenseCam. Partici-
pants wore the SenseCam for 6 days and kept a schedule
of each dayʼs activities to be used to parse the SenseCam
images into events. The daily schedule was recorded at
the end of each day, along with a unique identifier to dis-
tinguish that particular day from others (例如, “Today was
the day I had lunch with Ben”). Participants were instructed
to write about 10 到 15 brief sentences, one for each major
event during the day (例如, “Had breakfast,” “went to the
grocery store”) which were used to segregate the images
into events.

Cue Selection

Three days were randomly assigned to elicit self-projection
of oneʼs own self (SPS). The content of the remaining 3 天
was used for an analysis that was not the main focus of
the present investigation (英石. Jacques, 康威, & Cabeza,
under review). For each day, 12 events were selected to
be tested in the scanner. To elicit self-projection of other
perspectives (SPO), images were collected by three volun-
teers who wore a SenseCam in locations at least 100 miles
away from Duke University and whose lifestyles differed
from the participants. Images in the SPO condition depicted
events that would have been familiar to the participants (例如,
going to the grocery store, eating at a restaurant), but which
were not self-relevant, thus minimizing the likelihood that
the other-perspective condition would include pictures of
self-relevant people, 地方, and activities, which might in-
advertently trigger AMs. Debriefing following the scanning
session indicated that the SPO images were familiar to
many of the participants, but did not trigger personal mem-
ories. The SPO images were also carefully selected to be
similar to the SPS images (indoor/outdoor, time of day,
ETC。). Images in both conditions were selected to ensure
good picture quality.

fMRI Scanning

The scanning session took place one week following the
last day the SenseCam was worn (mean length of delay =
8 天, 标准差= 1.2). There were a total of nine fMRI runs
blocked by condition and presented in an alternating order
(IE。, ABCABCABC), counterbalanced across participants
based on a Latin square design. Three of the separate runs
consisted of another condition, which was included for a
separate analysis. The structure of the remaining six runs
was similar in each condition (see Figure 1C). 这些中的每一个
runs began with a 10-sec title screen (IE。, “Today was the
day I…”) and consisted of 12 cues presented in chrono-
logical order from that day, for a total of 36 events per con-
dition across 3 runs. Cues were presented for 20 秒, 和
participants were instructed to recall the events depicted
from their own perspective (SCS) or to understand the
events being depicted from another personʼs perspective
(SCO). The cues in each condition consisted of 40 Sense-
Cam pictures depicting a single event and presented at a
rate of two pictures per second.

Following each cue presentation, participants indicated
their subjective experience. In the SPS condition, par-
ticipants rated the subjective experience of recollection,
reliving, which refers to how much they were able to re-
experience the event depicted as if it were happening right
now or as if they were mentally traveling back to the time
when the event occurred. It is important to note that reliv-
ing is similar to other subjective measures of recollection,
such as the remember/know paradigm (for a review, 看
Yonelinas, 2002). 例如, in the remember/know
范例, participants are asked to use introspection to
classify items as recollected (vivid re-experiencing of the
original event and its context) or merely familiar. 虽然
introspection has its limitations, the results of hundreds
of remember/know studies are highly consistent with find-
ings of hundreds of studies using objective measures of
recollection, such as source memory ( Yonelinas, 2002).
然而, there are some critical differences between the
reliving scale and the remember/know paradigm, 哪个
make the reliving scale a better measure for AM. 第一的,
the reliving scale could be considered a better subjective
measure of recollection in AM than the remember/know
paradigm because it does not require the assumption
of a dual-process model (Wixted, 2007; Yonelinas, 2002).
第二, reliving is a better predictor of recollection in
AMs compared to the remember/know scale, 这是一个
better predictor of confidence in AMs (鲁宾, Schrauf, &
Greenberg, 2004; 鲁宾, Burt, & Fifield, 2003). 在里面
SPO condition, participants were asked to indicate the
amount of understanding of the other personʼs pers-
pective. While taking another personʼs perspective, par-
ticipants were instructed to try to understand what was
正在发生, where the event was taking place, and why
the event was occurring. Ratings were conducted on an
8-point scale from low to high, and were self-paced (最多
6 秒). Following a response, a fixation cross was presented

英石. Jacques et al.

1277

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

/

.

.

t

.

F


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

for a jittered interval between 4 和 8 sec plus any remain-
ing time from the response period.

fMRI Methods

Image Acquisition

Scanning was conducted using a 4-T GE magnet. Anatomi-
cal scanning included a T1-weighted sagittal localizer series
and 3-D fast spoiled gradient-echo recalled (SPGR) struc-
tural images were acquired in the coronal plane (2562 嘛-
trix, TR = 12.3 毫秒, = 5.4 毫秒, flip angle = 20°, FOV =
240, 68 slices, 1.9 mm slice thickness). Coplanar functional
images were acquired using an inverse spiral sequence (642
image matrix, TR = 2000 毫秒, = 6 毫秒, FOV = 240,
flip angle = 60°, 34 slices, 3.8 mm slice thickness).

fMRI Analyses

Image processing and analyses were performed using Sta-
tistical Parametric Mapping software in Matlab (SPM5; 出色地-
come Department of Imaging Neuroscience). Functional
images were corrected for slice acquisition order, realigned
to correct for motion artifacts, spatially normalized to a stan-
dard stereotactic space, and spatially smoothed using an
8-mm isotropic Gaussian kernel. Coordinates are reported
in Talairach space using a transformation from the Montreal
Neurological Institute coordinates (Brett, Christoff, Cusack,
& 兰卡斯特, 2001).

Self-projection of Self versus Other

To examine activation differences between self versus other
self-projection during the presentation of the SenseCam
图片, we used a Finite Impulse Response (FIR) basis func-
的. The FIR approach allowed us to examine potential ac-
tivation differences without assuming a particular canonical
hemodynamic response function, 因此, was appropri-
ate for the complex and temporally protracted processes
elicited in the present study. The FIR model included 16 关于-
gressors of peristimulus time bins of 2 sec duration (equal
to the TR) for each condition, yielding estimates of fMRI
signal change across the entire trial period (SenseCam pre-
sentation and rating).

We conducted a Condition (SPS, SPO) × Time (0 到 20 秒)
ANOVA implemented in SPM5 in order to isolate activa-
tion differences in the self versus other conditions across
the FIR timepoints associated with the presentation of the
SenseCam images. We examined the main effect of con-
dition at an FDR-corrected threshold of p = .05 using a
two-voxel extent threshold, and inclusively masked with
the effect of interest (SPS > SPO or SPO > SPS) at p =
.05 to determine the direction of the effect. An extent
threshold of 2 voxels was chosen here because the ROI ap-
proach combined with a corrected threshold was consid-
ered very conservative. 更远, we took an ROI approach
to examine brain regions associated with self versus other

self-projection in the present study based on a previous
quantitative meta-analysis which generated activation
likelihood estimation maps corresponding to statistically
significant concordance of activated voxels in 19 AM and
50 ToM studies (Spreng et al., 2009).

Parametric Modulation by Behavior

To examine the neural correlates associated with self ver-
sus other self-projection that was sensitive to on-line be-
havioral responses, we employed a parametric approach.
To identify increases in activity as a function of increasing
behavioral responses on each trial, we created a GLM in
which temporal versus mental self-projection was modu-
lated by reliving and understanding using the first-order

桌子 1. Self-projection of Self vs. 其他

BA

X

y

z

F

Voxels

Region

SPO > SPS

Dorsal mPFC

Dorsolateral PFC

9 −7

9 −11

9

19

38

56

49

33 14.17

25 13.67

33 30.51

Ventral parietal cortex

39 −45 −67

38 15.26

40 −56 −57

27 12.59

40

52 −53

34 10.93

10 −7

54 −3 53.56

10 −37

45 −41

48

23

12 80.47

16 17.94

47 −45

14 −7

47 −41

29 −8

6.79

6.73

47

52

24 −7

4

6

29 −5 17.06

30

10

20 32.01

52 16.61

4 −7

0 42.34

SPS > SPO

Ventral mPFC

Fronto-polar cortex

Ventrolateral PFC

Anterior cingulate

Supplemental motor area

Thalamus

Temporo-polar cortex

38 −37

17 −26 24.59

Middle temporal cortex

21 −52 −15 −12 27.71

21

41 −8 −9 17.46

Superior temporal cortex 22 −45 −57

17 25.61

Hippocampus

– −22 −12 −15 28.68

Retrosplenial cortex

29

0 −51

6 20.00

Posterior cingulate

31 −7 −31

33 19.20

Ventral parietal cortex

39 −37 −75

25 14.31

12

5

12

5

7

5

51

18

9

2

3

3

7

7

8

12

17

5

22

71

10

2

11

Talairach coordinates reported. BA = Brodmannʼs area; PFC = prefrontal
cortex; mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex.

1278

认知神经科学杂志

体积 23, 数字 6

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

.

F

t

.

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

parametric modulation option integrated in SPM5. 亚塞-
经常地, random effects analyses were performed on the
parameter estimate of the parametric regressor for the be-
havioral response. We used the results of the one-sample
t test ( p = .05) reflecting activity modulated by reliving or
understanding as an inclusive mask to determine whether
the regions showing activation differences in self versus
other self-projection were also sensitive to behavior.

Task-related Functional Connectivity Analysis

Seed voxels in ventral versus dorsal mPFC that were identified
in our previous analysis on self versus other self-projection
were further interrogated to examine the task-related net-
work of brain regions functionally connected with disso-
ciable mPFC regions. We should note that in the present
article we refer to dorsal mPFC (z-axis on Talairach atlas:
>20 mm) and ventral mPFC (z-axis on Talairach atlas:
<20 mm to>−15 mm; 例如, Krueger et al., 2009; 货车
Overwalle, 2009), 然而, the particular naming conven-
tion may differ among authors (例如, 巴克纳, Andrews-
Hanna, & Schacter, 2008; Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004).
To find these functional connectivity maps, we employed
a second analysis based on individual trial activity (Rissman,

Gazzaley, & DʼEsposito, 2004). 具体来说, we first created
a GLM in which each individual trial was modeled by a
separate covariate, thus yielding different parameter esti-
mates for each individual trial and for each individual
主题. The resulting correlation maps were Fisher trans-
formed to allow for statistical comparison. 然后, to exam-
ine differences in functional connectivity of ventral versus
dorsal mPFC regions associated with temporal versus men-
tal self-projection, we conducted a two-sample t test in
SPM5 using an FDR-corrected threshold of p = .05, and a
two-voxel extent threshold.

结果

Behavioral

SPS was associated with a mean reliving rating of 5.04 (标准差=
0.56; RT = 1.42 秒, 标准差= 0.67), and SPO was associated
with a mean understanding rating of 4.50 (标准差= 0.86; RT =
1.35 秒, 标准差= 0.61). There were no significant differ-
ences in the reaction time across the two conditions (Cohenʼs
d = 0.11). The behavioral results suggest that the Sense-
Cam images evoked a strong ability to re-experience the
personal past and to comprehend another individualʼs
看法.

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

t

.

F

/

.


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

数字 2. Self-projection of self versus other. There was a dorsal (A) versus ventral (乙) distinction in the recruitment of mPFC during self versus
other self-projection. BA = Brodmannʼs area.

英石. Jacques et al.

1279

桌子 2. Parametric Modulation by Behavior

Region

BA

X

y

z

F

Voxels

SPS: Understanding

Dorsal mPFC

9 −7

9

19

38

49

33 14.17

36 26.63

Ventral parietal cortex

39 −45 −67

42 13.38

SPO: Reliving

Ventral mPFC

10 −7

47 −6 21.66

Anterior cingulate

24 −4

16 31.90

Supplemental motor area

Thalamus

24

6

34

37

10

0

4

5 24.70

12

52 16.61

4 −7

0 42.34

Temporo-polar cortex

38 −37

17 −26 24.59

Middle temporal cortex

21 −56 −15 −15 22.29

Superior temporal cortex 22 −45 −57

17 25.61

Hippocampus

– −22 −12 −15 28.68

Retrosplenial cortex

29

0 −51

6 20.00

6

4

4

8

6

5

6

7

6

22

33

9

Talairach coordinates reported. BA = Brodmannʼs area; PFC = prefrontal
cortex; mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex.

功能磁共振成像

Self-projection of Self versus Other

The results of the self versus other self-projection revealed a
dorsal versus ventral distinction in mPFC (见表 1 and Fig-
乌尔 2). There was greater activity in the dorsal mPFC during
SPO compared to SPS (图2A). 此外, the SPO >

SPS contrast revealed greater recruitment of right dorsolat-
eral PFC and ventral parietal cortices. 相比之下, 有
greater recruitment of the ventral mPFC during self versus
other self-projection (图2B). 此外, the SPS >
SPO contrast revealed greater recruitment in several regions
associated with AM retrieval (Cabeza & 英石. Jacques, 2007), 在-
cluding the left hippocampus, lateral temporal, 后部
midline, and bilateral ventrolateral PFC. 有趣的是, 这
difference between SPS versus SPO was reflected by less
deactivation in ventral mPFC but greater activity in dorsal
mPFC, which is a pattern of findings consistent with others
(Gusnard, Akbudak, 舒尔曼, & Raichle, 2001), and linked
to a default state of cognitive processing (Gusnard, Raichle,
& Raichle, 2001). 总共, these results suggest that ventral
versus dorsal subregions of PFC contribute to separable
forms of self-projection.

Parametric Modulation of Behavior

In order to directly link activation differences in self-
projection to behavior, we examined activity in the above
regions that was modulated on each trial by the extent of
reliving in the case of SPS, and understanding in the case of
SPO (见表 2 和图 3). We found greater modula-
tion of ventral mPFC during highly relived trials for SPS, 但
no modulation by understanding in this same region for
SPO. 相比之下, there was greater modulation of dorsal
mPFC by better understanding for SPO, but no modulation
in this region by reliving for SPS. 总共, these results
show that activity in ventral mPFC is sensitive to reliving
when taking oneʼs own past perspective during memory
恢复, whereas activity in dorsal mPFC is sensitive to bet-
ter understanding when projecting oneʼs self onto a dif-
ferent mental perspective.

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

F

.

t

.

.

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

数字 3. Parametric modulation of behavior. Ventral versus dorsal mPFC modulated the amount of reliving during self-projection of oneʼs
own self (SPS) and the amount of understanding during self-projection of another perspective (SPO), 分别. The graphs depict high
and low ratings based on a median split; 然而, the parametric analysis is based on the continuous ratings. BA = Brodmannʼs area.

1280

认知神经科学杂志

体积 23, 数字 6

Task-related Functional Connectivity Analysis

For examining functional connectivity, we used the peak
voxels identified in the ventral and dorsal mPFC regions en-
gaged by self versus other self-projection as seed voxels in
individual trial-based analyses. These analyses revealed that
ventral versus dorsal mPFC showed task-related functional
connectivity with MTL and fronto-parietal networks (看
桌子 3 和图 4). Ventral mPFC showed greater co-
activation with the left hippocampus and precuneus, com-
pared to dorsal mPFC. 相比之下, dorsal mPFC showed

greater coactivation with bilateral frontal and parietal re-
祇翁, compared to ventral mPFC. The pattern of functional
connectivity of these ventral versus dorsal mPFC regions is
consistent with previous studies examining spontaneous
patterns of coherent activity during passive resting state,
which have revealed an MTL network (Kahn et al., 2008;
Vincent et al., 2006) and a fronto-parietal network (Vincent
等人。, 2008). 总共, the findings from the functional con-
nectivity analysis provide strong support for the dissociable
role of dorsal versus ventral mPFC in self versus other self-
投影.

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

桌子 3. Task-related Functional Connectivity of Medial
Prefrontal Cortex during Self-projection of Self vs. 其他

Region

BA

X

y

z

t

Voxels

SPO > SPS

Dorsal mPFC

Dorsolateral PFC

Ventrolateral PFC

Orbito-frontal cortex

Fronto-polar
cortex

Claustrum

Middle temporal

cortex

Superior temporal

cortex

Precuneus

Dorsal parietal

cortex

小脑

SPS > SPO

Ventral mPFC

Hippocampus

Precuneus

645

132

2

4

29

11

30

5

2

11

48

4

14

2

3

2

300

162

83

5

42

30 17.08

−7

−52

45

48

−52

−52

−45

48

26

38

24

35

37

34

37

16

36 −2

18 −4

43 −18

43 −9

51 −3

−22

21 −7

22

21 −7

−56 −30 −11

3.4

63 −33 −14

67 −26 −8

3.46

3.23

41 −33

5

3.66

4.8

3.35

3.29

4.84

4.19

4.32

3.72

3.61

4.55

5.71

3.82

6.04

5.02

4.54

3.79

4.67

15 −78

7/40 −45 −48

7/40

48 −49

46

55

48

−37 −60 −29

−15 −81 −22

37 −67 −25

9

9

9

46

47

47

11

11/47

10

21

21

21

41

7

10

7

−7

55

1 17.3

277

26 −22 −12

4.11

−11 −49

48

5.09

2

2

Talairach coordinates reported. BA = Brodmannʼs area; PFC = prefrontal
cortex; mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex.

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

/

F

.

.

t


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

讨论

The present fMRI study investigated the neural mechanisms
underlying self-projection to the personal past and into the
life of another person within the same individuals via natu-
ralistic stimuli that were prospectively generated using a
novel camera technology. Our data indicate that there is
a ventral versus dorsal distinction in mPFC during self-
projection relying on shifts in perspective from oneʼs own
self versus another individual. There were three main find-
ings supporting this result. 第一的, we found that temporal
self-projection into the personal past recruited greater
ventral mPFC, whereas mental self-projection into another
personʼs perspective recruited greater dorsal mPFC. 秒-
另一, activity in ventral versus dorsal mPFC was sensitive
to parametric modulation on each trial by the ability to re-
live the personal past or to understand anotherʼs perspec-
主动的. 第三, task-related functional connectivity analysis
revealed that ventral mPFC contributed to the medial tem-
poral lobe network linked to memory processes, 然而
dorsal mPFC contributed to the fronto-parietal network
linked to controlled processes.

mPFC is a critical node in the network of regions sup-
porting tasks relying on self-projection such as AM retrieval
(米切尔, 2009; Spreng et al., 2009; 巴克纳 & 卡罗尔,
2007) and ToM (米切尔, 2009; Spreng et al., 2009; 巴克纳
& 卡罗尔, 2007). Here we show that ventral versus dorsal
mPFC may differentially support self versus other forms of
self-projection during these tasks. The mPFC coordinates
reported here also overlap with those found by a previ-
ous meta-analyses on social cognition ( Van Overwalle,
2009) and one on self-referential processing (Northoff
等人。, 2006). The exact role of particular subregions within
mPFC has been a matter of considerable debate, 和
some studies observing recruitment of ventral mPFC when
making inferences about oneʼs own self and dorsal mPFC
when mentalizing about others (Krueger et al., 2009; 货车
Overwalle, 2009; also see Spreng et al., 2009), 和别的
studies observing that both dorsal and ventral mPFC re-
cruited during self-referential processes (Northoff et al.,
2006). The recruitment of particular subregions of mPFC
during self-projection may vary according to the degree
of personal relevance. Consistent with this idea, in a series
of studies, Mitchell and colleagues (Ames, Jenkins, Banaji, &
米切尔, 2008; Jenkins, Macrae, & 米切尔, 2008; 米切尔,

英石. Jacques et al.

1281

数字 4. Task-related
functional connectivity analysis.
Ventral versus dorsal mPFC
were functionally connected to
separate neural networks
during self-projection of oneʼs
own self (SPS; orange/red
activation) versus self-
projection of another
看法 (SPO; 蓝色的
activation). The activation in
showing the results of the
functional connectivity in each
condition was projected to the
cortical surface using CARET.

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

t

/

.

F

.


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

Macrae, & Banaji, 2006; 米切尔, Banaji, & Macrae, 2005)
found that ventral mPFC was recruited to a greater extent
when mentalizing about similar others (例如, people with
the same political beliefs), presumably because they could
rely more on the retrieval of information from their own life.
相比之下, dorsal mPFC was recruited more when making
inferences about dissimilar others. In the case of the current
学习, we found dorsal mPFC for a dissimilar other and ven-
tral mPFC for a very similar other—oneʼs own past self. 二
previous studies investigating self-projection within the
same individuals also found greater recruitment of ventral
mPFC for AM versus ToM, but they did not observe dif-
ferences in the recruitment of dorsal mPFC (Rabin et al.,
2010; Spreng & Grady, 2010). Control over the use of
the first-person perspective during self-projection (例如,
DʼArgembeau et al., 2007), employment of naturalistic
dynamic visuospatial cues, and other methodological differ-
ences may potentially account for the observed difference
between the current and previous studies.

The recruitment of mPFC also modulated the extent
of self-projection to the personal past or into the life of
another person. Ventral mPFC was recruited to a greater
extent when temporal self-projection involved greater re-
experience of the personal past as measured by subjec-
tive ratings of the amount of reliving. These results are
consistent with prior functional neuroimaging studies
showing that ventral mPFC is sensitive to the ability to re-
experience the self in time during AM (Levine et al., 2004;
Maguire & Mummery, 1999), and with patient studies

showing that lesions with overlap here impair the sub-
jective experience of re-experiencing the personal past
(Wheeler & 愚蠢的, 2003). 相比之下, dorsal mPFC was re-
cruited to a greater extent when mental self-projection
involved better understanding of another personʼs per-
观望的. Dorsal mPFC is recruited during tasks that rely
on evaluation (Northoff et al., 2006) such as during impres-
sion formation of another individual (米切尔, Neil Macrae,
& Banaji, 2005). The sensitivity of dorsal mPFC to better
understand a dissimilar other may reflect increased reliance
on rule-based strategies to infer an alternative perspective.
Ventral versus dorsal mPFC were functionally connected
to separate neural networks, which differentially con-
tributed to self-projection. Ventral mPFC showed greater
functional connectivity with the hippocampus and precu-
neus, a pattern of results consistent with the MTL network
supporting memory (Kahn et al., 2008; Vincent et al., 2006),
whereas dorsal mPFC showed greater functional connec-
tivity with lateral frontal, fronto-polar, and dorsal parietal
cortices, a pattern of functional connectivity which is con-
sistent with the fronto-parietal network supporting con-
trolled processes (Vincent et al., 2008). The MTL network
is a subsystem of the default network, the set of brain re-
gions that are coactive during passive resting states and as-
sociated with internally directed processes such as memory
(Buckner et al., 2008). Greater involvement of the MTL
network during self-projection to the personal past is con-
sistent with the idea that the ability to take oneʼs own per-
spective may rely upon the recovery of memory details. 在

1282

认知神经科学杂志

体积 23, 数字 6

对比, the fronto-parietal network supports the initiation
and flexible adjustment of controlled processes (Dosenbach,
Fair, 科恩, 施拉加尔, & 彼得森, 2008). Vincent et al.
(2008) suggested that the fronto-parietal network may
contribute to the integration between externally directed
attention and internally directed thought, given that it is
anatomically juxtaposed between the dorsal attention
and default networks. These types of controlled process-
ing may be particularly important during self-projection
into the life of another person, which potentially involves
greater integration between an externally presented per-
spective and internally directed processes. The functional
dissociation between the neural networks supporting
self-projection observed in the present study is also con-
sistent with evidence that AM and ToM are independent
(Rosenbaum, 愚蠢的, 莱文, & Tulving, 2007).

Although mPFC is frequently observed in tasks relying on
self-projection (Spreng et al., 2009), it is also one of the
most frequent regions observed during emotional tasks
(Phan, Wager, 泰勒, & Liberzon, 2002). Very few studies
have controlled for both self-reference and emotion within
the same individuals, thus it is difficult to definitively dis-
tinguish these processes (for a meta-analysis, see Gilbert
等人。, 2006). 然而, one study found that ventral mPFC
was sensitive to the self-relevance of stimuli irrespective of
情感 (Moran, Macrae, Heatherton, Wyland, & Kelley,
2006). In the present study, the elicitation of emotional re-
sponses during self-projection was minimized because the
SenseCam images depicted very recent, everyday events
(attending class, studying, ETC。). 而且, the dorsal versus
ventral distinction observed in the current study would be
difficult to explain based on the role of these mPFC subre-
gions in emotion (例如, Dolcos, LaBar, & Cabeza, 2004). 因此,
the pattern of results observed in the present study is more
consistent with the suggestion that the ventral versus dorsal
distinction in mPFC is related to differences in the nature of
self-projection rather than emotion. Future studies, 如何-
曾经, should directly manipulate the elicitation of emotion
during self-projection for self versus other.

结论

The dynamic visuospatial cues employed in the current
study provided a novel way to investigate self-projection
of oneʼs own life or the life of another individual. 我们发现
a ventral versus dorsal distinction in the recruitment of
mPFC for self versus other shifts in perspective. 更远,
ventral mPFC modulated the extent to which oneʼs own
perspective was re-experienced, whereas dorsal mPFC
modulated the ability to understand an alternative perspec-
主动的. Supporting the dissociable role of these mPFC subre-
gions during different forms of self-projection, task-related
functional connectivity analysis revealed that ventral versus
dorsal mPFC were nodes in different neural networks.
Ventral mPFC contributed to the MTL network linked to
memory processes, whereas dorsal mPFC contributed to
the fronto-parietal network linked to controlled processes.

总共, the results of the current study suggest that mPFC
contributes to shifts from the present moment to alterna-
tive self and other perspectives.

致谢
We thank James A. Kragel for help with programming. This re-
search was supported by a Microsoft Research Cambridge grant
awarded to R. C. 和M. A. C。, and a National Institute on Aging
RO1 (AG 23770) awarded to R. C.

Reprint requests should be sent to Peggy L. 英石. Jacques, Center for
Cognitive Neuroscience, 杜克大学, Box 90999, 达勒姆,
NC 27708, or via e-mail: peggy.st.jacques@duke.edu.

参考

Ames, D. L。, Jenkins, A. C。, Banaji, 中号. R。, & 米切尔, J. 磷.
(2008). Taking another personʼs perspective increases
self-referential neural processing. 心理科学,
19, 642–644.

Amodio, D. M。, & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds:
The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. 自然
评论 神经科学, 7, 268–277.

Brett, M。, Christoff, K., Cusack, R。, & 兰卡斯特, J. L. (2001).

Using the Talairach atlas with the MNI template.
Neuroimage, 13, S85.

巴克纳, 右. L。, Andrews-Hanna, J. R。, & Schacter, D. L. (2008).

The brainʼs default network: Anatomy, function, 和
relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy
of Science, 1124, 1–38.

巴克纳, 右. L。, & 卡罗尔, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and
大脑. 认知科学的趋势, 11, 49–57.
Cabeza, R。, 王子, S. E., Daselaar, S. M。, Greenberg, D. L。,
Budde, M。, Dolcos, F。, 等人. (2004). Brain activity during
episodic retrieval of autobiographical and laboratory events:
An fMRI study using a novel photo paradigm. 杂志
Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1583–1594.

Cabeza, R。, & 英石. Jacques, 磷. L. (2007). Functional neuroimaging

of autobiographical memory. Trends in Cognitive
科学, 11, 219–227.

DʼArgembeau, A。, Ruby, P。, Collette, F。, Degueldre, C。,

Balteau, E., Luxen, A。, 等人. (2007). Distinct regions of
the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-
referential processing and perspective taking. 杂志
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 935–944.

Dolcos, F。, LaBar, K. S。, & Cabeza, 右. (2004). Dissociable
effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity
indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory:
An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23, 64–74.
Dosenbach, 氮. U。, Fair, D. A。, 科恩, A. L。, 施拉加尔, 乙. L。,
& 彼得森, S. 乙. (2008). A dual-network architecture of
top–down control. 认知科学的趋势, 12,
99–105.

加拉格尔, H. L。, & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging
of “theory of mind”. 认知科学的趋势, 7,
77–83.

吉尔伯特, S. J。, Spengler, S。, Simons, J. S。, Steele, J. D ., Lawrie,
S. M。, Frith, C. D ., 等人. (2006). Functional specialization
within rostral prefrontal cortex (区域 10): A meta-analysis.
认知神经科学杂志, 18, 932–948.

Gusnard, D. A。, Akbudak, E., 舒尔曼, G. L。, & Raichle, 中号. 乙.
(2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental
活动: Relation to a default mode of brain function.
美国国家科学院院刊, 美国。,
98, 4259–4264.

英石. Jacques et al.

1283

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

t

.

F

/

.

.


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

Gusnard, D. A。, Raichle, 中号. E., & Raichle, 中号. 乙. (2001).

Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting
人脑. 自然评论神经科学, 2, 685–694.
Jenkins, A. C。, Macrae, C. N。, & 米切尔, J. 磷. (2008). Repetition

suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during
judgments of self and others. 国家会议录
Academy of Sciences, 美国。, 105, 4507–4512.

Kahn, 我。, Andrews-Hanna, J. R。, Vincent, J. L。, 斯奈德, A. Z。,
& 巴克纳, 右. L. (2008). Distinct cortical anatomy linked
to subregions of the medial temporal lobe revealed by
intrinsic functional connectivity. 神经生理学杂志,
100, 129–139.

Krueger, F。, Barbey, A. K., & Grafman, J. (2009). The medial
prefrontal cortex mediates social event knowledge. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 103–109.

莱文, B., 车工, G. R。, Tisserand, D ., Hevenor, S. J。, Graham,
S. J。, & McIntosh, A. 右. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy
of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering:
A prospective functional MRI study. 认知杂志
神经科学, 16, 1633–1646.

Rissman, J。, Gazzaley, A。, & DʼEsposito, 中号. (2004). 测量
functional connectivity during distinct stages of a cognitive
任务. Neuroimage, 23, 752–763.

Rosenbaum, 右. S。, 愚蠢的, D. T。, 莱文, B., & Tulving, 乙.

(2007). Theory of mind is independent of episodic memory.
科学, 318, 1257.

鲁宾, D. C。, Burt, C. D ., & Fifield, S. J. (2003). Experimental
manipulations of the phenomenology of memory. 记忆
& 认识, 31, 877–886.

鲁宾, D. C。, Schrauf, 右. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2004). Stability

in autobiographical memories. 记忆, 12, 715–721.

Spreng, 右. N。, & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity
supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, 和
theory-of-mind and their relationship to the default mode
网络. 认知神经科学杂志, 22, 1112–1123.

Spreng, 右. N。, 三月, 右. A。, & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common
neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection,
导航, 心理理论, and the default mode: A
quantitative meta-analysis. 认知杂志
神经科学, 21, 489–510.

Maguire, 乙. A。, & Mummery, C. J. (1999). Differential

英石. Jacques, 磷. L。, 康威, 中号. A。, & Cabeza, 右. (under review).

modulation of a common memory retrieval network revealed
by positron emission tomography. Hippocampus, 9, 54–61.

米切尔, J. 磷. (2009). Inferences about mental states.

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London,
Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 1309–1316.

米切尔, J. P。, Banaji, 中号. R。, & Macrae, C. 氮. (2005). The link
between social cognition and self-referential thought in the
medial prefrontal cortex. 认知神经科学杂志,
17, 1306–1315.

米切尔, J. P。, Macrae, C. N。, & Banaji, 中号. 右. (2006). Dissociable
medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and
dissimilar others. 神经元, 50, 655–663.

米切尔, J. P。, Neil Macrae, C。, & Banaji, 中号. 右. (2005). Forming
impressions of people versus inanimate objects: Social–
cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage, 26, 251–257.

Moran, J. M。, Macrae, C. N。, Heatherton, 时间. F。, Wyland, C. L。,
& Kelley, 瓦. 中号. (2006). Neuroanatomical evidence for
distinct cognitive and affective components of self. 杂志
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1586–1594.

Northoff, G。, & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures

and the self. 认知科学的趋势, 8, 102–107.

Northoff, G。, Heinzel, A。, de Greck, M。, Bermpohl, F。,

Dobrowolny, H。, & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential
processing in our brain: A meta-analysis of imaging studies
on the self. Neuroimage, 31, 440–457.

Phan, K. L。, Wager, T。, 泰勒, S. F。, & Liberzon, 我. (2002).
Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of
emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage,
16, 331–348.

Rabin, J. S。, Gilboa, A。, 愚蠢的, D. T。, 三月, 右. A。, & Rosenbaum,
右. S. (2010). Common and unique neural correlates of
autobiographical memory and theory of mind. 杂志
Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1095–1111.

Gender differences in autobiographical memory for
everyday events: Retrieval elicited by SenseCam images
与. verbal cues.

英石. Jacques, 磷. L。, 鲁宾, D. C。, Labar, K. S。, & Cabeza, 右. (2008).
The short and long of it: Neural correlates of temporal-order
memory for autobiographical events. 认知杂志
神经科学, 20, 1327–1341.

Suddendorf, T。, & Corballis, 中号. C. (2007). The evolution of
foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to
人类? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–313;
discussion 313–351.

Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain:
A meta-analysis. 人脑图谱, 30, 829–858.

Vincent, J. L。, Kahn, 我。, 斯奈德, A. Z。, Raichle, 中号. E., & 巴克纳,
右. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system
revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. 杂志
Neurophysiology, 100, 3328–3342.

Vincent, J. L。, Patel, G. H。, 狐狸, 中号. D ., 斯奈德, A. Z。, 贝克,
J. T。, Van Essen, D. C。, 等人. (2007). Intrinsic functional
architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. 自然,
447, 83–86.

Vincent, J. L。, 斯奈德, A. Z。, 狐狸, 中号. D ., Shannon, 乙. J。, Andrews,
J. R。, Raichle, 中号. E., 等人. (2006). Coherent spontaneous
activity identifies a hippocampal–parietal memory network.
神经生理学杂志, 96, 3517–3531.

Wheeler, 中号. A。, & 愚蠢的, D. 时间. (2003). Remembering and
knowing in patients with frontal lobe injuries. Cortex,
39, 827–846.

Wixted, J. 时间. (2007). Dual-process theory and signal-detection
theory of recognition memory. 心理评论, 114,
152–176.

Yonelinas, A. 磷. (2002). The nature of recollection and

familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. 杂志
记忆与语言, 46, 441–517.

1284

认知神经科学杂志

体积 23, 数字 6

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
3

6
e

1
p
2
d
7
F
5
/
1
2
9
3
4
/
1
6
7
/
0
1
9
2

7
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
1
7
0
5
7
2
0
1
7
5
1
/
8
j

p
C
d
n
.

y
2
0
G
1

0
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
5
1
0
8
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
0
e
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

F

.

.

t

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study image
Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study image
Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study image
Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study image
Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study image
Watching My Mind Unfold versus Yours: An fMRI Study image

下载pdf