Span, CRUNCH, and Beyond: Working Memory Capacity
and the Aging Brain
Nils J. Schneider-Garces, Brian A. Gordon, Carrie R. Brumback-Peltz,
Eunsam Shin, Yukyung Lee, Bradley P. Sutton, Edward L. Maclin,
Gabriele Gratton, and Monica Fabiani
D
哦
w
n
我
哦
A
d
e
d
抽象的
■ Neuroimaging data emphasize that older adults often show
greater extent of brain activation than younger adults for similar
objective levels of difficulty. A possible interpretation of this find-
ing is that older adults need to recruit neuronal resources at lower
loads than younger adults, leaving no resources for higher loads,
and thus leading to performance decrements [Compensation-
Related Utilization of Neural Circuits Hypothesis; 例如, Reuter-
Lorenz, 磷. A。, & Cappell, K. A. Neurocognitive aging and the
compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological
科学, 17, 177–182, 2008]. The Compensation-Related Utiliza-
tion of Neural Circuits Hypothesis leads to the prediction that acti-
vation differences between younger and older adults should
disappear when task difficulty is made subjectively comparable.
In a Sternberg memory search task, this can be achieved by as-
sessing brain activity as a function of load relative to the indi-
vidualʼs memory span, which declines with age. 具体来说, 我们
hypothesized a nonlinear relationship between load and both
performance and brain activity and predicted that asymptotes in
the brain activation function should correlate with performance
asymptotes (corresponding to working memory span). 那里-
sults suggest that age differences in brain activation can be
largely attributed to individual variations in working memory span.
有趣的是, the brain activation data show a sigmoid relation-
ship with load. Results are discussed in terms of Cowanʼs [考恩,
氮. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsidera-
tion of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
24, 87–114, 2001] model of working memory and theories of im-
paired inhibitory processes in aging. ■
介绍
Working memory (WM) is a system that allows us to store
and manipulate small amounts of information for a short
时间 (Baddeley, 1986; Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). 之一
the most intriguing findings in cognitive psychology is that
the capacity of WM is in fact very limited, although there is
some debate as to exactly how many items can be main-
tained and manipulated. 磨坊主 (1956), in a classic article,
proposed that the capacity of WM is 7 ± 2 项目. 豪维-
是, in a more recent review of a large number of studies,
考恩 (2001) proposed that the core of the WM system
can only hold 4 ± 1 items and that additional processes
such as “chunking” are required for more items. 开启于-
portant aspect of Cowanʼs model is that WM is seen as a
part of a more extended memory system, in which a small
number of items are activated out of a much larger pool,
so as to be readily available for the performance of a par-
ticular task. The limitation, 所以, is not really in mem-
ory capacity per se but in how many items can be kept
into the focus of attention at any point in time. 因此, 这
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
“activation capacity” is assumed to be dependent on atten-
tion deployment, and WM is assumed to be limited by at-
tention span. A similar view has been proposed by Engle
and Kane (2004), Kane, Bleckley, 康威, and Engle
(2001), and Kane and Engle (2000). A related question is
how WM capacity is linked to brain activations during WM
任务. To address this question, this study aims at examin-
ing in detail the changes in brain activity that are observed
when WM capacity limits are reached.
Although the majority of studies of WM capacity have
been carried out in young adults, in the last several de-
cades researchers have also investigated how WM changes
with age (例如, Craik & Byrd, 1982; Craik, 1968). 一些
studies have shown that, similarly to other cognitive func-
系统蒸发散, WM performance declines with increasing age (例如,
Bopp & Verhaeghen, 2005; Park et al., 2002; Verhaeghen
& Salthouse, 1997). Several theories have been developed
to explain this decline. 例如, Salthouse (1996)
proposed that aging leads to reduced speed of process-
英, rendering it more difficult to maintain many items
in memory at a time. 然而, it is also possible to link
age-related WM decline to a reduced ability to maintain
an appropriate/stable attention focus. 实际上, there is sub-
stantial evidence of reduced inhibition of the processing
© 2009 麻省理工学院
认知神经科学杂志 22:4, PP. 655–669
我
我
/
/
/
/
j
t
t
F
/
我
t
.
:
/
/
F
r
哦
米
D
H
哦
t
w
t
n
p
哦
:
A
/
d
/
e
米
d
我
F
t
r
哦
p
米
r
C
H
.
s
p
我
我
d
v
我
e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H
米
A
我
r
e
.
d
你
C
哦
哦
米
C
/
n
j
A
哦
r
C
t
我
n
C
/
e
A
–
p
r
d
t
我
2
C
2
我
4
e
–
6
p
5
d
5
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
哦
5
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
哦
p
C
d
n
.
乙
2
y
0
G
0
你
9
e
.
s
t
2
哦
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e
米
乙
y
乙
e
G
r
你
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
.
F
t
/
.
哦
n
1
8
中号
A
y
2
0
2
1
of distracting or irrelevant information in older adults,
which may support such a scenario (Hasher, Lustig, &
Zacks, 2008; Hasher & Zacks, 1988). In our own research,
we have also found evidence supporting the claim that
older adults may have problems inhibiting the processing
of irrelevant information present within the experimental
语境 (Fabiani, 低的, Wee, Sable, & Gratton, 2006; Fabiani,
弗里德曼, & Cheng, 1998; Fabiani & 弗里德曼, 1995). 一个
inappropriate focus on irrelevant/distracting information,
stemming from age-related difficulties in attention control,
may effectively reduce the WM capacity that is available
for the task at hand and lead to decreased performance.
有趣的是, this view of WM decline in aging does
not necessarily imply that older adults should show down-
regulation of their brain activity during WM tasks compared
with younger adults. 的确, functional neuroimaging data
provide numerous examples of an increased number of
areas showing up-regulation during the performance of
several cognitive tasks, including WM tasks (例如, Riecker
等人。, 2006; Park et al., 2003; Reuter-Lorenz, Stanczak, &
磨坊主, 1999; Grady et al., 1994; for a review, see Reuter-
Lorenz & Lustig, 2005). 在很多情况下, the data indicate
the occurrence of bilateral activations in older adults when
younger adults only show unilateral activity (Hemispheric
Asymmetries Are Reduced in OLD, HAROLD); Cabeza et al.,
2004; Cabeza, 2002; see also Reuter-Lorenz et al., 1999). 在
other studies, this up-regulation has involved areas within
the same hemisphere (例如, Payer et al., 2006).
These age-related increases in brain activity are consis-
tent with the concept of dedifferentiation (Lindenberger
& Baltes, 1997; see also Spearman, 1927): Older adults
may not be able to activate networks as selectively or as
efficiently as younger adults, therefore activating networks
in both hemispheres or involving additional areas. 因此,
such data could be interpreted as indicators of neuronal
dysfunction (IE。, the inability to suppress inappropriate
processing leading to conflict or reduced availability of
资源; 例如, Zarahn, Rakitin, Abela, Flynn, &
Stern, 2007; Rypma, Berger, & DʼEsposito, 2002) or as
compensatory activity for impaired functioning (IE。, a vi-
carious processing route may be used when the appro-
priate processing units are not as readily available; 为了
例子, Cabeza, 2002; Reuter-Lorenz, Marshuetz, Jonides,
& 史密斯, 2001; Rypma & DʼEsposito, 2001; McIntosh et al.,
1999).
Recent studies show that increasing task loads may in-
duce not only older adults but even younger adults to up-
regulate activity in some cortical regions (例如, Mattay
等人。, 2006). To account for both the age-related deficits
and these load effects, Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell (2008)
and Reuter-Lorenz and Lustig (2005) proposed that, 在
一般的, people will activate more cortical regions as task
load increases (Compensation-Related Utilization of Neu-
ral Circuits Hypothesis; CRUNCH). 然而, because of
less efficient processing, it may be necessary for older
adults to recruit these regions at lower load levels than
younger adults. This hypothesis thus argues that older
adults might recruit cognitive resources at lower loads to
compensate for cognitive decline. 所以, one would
expect to see a sharper increase in fMRI signal for low load
levels in older adults than in younger adults.
How would this hypothesis interact with the capacity
limits of WM? We hypothesize that, as WM load increases,
brain activity should increase up to where the memory
capacity limit is reached. After that, brain activity should
stop increasing, either because there are no further re-
sources available or because there is no performance
advantage in deploying brain resources any further. 如何-
曾经, this limit should be reached earlier in older adults
than in younger adults, resulting in a ceiling effect for
both the fMRI signal and performance. If load is varied
parametrically across several levels from low to high,
older adultsʼ fMRI activation should follow a nonlinear
pattern, with a sharp increase at the beginning and a flat-
tening at higher loads. The predicted fMRI asymptote
at higher loads corresponds to the performance pat-
tern predicted by the Cowanʼs (2001) 理论; CRUNCH
adds the prediction of a sharper increase in fMRI signal
for low loads in older adults. Observation of both pat-
terns requires data from several levels at both low and
high loads.
The Sternberg (1966) memory search task appears to
be a particularly useful tool to examine the relationship
between memory load and brain activation in younger
and older adults. This paradigm allows for parametric var-
iations of memory load by using different memory set
sizes. It has also been extensively studied using fMRI
( Veltman, Rombouts, & Dolan, 2003; Bunge, Ochsner,
德斯蒙德, Glover, & Gabrieli, 2001; DʼEsposito, Postle,
& Rypma, 2000; Henson, 伯吉斯, & Frith, 2000), 包括
experiments comparing the results obtained in younger
and older adults (例如, Zarahn et al., 2007; Rypma et al.,
2002). These data generally show increasing activations
in medial and lateral pFC as well as in parietal cortex, 和
increasing memory loads. Results have also indicated the
existence of age-related changes, but the interpretation
of these effects has been complicated by the presence
of large individual differences (Rypma, Berger, Genova,
Rebbechi, & DʼEsposito, 2005), which have been attributed
to variations in strategies. These strategic differences may
have been in part due to the use of a slow event-related
设计, with very long intervals (>10 sec) between the
presentation of the memory set and of the probe stimulus,
which may have encouraged participants to use elaborative
rehearsal. In the current study, we parametrically varied
memory set size from 2 到 6 and chose a much shorter
间隔 (4 秒), which should reduce strategic differences,
but still leave enough time for both younger and older
adults to encode the memory set stimuli.
When considered in all its implications, the CRUNCH
model explains overrecruitment and underrecruitment
of brain areas in older adults in terms of the relative ac-
tivation necessary to cope with the task and to compen-
sate for deficits. Taken in this light, 所以, CRUNCH
656
认知神经科学杂志
体积 22, 数字 4
D
哦
w
n
我
哦
A
d
e
d
我
我
/
/
/
/
j
t
t
F
/
我
t
.
:
/
/
F
r
哦
米
D
H
哦
t
w
t
n
p
哦
:
A
/
d
/
e
米
d
我
F
t
r
哦
p
米
r
C
H
.
s
p
我
我
d
v
我
e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H
米
A
我
r
e
.
d
你
C
哦
哦
米
C
/
n
j
A
哦
r
C
t
我
n
C
/
e
A
–
p
r
d
t
我
2
C
2
我
4
e
–
6
p
5
d
5
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
哦
5
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
哦
p
C
d
n
.
乙
2
y
0
G
0
你
9
e
.
s
t
2
哦
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e
米
乙
y
乙
e
G
r
你
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
t
.
.
.
/
F
哦
n
1
8
中号
A
y
2
0
2
1
leads to a very strong prediction: The difference in brain
activation level between younger and older subjects
should disappear once the difficulty of the task is equated
between the two groups. This should occur when mem-
ory load is not considered in absolute terms but relative
to WM span. 反过来, this requires the assessment of
memory capacity/span in each individual, so that a curve
of brain activation by subjective memory load can be
computed on a subject-by-subject basis and then exam-
ined across groups. There are in fact several procedures
that are commonly used to assess memory span. 的
这些, two of the most frequent are backward digit span
(Wechsler, 1981) and operation span (O-SPAN; Engle &
Kane, 2004; Kane et al., 2001; Kane & Engle, 2000). 铝-
though very useful, these measures are limited because
they do not directly estimate WM capacity within the
same task used to assess brain activity (在这种情况下, 这
Sternberg task). It is therefore difficult to exactly scale
the scores obtained by each individual subject in these
span tests so that they are made consistent with the mem-
ory loads used in the Sternberg task.
To address this problem, we derived a measure of
WM span directly from the performance obtained within
the Sternberg task. We followed suggestions by Cowan
(2001) to estimate the amount of information that is trans-
mitted during the memory task (which we refer to here
as “throughput”). We then used this measure to estimate
the memory span of each individual within the Sternberg
任务, providing estimates that are expressed in the same
单元 (memory load as a function of the number of items
in the memory set) used to classify the brain activation
function. This allowed us to measure brain activity as
a function of how large the memory load was with re-
spect to memory span for each individual (for a simi-
lar approach in young adults see Todd & Marois, 2005).
Using these data, we could then evaluate whether simi-
lar activation by subjective load functions were found in
younger and older adults—or, 换句话说, 无论
age-related differences in these functions disappeared
when difficulty was normalized by WM capacity, as pre-
dicted by CRUNCH.
方法
参加者
This study was part of a more extended project aimed at
examining changes in neurovascular coupling as a function
of aging and physical fitness. 为此原因, 年长的
group was larger than the younger group. The original
sample included 17 younger adults recruited from the Uni-
versity of Illinoisʼ student population and 33 老年人
recruited through ads in local newspapers, campus-wide
e-mailings, and postings at area gyms, retirement homes,
and community centers. For the purposes of the current
学习, 然而, the relevant measures were only available
from a smaller set of 42 subjects (behavioral measures
were not available in 4 subjects, 和 4 additional subjects
were discarded because of significant movement artifacts
in fMRI recordings). 因此, the younger sample included
12 subjects (age range = 18–27 years, mean age = 23.8 年,
6 女性); the older sample included 30 subjects (年龄
range = 65–80, mean age = 70.9 年, 13 女性). Youn-
ger and older adults did not differ in years of education
or scores in the Vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale-Revised ( Wechsler, 1981). They were
significantly different on the modified Mini-Mental Status
examination (Mayeux, Stern, 罗森, & Leventhal, 1981)
and on the O-SPAN (La Pointe & Engle, 1990). 演示-
graphic characteristics of the participants are summarized
表中 1.
Screening Procedures
Participants were screened based on a number of health
and cognitive criteria. Prospective subjects were excluded
from the study if they regularly took medications that are
known to affect the CNS (例如, beta blockers, CNS stimu-
lants, antidepressants, antipsychotics, sedating antihista-
mines, or migraine medications). Subjects with serious
or chronic medical conditions were also excluded. 阿迪-
理论上, subjects had to score at least 51 on the modified
Mini-Mental Status examination, show no signs of depres-
sion on Beckʼs Depression Scale (Beck & Steer, 1996), 和
D
哦
w
n
我
哦
A
d
e
d
我
我
/
/
/
/
j
t
t
F
/
我
t
.
:
/
/
F
r
哦
米
D
H
哦
t
w
t
n
p
哦
:
A
/
d
/
e
米
d
我
F
t
r
哦
p
米
r
C
H
.
s
p
我
我
d
v
我
e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H
米
A
我
r
e
.
d
你
C
哦
哦
米
C
/
n
j
A
哦
r
C
t
我
n
C
/
e
A
–
p
r
d
t
我
2
C
2
我
4
e
–
6
p
5
d
5
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
哦
5
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
哦
p
C
d
n
.
乙
2
y
0
G
0
你
9
e
.
s
t
2
哦
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e
米
乙
y
乙
e
G
r
你
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
F
t
.
.
/
哦
n
1
8
中号
A
y
2
0
2
1
桌子 1. 意思是 (with Estimated Standard Error in Parentheses) Demographic Characteristics for Younger and Older Adults
Measure
年龄 (年)
教育 (年)
Modified Mini-Mental Status examination
Vocabulary subscore of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised
O-SPAN
t tests between groups (two tailed): df = 40.
**p < .05.
***p < .01.
Young (n = 12)
Old (n = 30)
t Test
23.8 (0.7)
16.4 (0.7)
56.7 (0.2)
13.0 (1.0)
25.0 (4.1)
70.9 (0.8)
16.1 (0.6)
55.5 (0.3)
13.3 (0.4)
13.9 (1.5)
0.23
2.37**
0.36
3.33***
Schneider-Garces et al.
657
score above or within one standard deviation of the aver-
age score for their age group on the Vocabulary subtest of
the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (Wechsler,
1981). All participants were right-handed (as assessed by
the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971)
and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
Memory Paradigm and Procedures
We used a modified version of Sternbergʼs memory search
task (Sternberg, 1966), with memory set sizes two through
six (see Figure 1). The stimuli to be encoded were up-
percase letters (B, D, F, G, H, J, M, R, and T). To prevent
a direct visual match, their corresponding lowercase letters
were used as probes (see Bunge et al., 2001). The letters
were selected because of their different shapes when pre-
sented in upper and lower case. Each letter subtended
approximately 1.4° of visual angle in the diagonal and was
presented using a Resonance Technologies goggles system
(Resonance Technologies, Northridge, CA).
Each trial was initiated by the presentation of a mem-
ory set comprising two to six uppercase letters presented
simultaneously for 3 sec, followed by a screen containing
only a fixation cross presented for 1 sec. After that, the
probe was presented for 500 msec, followed by another
fixation cross presented for 1.5 sec. During this 2-sec
interval, participants had to indicate whether the probe
was part of the preceding memory set by pressing the
right or the left button on a response box with the cor-
responding hand. The response-hand assignments were
counterbalanced across subjects. Each memory set was
composed of letters chosen randomly from the set of let-
ters listed above, with the proviso that no identical letters
were allowed within the same memory set. The probe
was part of the memory set on 50% of the trials. Five mem-
ory set size conditions (2, 3, 4, 5, and 6) were used in a
blocked fashion and were presented in either ascending
(2–6) or descending (6–2) order, counterbalanced across
subjects. Set Sizes 2–6 were chosen because they encom-
pass the memory span predicted by Cowan (2001). For
each set size condition, a run consisted of four blocks of
eight trials each, with a 20-sec fixation period before the
first block and between each block. This yielded a total
of four task blocks (32 trials) and four rest blocks per
set size condition.
All subjects underwent a training session with 128 trials
using Set Sizes 4–6 before being tested inside the MRI
scanner. Further, a short training block with approximately
32 trials using Set Size 4 was administered just before the
fMRI recording began to ensure that participants remem-
bered the task instructions.
Data Acquisition and Preprocessing
The MRI data were recorded with a Siemens Allegra 3-T
head-only scanner. The fMRI data were recorded with a
fast echo-planar protocol (repetition time = 2 sec, echo
time = 25 msec, flip angle = 80°). Thirty-eight slices (3-mm
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
.
f
.
/
t
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Figure 1. Procedures and time line. Top: example trials for Set Sizes 2 and 4. Bottom: order of trials for one subject.
658
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 22, Number 4
thickness, 3-mm in-plane resolution, 0.3-mm gap) were
collected interleaved and parallel to the anterior and poste-
rior commissures. A high-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE
(192 slices, 1 × 1 × 1 mm) was also recorded to enable ac-
curate anatomical coregistration. Finally, a fast T2-weighted
image was also collected for coregistering the T2* image
used for fMRI with the T1 image used for anatomical analysis.
The neuroimaging data were preprocessed and analyzed
using FSL version 3.1 (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/ ). Struc-
tural images were processed with SUSAN (part of FSL) to
improve the signal-to-noise ratio and BET (part of FSL)
was used to perform skull stripping. BET was also used
on the functional images. In addition, the functional images
were slice-time corrected, motion corrected using MCFLIRT,
temporally filtered with a Gaussian high-pass cutoff of
70 sec, and spatially smoothed with a 6-mm FWHM three-
dimensional Gaussian kernel. Functional and structural
images were coregistered and transformed into the Mon-
treal Neurological Institute coordinates before group analy-
ses were carried out.
Data Analysis
Behavior
The behavioral data (RT and accuracy) were analyzed with
mixed-design ANOVAs with one between-subjects factor
(Age) and one within-subjects factor (Set Size). For the
ANOVA, the accuracy data were first transformed using
the Fisher logit approximation to avoid ceiling effects. Note
that, due to the use of a blocked fMRI design, we collapsed
across probe items requiring yes or no responses for all
these analyses. Further, no significant differences were
found between descending and ascending set size presen-
tation orders, so the data were combined for all behavioral
and neuroimaging analyses.
In addition, we also estimated the amount of informa-
tion transmitted (throughput), given the number of items
in the memory set. Throughput is derived according to
the following formula, which is mathematically identical
to the k formula introduced by Cowan (2001; see also
Cowan et al., 2005):
Throughput ¼ ACC − 0:5
0:5
(cid:1) N items;
where the chance level of 0.5 is subtracted from the un-
corrected overall accuracy (ACC), then range corrected
by dividing by 0.5 (as above-chance accuracy can only vary
between 0.5 and 1) and finally multiplied by the number
of items included in the memory set for that condition.
This formula corrects for chance level and takes into ac-
count that more information is available at higher load
levels. Note that if accuracy is 1 (perfect), the throughput
is equal to the number of items in the memory set, which
would indicate that all information available is processed
(ideal function in Figure 3). By measuring throughput
across increasing set sizes, we will be able to estimate WM
capacity as the maximum amount of information transmit-
ted across set sizes.
fMRI
The statistical analysis of fMRI data was carried out using
FEAT (fMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 5.63, part of FSL
(FMRIBʼs Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Group-
level analyses were carried out using FLAME (FMRIBʼs Local
Analysis of Mixed Effects) Stage 1 only (i.e., without the
final MCMC-based stage; Woolrich, Behrens, Beckmann,
Jenkinson, & Smith, 2004; Beckmann, Jenkinson, & Smith,
2003). The overall mean of each group was thresholded
using clusters determined by Z > 5.0 and a (corrected)
cluster significance threshold of p = .05 (Worsley, 埃文斯,
Marrett, & Neelin, 1992).1 A linear trend analysis was ap-
plied to the group-level analysis, separately for Set Sizes
2–4 and 4–6. Resulting Z (Gaussianized T/F) statistic images
were thresholded using clusters determined by Z > 3.1
and a (corrected) cluster significance threshold of p = .05
(Worsley et al., 1992).
To determine differences in MR activity by set size slopes
between younger and older adults, a peak voxel analysis
was performed.2 The voxel showing the largest Z-score
within each of a series of ROIs was selected for each
subject and condition. The ROIs were drawn according
to Brodmannʼs areas (BA) as implemented in the WFU
Pickatlas (http://www.fmri.wfubmc.edu; Maldjian, Laurienti,
Kraft, & Burdette, 2003). 具体来说, we used the follow-
ing ROIs, each separately for the left and right hemisphere:
BA 18/19, BA 7, BA 6, BA 24/32, BA 44/45/47, and BA 10.
Because the voxel with the largest slope was selected, A
bias toward higher values was introduced. 然而, 这
bias should operate equally for each set size condition
and for younger and older adults. 所以, 虽然
the actual values should not be considered as meaningful,
the comparisons of slopes for Set Sizes 2–4 and 4–6 and the
comparisons between younger and older adults are legiti-
mate. This procedure was selected over alternatives (例如,
using a fixed voxel per subject or per group) because these
alternative analyses may bias the results against the older
group due to increased anatomical variability with age,
which must be taken into consideration given the numer-
ous findings of brain matter loss with age (Gordon et al.,
2008; Rettmann, Kraut, 王子, & Resnick, 2006; Resnick
等人。, 2000; Raz et al., 1997). 更远, because the fMRI data
were spatially filtered, this procedure is analogous to con-
sidering the weighted average of the largest adjacent voxels
within each ROI.
The resulting peak-voxel data for each ROI were first
tested for a significant overall slope from Set Sizes 2–6,
collapsed across groups and set size conditions. 那些
showing a significant slope were then further analyzed to
determine the presence of significant slopes within the
younger and the older groups separately for both lower
(2–4) 及更高 (4–6) set sizes. We also compared how
Schneider-Garces et al.
659
D
哦
w
n
我
哦
A
d
e
d
我
我
/
/
/
/
j
F
/
t
t
我
t
.
:
/
/
F
r
哦
米
D
H
哦
t
w
t
n
p
哦
:
A
/
d
/
e
米
d
我
F
t
r
哦
p
米
r
C
H
.
s
p
我
我
d
v
我
e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H
米
A
我
r
e
.
d
你
C
哦
哦
米
C
/
n
j
A
哦
r
C
t
我
n
C
/
e
A
–
p
r
d
t
我
2
C
2
我
4
e
–
6
p
5
d
5
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
哦
5
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
哦
p
C
d
n
.
乙
2
y
0
G
0
你
9
e
.
s
t
2
哦
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e
米
乙
y
乙
e
G
r
你
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
/
.
t
.
F
.
哦
n
1
8
中号
A
y
2
0
2
1
数字 2. Mean RT (左边) 和
Fisher-transformed accuracy
(正确的) across set sizes for
younger and older adults with
estimated standard errors of
the means.
the two slopes (2–4 and 4–6) differed within each group
(younger and older adults).
结果
Behavioral Results
The mean RTs and Fisher-corrected accuracy values are
presented in Figure 2, separately for each age group, 塞斯-
锡安, and set size condition. Results from the mixed-design
ANOVA for RT revealed a significant main effect of set size,
F(4,156) = 55.97, p < .01,3 and a significant main effect of
age, F(1,39) = 7.16, p < .05, indicating that both younger
and older adults were slower with increasing memory load
and that older adults, overall, were significantly slower
than younger adults. The Set Size × Age interaction was
not significant, F(4,156) = 0.56, ns, indicating that the in-
crease in RT with increasing memory load was not signif-
icantly different for younger and older adults. To keep
behavioral analyses in line with those of fMRI data, we
also performed two-tailed t tests, directly comparing the
slopes for the younger and the older adults for low (2–
4) and high (4–6) set sizes. We also compared the slopes
for low and high set sizes separately for younger and older
adults. These comparisons revealed no significant effects
(for details, see Table 2).
Figure 3. WM capacity measured by throughput as a function of
set size, separately for younger and older adults, with estimated
standard errors of the means. The ideal function (accuracy = 1)
is provided for reference purposes.
The accuracy analysis also showed a main effect of set
size, F(4,156) = 11.47, p < .01, and a main effect of age,
F(1,39) = 12.08, p < .01, indicating that both younger and
older adults were less accurate for higher memory loads
and that the older adults were less accurate compared with
the younger adults, respectively. The Set Size × Age inter-
action was marginally significant, F(4,156) = 2.05, p < .10.
The throughput data are shown in Figure 3. The younger
adultsʼ function approached the ideal function, but with a
shallower slope. In other words, younger adults were able,
on average, to increase throughput up to 4.98 items, oc-
curring at Set Size 6, whereas older adults only showed an
increase up to 3.46 items, occurring at Set Size 5 with no
additional information throughput for Set Size 6. The sepa-
rate planned t tests revealed significantly different slopes
between groups for low and high set sizes, with the older
adults showing a smaller increase for low set sizes and nearly
no increase for high set sizes (see details in Table 2). These
data indicate that older adults may be unable to maintain, on
average, more than four items in WM because additional
items beyond Set Size 4 did not significantly increase their
throughput measurement. Younger adults, on the other
hand, may retain significantly more information.
fMRI Results
Overall mean contrasts for each age group are presented
in Figure 4 and show a number of regions being active
during the task. Younger adults showed foci of activation
in bilateral occipital, left parietal, left premotor, and left
medial frontal cortex. Older adults showed bilateral foci
of activation in occipital, parietal, premotor, and medial
frontal cortex. These results replicate findings reported
in other studies, indicating that WM tasks induce the
activation of a dorsal fronto-parietal network (Champod
& Petrides, 2007; Cabeza et al., 2004; Cabeza, Dolcos,
Graham, & Nyberg, 2002; Cornette, Dupont, Salmon, &
Orban, 2001; Jonides et al., 1997), which is left lateralized
in the younger adults and bilaterally activated in the older
adults. The presence of bilateral activity in older adults
in a task showing unilateral activity in younger adults is
a common observation in brain imaging studies, as sum-
marized by the HAROLD model (Cabeza, 2002). In ad-
dition, bilateral activation of occipital areas was found in
660
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 22, Number 4
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
/
.
.
t
.
f
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Table 2. Slopes for RT and Accuracy by Set Size, Separately for Low (2–4) and High (4–6) Set Sizes and Younger and Older Adults
RT
ACC
Throughput
Young
43.79
−0.17
0.98
Slopes 2–4
Old
62.69
−0.21
t Test
Young
1.38
0.41
64.10
−0.16
0.75
0.72
2.16**
t tests between groups (two tailed): df = 40.
*p < .1.
**p < .05.
***p < .01.
both younger and older adults. This may reflect the visual
nature of the task.
To examine load effects, we conducted linear trend
analyses, separately for low (2–4) and high (4–6) set sizes.
These analyses revealed a clear differentiation between
the two age groups, with younger adults showing no sig-
nificant (i.e., subthreshold) linear trends for low set sizes
but pronounced linear increases in several areas for high
set sizes and older adults showing significant effects at
low set sizes but no further significant increases at high
set sizes (see Figure 5).4 For the younger adults, low set
sizes were associated with foci of linear increase only in
left occipital cortex, whereas high set sizes were associated
with linear increases in bilateral parietal and frontal cortex
in addition to the left occipital cortex (see Table 3). The
older adults showed foci of linear increase in left occipital,
bilateral parietal, bilateral premotor, bilateral inferior front-
al, and medial frontal cortex at low set sizes but not at high
set sizes.
These data show both overrecruitment (at low set sizes)
and underrecruitment (at high set sizes) in older adults,
as postulated by the CRUNCH model. Interestingly, how-
Figure 4. Statistical brain maps (axial surface projection) of the task
minus rest contrast for younger and older adults, collapsed across
set sizes. LF = left front.
Slopes 4–6
Old
48.92
−0.27
t Test
0.87
1.04
0.02
3.95***
Slopes Difference
Young
20.31
0.01
−0.23
Old
t Test
−13.77
−0.05
−0.7
1.42
0.36
1.93*
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
.
/
.
f
t
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
ever, they also show some bilateral recruitment in younger
adults at high loads, suggesting that the recruitment of ad-
ditional areas may be a common mechanism to deal with
increasing task difficulty or load rather than a mechanism
geared at compensating for loss in neuronal efficiency that
is specific to aging.
To further examine these load effects, we focused our
statistical analyses on the peak voxels in the ROIs showing
large changes as a function of memory load, which includ-
ed BA 18/19, BA 7, BA 6, BA 24/32, and BA 44/45/47. For
each of these regions, the voxel corresponding to the peak
response was identified, separately for each subject, set
size condition, and hemisphere. Because the interest of
this study is to evaluate differences in brain activation as
a function of memory load, it is important to first minimize
the impact of individual (or group) differences on the
overall magnitude of the brain oxygen-level dependent
(BOLD) response. Therefore, we scaled the peak values ob-
served for each subject, hemisphere, and memory set size
by the amplitude of the largest response observed across
set sizes for each individual subject. These relative am-
plitude values were then used for all following analyses.
The brain-activation-by-memory-load functions for each
ROI peak voxel and for the average across ROIs are pre-
sented in Figure 6 (panels B and A, respectively). Results
of t test analyses are presented in Table 4. These data in-
dicate that, although an increase in brain activity as a func-
tion of memory load was observed for most areas in both
hemispheres, the pattern was quite different for younger
and older subjects: Whereas the younger adults showed
most of the increase between Load 4 and Load 6, the older
adults showed most of the increase between Load 2 and
Load 4.
Relationship between Relative fMRI Activation,
Memory Load, and WM Span
One of the most important predictions of the CRUNCH
model is that age-related differences in brain activity are a
reflection of the more limited processing capacity of the
older adults. As a consequence, older adults require a
Schneider-Garces et al.
661
Figure 5. Statistical brain
maps (axial surface projection)
of linear trend analyses for
Set Sizes 2–4 and 4–6 for
younger and older adults.
LF = left frontal.
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
t
/
.
.
.
f
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Table 3. Regions of Significant Linear Trends in Younger and Older Participants, Separately for Low (2–4) and High (4–6) Loads
Lobe
Region of Activation/
Linear Trend
Younger adults, linear trend for Set Sizes 2–4
Hemisphere/BA
x
y
z
Z Max
Cluster Size ( Voxel)
Occipital
Medial/inferior
L 18/19
−16
−100
−6
5.7
Younger adults, linear trend for Set Sizes 4–6
Frontal
Middle/premotor
Superior/middle/premotor
L 6/8
R 6/8
Medial
R/L 8, 32/24
Parietal
Inferior/superior
Inferior/superior
Occipital
Medial/inferior
Cerebellum
Older adults, linear trend Set Sizes 2–4
Frontal
Premotor/inferior
Inferior
Medial
Medial
Inferior
Inferior
Parietal
Occipital
Medial/inferior
L 7/40
R 7/40
L 18/19
R
L 44/6/43
R 45/46
L 6
R 32/24
L 7
R 7
L 18/19
−24
34
6
−28
32
−44
32
−40
52
−4
6
−26
30
−28
0
0
14
−46
−50
−86
−66
0
34
2
14
−76
−58
−96
50
42
46
46
44
−6
−24
28
18
60
44
38
30
2
4.38
3.77
4.25
4.86
4.02
4.79
4.03
4.93
5.04
5.77
4.7
4.66
3.93
4.83
558
601
426
966
1947
674
2232
344
813
254
277
217
397
426
1237
662
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 22, Number 4
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
f
t
.
/
.
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Figure 6. (A) Relative signal change of BOLD response as a function of set size, averaged across ROIs (peak values) in younger and older adults.
(B) Relative signal change of BOLD response as a function of set size, separately for each ROI (peak values). (C) Relative signal change of BOLD
response as a function of set size, averaged across ROIs (peak values), when WM load is adjusted as a function of each participantʼs span.
greater amount of brain activation to handle relative low
loads. This implies that differences between younger and
older adults should disappear once the load is computed
relative to each individualʼs WM span rather than in ab-
solute terms. The throughput measure that we have de-
scribed earlier can provide a tool for quantifying WM span
within the context of the Sternberg memory search task.
Specifically, we expect that, when WM span is reached,
the throughput measure should reach an asymptote—that
is, it should not increase with further increases in memory
load. The asymptotic (maximum) value can then be con-
sidered as an estimate of WM span, which can be assessed
individually for each participant in the study. Because we
were interested in using this measure to estimate the brain
activation levels relative to memory span, we approximated
this value to the nearest integer. The resulting estimates
of WM span (based on each personʼs throughput asymp-
tote) correlated significantly with O-SPAN measures (r =
0.38, p < .01, one tailed). This indicates that this measure
is a valid estimate of WM span. Importantly, it is also a mea-
sure obtained during the same task in which the fMRI was
recorded. Interestingly, this measure differed significantly
between younger (mean = 5.08 items) and older adults
(mean = 3.80 items), t(40) = 5.06, p < .0001, although
for both groups the estimates were relatively close to the
memory span ranges given by Cowan (2001).
Another useful characteristic of this measure is that it
allows us to evaluate, for each individual subject, what
amount of brain activity is required as a function of the
relationship between memory load and WM span. The
results of this analysis, averaged across all the ROIs that
showed significant increases of BOLD response as a func-
tion of relative set size and across all subjects in the study,
are presented in Figure 6C.
The data presented in this figure indicate that the BOLD
response increased in a sigmoid (rather than linear) fashion.
For very low memory loads (relative to WM span), the curve
was essentially flat. However, when memory loads were
Schneider-Garces et al.
663
Table 4. Two-tailed t Tests for Slopes of Activation as a Function of Set Size
Slope 2–6
Slope 2–4
Slope 4–6
Slope 4–6 Minus 2–4
t Test Overall Young
Old
t Test between Young
Old
t Test between Young
Old
t Test between
Overall
4.83***
1.84*
4.30***
BA 18/19 left
BA 18/19 right
BA 7 left
BA 7 right
BA 6 left
BA 6 right
4.37***
3.73***
4.05***
1.52
1.03
0.63
3.58***
4.28***
3.96***
4.17***
1.86*
3.65***
5.74***
2.60**
1.59
1.58
4.27***
1.76*
BA 24/32 left
5.78***
2.58**
4.76***
BA 24/32 right
4.06***
0.97
3.01***
BA 44/45/47 left
4.14***
3.91*** 2.87**
BA 44/45/47 right
2.53**
1.30
2.64**
−1.17
−1.35
−1.66
−1.44
−0.95
−1.02
−0.23
−0.34
−0.71
−0.17
−0.25
2.87** −0.48
2.68** −0.69
2.32**
2.17**
1.09
−0.67
1.27
2.53** −0.55
2.27**
0.80 −2.39**
2.10**
0.83 −2.21**
0.13 −2.47**
0.99 −2.25**
1.01
−0.48
1.06
−0.25 −1.98*
3.96*** −0.47
2.79**
1.11 −2.37**
1.59
−0.22
1.36
2.92** −0.23
2.87** −0.25
2.46**
0.39
0.68
−1.50
2.66**
2.61**
1.44
1.46
0.16 −1.06
0.54 −2.57**
1.22 −1.73
0.10 −1.22
−0.31 −2.26**
2.01*
1.59
2.16**
1.11
2.32**
0.77
1.93*
2.06**
0.90
0.98
Overall, df = 41; young, df = 11; old, df = 29; young versus old, df = 40.
*p < .1.
**p < .05.
***p < .01.
closer to span, the curve rose steeply but flattened once WM
span was attained, reaching an asymptote related to
the performance (throughput) asymptote. To confirm this
visual impression, a set of t tests were performed for
consecutive steps of increasing memory load, corrected
for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni procedure.
The step between span −1 and span showed a signifi-
cant increase in BOLD response, t(41) = 2.98, p < .05
(Bonferroni corrected). None of the other steps reached
statistical significance. Such sigmoid function was evident
for both younger and older adults, and in both groups
of subjects it reached its asymptote when the load was
equal to the WM span. No point in the curve showed a
significant difference between younger and older adults
(all tʼs < 1). Thus, the difference between the fMRI load
functions in younger and older subjects (presented in
the upper portion of Figure 6A) disappears when individ-
ual subjectsʼ WM spans are taken into consideration. This
finding is clearly consistent with CRUNCH. In addition, how-
ever, it also underscores the fact that a common mech-
anism may come into play as subjective load increases,
regardless of age (i.e., once individual differences in span
are taken into account).
To provide further evidence of the relationship between
the increase in BOLD activity with load and WM span, we
considered the relationship between the throughput mea-
sure representing the behavioral span and the load at
which the BOLD response reached its asymptote. This
was estimated as the first memory load condition at which
the fMRI activation reached 80% of its maximum value.
This point was established for each subject across regions.
The mean value of the fMRI asymptotic point was larger
for the younger (mean load = 4.67 items) than for the older
adults (mean load = 3.80 items), t(40) = 2.05, p < .05,
paralleling the difference in span size between the two
groups. In fact, the difference between the point of asymp-
tote in the fMRI load function and the span size was similar
across groups (younger adults = 0.42 items; older adults =
0.00 items; t = 1.01, ns). The relationship between these two
measures for individual subjects is presented in Figure 7.
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Figure 7. Three-dimensional scatter plot illustrating the relationship
between the point of asymptote in the fMRI activation function
and the WM span (maximum value in the throughput function).
z-Axis = number of subjects; x-axis = behavioral asymptote;
y-axis = fMRI asymptote.
664
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 22, Number 4
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
/
t
.
.
f
This figure indicates a significant correlation between these
two measures (r = .44, p < .002, one tailed).
DISCUSSION
Taken together, the results of this study provide strong,
quantitative support for the utilization of neural networks
proposed by CRUNCH (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008).
They further indicate that brain activity is nonlinearly
related to WM load. Both of these findings have impli-
cations: the first for theories of aging and the second
for theories of WM.
The results of this study are largely consistent with pre-
vious brain imaging data obtained with the Sternberg task
(Bunge et al., 2001; DʼEsposito et al., 2000; Henson et al.,
2000; Rypma & DʼEsposito, 1999). As in previous work, a
large number of areas were activated during this task, in-
cluding occipital, prefrontal, parietal, and medial areas.5
When all memory load conditions were combined togeth-
er and compared with the rest period (Figure 4), prepon-
derantly left hemisphere activations were observed
in younger adults, whereas more bilateral activations were
observed in older adults. Although this observation is con-
sistent with previous aging data (see HAROLD; Cabeza
et al., 2004; Cabeza, 2002; see also Reuter-Lorenz et al.,
1999), this group difference in lateralization was not evi-
dent when the memory-load conditions were contrasted
with each other, as bilateral differences in activation were
evident at high loads even for younger adults (Figure 5; for
a similar finding, see also Bunge et al., 2001). It is possible
that the age-related differences in lateralization regardless
of load may not be related to WM function per se (which
should vary with memory load), but to other aspects of
the task, such as perceptual and motor function, which
may be common to all memory load conditions.
The main purpose of this study was to quantitatively
investigate predictions made by CRUNCH (Reuter-Lorenz
& Cappell, 2008). CRUNCH is centered on the idea that
differences in overrecruitment and underrecruitment of
brain areas commonly observed between younger and
older adults may reflect age-related differences in process-
ing capacity (or ability). Older adults, for reasons that are
yet to be completely understood, require more resources
than younger adults for processing equivalent amounts of
information. For this reason, they require additional re-
cruitment of brain activity at lower task loads than younger
adults.
In our study, we manipulated task load in a parametric
fashion, using memory set sizes varying between 2 and 6.
The behavioral data indicated that older adults, on aver-
age, had significantly more problems with the task than
younger adults. This was particularly true at high (>4)
memory set sizes. 实际上, in these conditions, their accu-
racy declined, and the “throughput” analysis revealed
that they reached a ceiling in their capacity to transmit
信息 (IE。, to correctly identify, above chance, 差异-
ferent targets in the presence of increasing information)
at about four items. This number was significantly smaller
than that for younger adults (>5). 换句话说, 这
two age groups differed by more than one full item in
terms of their memory span.
This asymptotic performance level in the older adults
was associated with a clear asymptotic level in brain acti-
休假, as measured with fMRI. The fMRI data indicated
that a number of regions, 即, occipital cortex, pre-
frontal regions, dorsal parietal cortex, and cingulate cor-
tex, showed significant bilateral increases of activity with
set size, presumably reflecting the greater load that a
large memory set imposes on the information processing
系统. In all these areas, the activation-by-set-size func-
tions suggested, for the older adults, a large increase be-
tween Set Size 2 and Set Size 4 and a small-to-negligible
further increase at larger set sizes (见图 6). 这
pattern contrasts quite obviously with the data obtained
in the younger group. Although set size effects were ob-
served in similar areas in younger and older adults, 这
younger group showed smaller growth in brain activation
as a function of set size until at least Set Size 4. A pro-
nounced increase in brain activity in the younger group
was observed at higher set sizes.
If the fMRI data were observed in isolation, various hy-
potheses could be entertained about the significance of
these effects. 例如, it could be argued that the
neurovascular system is limited in its capacity to provide
additional oxygenated blood (and therefore flush out
deoxyhemoglobin leading to the BOLD signal) 然后
this limit is reached at lower loads in older than younger
adults. This would explain the earlier asymptote in the
较老的 (occurring at Set Size 4) than in the younger sub-
项目 (occurring at Set Size 6 or beyond). Although a pos-
sible role of an impaired neurovascular system on brain
function cannot be ruled out by the present study, 这
behavioral data clearly indicate that the older adults reach
a performance asymptote at about Set Size 4—a value
that represents a real limit in processing capacity rather
than a mere artifact of the measuring system.
因此, the data indicate that, whatever the reason, 较老的
adults reach an asymptote in both behavior and brain
activation at lower levels than younger adults. 在-
dividualized span analysis provides an even stronger
quantitative support for CRUNCH. It shows that the dif-
ferences in the brain-activation-by-memory-load function
between younger and older adults can be entirely ac-
counted for by differences in span across individuals, 关于-
gardless of their age. When these differences are taken
into account, the curves for younger and older adults
are virtually identical. 因此, no special mechanism is re-
quired to account for the different pattern of brain activ-
ity in older adults with respect to younger adults, as this
difference is explained by relative task difficulty. 虽然
some extant data (例如, Stern et al., 2005) are consistent
with the premises of CRUNCH (explicated in Figure 2 的
Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008), the current data dem-
onstrate for the first time that relative task difficulty alone
Schneider-Garces et al.
665
D
哦
w
n
我
哦
A
d
e
d
我
我
/
/
/
/
j
F
/
t
t
我
t
.
:
/
/
F
r
哦
米
D
H
哦
t
w
t
n
p
哦
:
A
/
d
/
e
米
d
我
F
t
r
哦
p
米
r
C
H
.
s
p
我
我
d
v
我
e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H
米
A
我
r
e
.
d
你
C
哦
哦
米
C
/
n
j
A
哦
r
C
t
我
n
C
/
e
A
–
p
r
d
t
我
2
C
2
我
4
e
–
6
p
5
d
5
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
哦
5
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
哦
p
C
d
n
.
乙
2
y
0
G
0
你
9
e
.
s
t
2
哦
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e
米
乙
y
乙
e
G
r
你
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
t
.
.
/
F
.
哦
n
1
8
中号
A
y
2
0
2
1
is sufficient to account for all of the age-related brain ac-
tivation differences, and no other mechanism is required.
This is made possible by the use of an individualized
measure of task-related ability, the throughput measure.
The activation-by-task-load functions are also useful to
highlight another important observation. 即, 他们
are clearly nonlinear, showing two asymptotes: a floor at
low set sizes and a ceiling at high set sizes, separated by
a region of rapid growth when memory load is approach-
ing span. The presence of a ceiling can be easily explained:
在某种程度上, the brain is not capable of producing fur-
ther activation (at least as measured with fMRI; see also
Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008). This is associated with a
ceiling in performance (when memory span is reached).
Although causality cannot typically be inferred with brain
imaging data alone, it is very tempting to hypothesize that
a common mechanism leads to both activation and per-
formance asymptotes. This common mechanism may be
a limited capacity of the WM system (a “cognitive” expla-
国家) or a limited capacity of the neurovascular supply to
大脑 (an “energetic” explanation). The current data
are insufficient to tease apart these hypotheses, 事实上
there may be no need to do that, as one could argue that
the neurovascular system is built to satisfy functional re-
quirements, and therefore it can be expected that the
two limits should coincide. 更远, the current study can-
not be used to determine which specific process of the
many involved during the Sternberg task is the one re-
sponsible for the limitations in performance and brain
activation. The blocked design and fast pace of the study
do not allow us to use the relatively slow hemodynamic
data provided by fMRI to determine whether the effects
are due to processes occurring during encoding, mainte-
南斯, or retrieval of information from WM. Other studies
based on event-related fMRI designs, often with longer
and variable delays (例如, Grady, 于, & Alain, 2008; Rypma
等人。, 2005; Rypma & DʼEsposito, 2000), or other brain im-
aging methods with higher temporal resolution such as
ERPs or event-related optical signal (Fabiani & Gratton,
2005; Gratton & Fabiani, 2001) can be more useful for this
purpose.
The presence of a floor effect in the brain imaging
data is not predicted by or related to CRUNCH and re-
quires some further consideration. There are three pos-
sible explanations for this phenomenon. 第一的, the floor
effect may be an artifact due to the insensitivity of the
hemodynamic measures to lower levels of brain activa-
的, especially when thresholding is used for statistical
分析. This possibility is difficult to rule out completely,
although the effect sizes present in this study are in line
with many other published reports. A more interesting
explanation for the floor effect is that it is related to the
presence of a real floor in brain activation. 有
two possible interpretations for this. 首先, 这是
embedded in the model proposed by Cowan (2001), 是
that WM can itself be partitioned into an easily accessible
core of highly activated nodes (whose use requires little
努力) and a “halo” of less highly activated nodes (谁的
use requires more substantial effort). In the present case,
when the set size is small, only the core of WM needs to be
accessed and very little effort (and brain activity) is re-
询问. 然而, when the set size is large, the halo comes
into play, with the consequence that a much greater effort
(and brain activity) is involved.
The second interpretation is that the brain activation
observed in this task is, to a great extent, related to the
necessity of maintaining independent chunks of informa-
tion active in WM. 换句话说, the difficulty is not in
maintaining the information in an active form but in
maintaining the different pieces of information as distinct
and eliminating cross talk. 在这种情况下, the amount of ac-
tivity should be related to the number of negative cross-
links that need to be established between the different
chunks, which should grow in a combinatory (or expo-
nential) fashion with the number of active chunks. 乙酰胆碱-
cording to this interpretation, brain activation should
grow exponentially until a ceiling is reached (due to lack
of sufficient resources for keeping so many different
representations distinct from each other). 因此, 这
brain-activation-by-set-size function should be a sigmoid,
with the largest growth occurring around span. A depic-
tion of this theoretical view is presented in Figure 8.
Within this framework, the main difference between
younger and older adults is that the sigmoid function
relating memory load to brain activity and performance
is shifted to the left in older adults (which is consistent
with the model proposed by Rypma, Eldreth, & Rebbechi,
2007). For either of the “cognitive” accounts described
多于, we would need to determine why older adults
有, 一般, a smaller span than younger adults, 阿尔-
though their respective functions are very similar when
scaled by the individual spans. With respect to the first
假设, this would suggest that the difference between
younger and older adults is in the size of the core area,
which could reflect the ability to maintain focus on par-
ticular memory nodes that carry task-relevant information.
D
哦
w
n
我
哦
A
d
e
d
我
我
/
/
/
/
j
t
t
F
/
我
t
.
:
/
/
F
r
哦
米
D
H
哦
t
w
t
n
p
哦
:
A
/
d
/
e
米
d
我
F
t
r
哦
p
米
r
C
H
.
s
p
我
我
d
v
我
e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H
米
A
我
r
e
.
d
你
C
哦
哦
米
C
/
n
j
A
哦
r
C
t
我
n
C
/
e
A
–
p
r
d
t
我
2
C
2
我
4
e
–
6
p
5
d
5
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
哦
5
C
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
哦
p
C
d
n
.
乙
2
y
0
G
0
你
9
e
.
s
t
2
哦
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e
米
乙
y
乙
e
G
r
你
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
.
/
.
t
.
F
哦
n
1
8
中号
A
y
2
0
2
1
数字 8. Schematic depiction of the relationship between subjective
WM load and related brain activation. The average function of older
adults is expected to be shifted toward the left, that of younger
adults toward the right.
666
认知神经科学杂志
体积 22, 数字 4
This may in turn be seen as an attention control function,
as in fact proposed by both Engle and Kane (2004) 和
考恩 (2001). 在这种情况下, older adults may have a smaller
WM span because they have poorer control of attention—
换句话说, the WM core may be less stable regardless
of its size. There is in fact substantial evidence that older
adults may have problems controlling attention and be
more distractible than younger adults (Hasher et al., 2008;
Hasher & Zacks, 1988). This in itself may reflect difficulties
in suppressing the processing of irrelevant information
and be related to a general deficit in inhibitory processes
(例如, Fabiani et al., 1998, 2006; Gazzaley, Cooney, Rissman,
& DʼEsposito, 2005; Fabiani & 弗里德曼, 1995).
有趣的是, inhibitory processes may also be very im-
portant for maintaining separate memory representations—
the crucial element in our second hypothesis. The inhib-
itory deficits commonly observed in older adults may then
account for the difficulty in maintaining separate memory
陈述: The increased brain activity observed at
lower loads in older adults may then reflect the require-
ment to overcome the deficit in reciprocal inhibition be-
tween different memory representations. 实际上, the two
hypotheses described above may be two sides of the
same coin: In both cases, deficits in inhibitory processes
may lead to an inability to maintain attentional focus and
to keep different memory representations active and dis-
tinct and in turn limit the effective size of WM span.
综上所述, the results of this study provide strong
quantitative support for CRUNCH and in particular for
the relative utilization of neural networks depending on
表现. They are less conclusive, 然而, regarding
the idea of compensation also embedded in CRUNCH.
The difference in brain activation as a function of load that
exists between younger and older adults can be entirely
accounted for by the difference in their WM capacity. 在
也就是说, the data indicate that, given equal objective
memory loads, individuals with lower memory abilities
are deploying more brain activation than those with higher
memory abilities, regardless of age. To the extent that “com-
pensation” is intended as the amount of effort needed to
reach a given level of performance, the data are consistent
with the predictions of the compensation hypothesis. 如何-
曾经, because of their correlational nature, the data do not
provide conclusive information about the causal direction
of the relationship between brain activity and behavior:
We cannot say whether the increased brain activity is used
to improve performance or whether the lack of some gen-
eral ability causes both the increased brain activity and the
decrement in performance. The data also show that the
brain-activation-by-memory-load functions are nonlinear,
displaying both a ceiling and a floor effect. The ceiling effect
is associated with a limit in WM span. The floor effect may
be a reflection of the large difference in the mental effort
required to maintain an increasing number of items active
and/or distinct within WM. This mental effort is likely to
be largely related to inhibitory processes, which may be
impaired in older adults.
致谢
This work was supported in part by NIA grant #AG21887 to
Monica Fabiani. We wish to thank Kirk Erikson and Paige Scalf
for advice regarding the fMRI analysis and Nelson Cowan and
Kathy Low for helpful comments on an earlier version of this
manuscript.
Reprint requests should be sent to Monica Fabiani, Beckman
研究所, University of Illinois, 405 North Mathews Avenue,
厄巴纳, 伊尔 61801, or via e-mail: mfabiani@illinois.edu.
Notes
1. A high Z threshold was used in this analysis (which com-
pares the activity during the task with the activity during fixa-
的) to prevent merging all the brain activations into a single,
large undifferentiated cluster, given the very large level of activ-
ity in this task. More standard and less conservative thresholds
were used for all other analyses.
2. Given the large size of each ROI, and the fact that in such
cases function within each ROI may not be unitary, we felt that
using the more standard practice of averaging the activity with-
in each ROI would not be appropriate. 然而, because sin-
gle voxel analyses may be unreliable, we also repeated the ROI
analysis using the average of a “box” encompassing 27 contiguous
voxels surrounding the peak as a method for providing a more
stable estimate of activity for each subject and condition. 那里-
sults were virtually identical to those obtained with the single voxel
分析. All of the effects that were significant at a p < .05 level
with the single voxel analysis were also significant with the 27-
voxel analysis, and those that were not still remained nonsignifi-
cant. The 27-voxel analysis is available as supplementary material.
3. The degrees of freedom in the behavioral analyses are re-
duced due to one subjectʼs missing the RT value for Set Size 2
because of response box malfunction.
4. Note that a lower threshold was used for the younger adults
to adjust for the smaller number of subjects in that group.
5. The ROIs used in the present study were selected to include
BA areas for which a significant load effect (independent of
age) was observed. No BA area in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
(DLPFC) made this cut. However, we did observe activation in
DLPFC (e.g., in BA 10) during the task, but this activity did not
significantly differ as a function of load. It is possible that the
apparent lack of load effects in DLPFC is due to the relatively
short duration of the maintenance period in the Sternberg task
used in this study when compared with that of previous fMRI
studies using the same paradigm (e.g., Rypma & DʼEsposito,
1999) or to the continuous maintenance required by the n-back
task used by Mattay et al. (2006).
REFERENCES
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory.
In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning
and motivation ( Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York:
Academic Press.
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1996). Manual for the Beck
Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. M. (2003).
General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis
in fMRI. Neuroimage, 20, 1052–1063.
Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal
memory span: A meta-analysis. Journals of Gerontology,
Schneider-Garces et al.
667
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
t
t
f
/
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
f
.
t
.
/
.
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,
60, 223–233.
Bunge, S. A., Ochsner, K. N., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H.,
& Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Prefrontal regions involved
in keeping information in and out of mind. Brain, 124,
2074–2086.
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in
older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging,
17, 85–100.
Cabeza, R., Daselaar, S. M., Dolcos, F., Prince, S. E., Budde, M.,
& Nyberg, L. (2004). Task-independent and task-specific
age effects on brain activity during working memory,
visual attention and episodic retrieval. Cerebral Cortex,
14, 364–375.
Cabeza, R., Dolcos, F., Graham, R., & Nyberg, L. (2002).
Similarities and differences in the neural correlates of
episodic memory retrieval and working memory.
Neuroimage, 16, 317–330.
Champod, A. S., & Petrides, M. (2007). Dissociable roles
of the posterior parietal and the prefrontal cortex in
manipulation and monitoring processes. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 104,
14837–14842.
Cornette, L., Dupont, P., Salmon, E., & Orban, G. A. (2001).
The neural substrate of orientation working memory.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 813–828.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term
memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–114.
Cowan, N., Elliot, E. M., Scott-Saults, J., Moray, C. C., Mattox, S.,
Hismjatullina, A., et al. (2005). On the capacity of attention:
Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive
aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 42–100.
Craik, F. (1968). Short-term memory and the aging process.
In G. Talland (Ed.), Human aging and behavior
(pp. 131–168). New York: Academic Press.
Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive
deficits: The role of attentional resources. In F. I. M. Craik
& S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes
(pp. 191–211). New York: Plenum Press.
DʼEsposito, M., Postle, B. R., & Rypma, B. (2000). Prefrontal
cortical contributions to working memory: Evidence from
event-related fMRI studies. Experimental Brain Research,
133, 3–11.
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention,
working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of
cognitive control. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology
of learning and motivation ( Vol. 44, pp. 145–199).
New York: Elsevier.
Fabiani, M., & Friedman, D. (1995). Changes in brain activity
patterns in aging: The novelty oddball. Psychophysiology,
32, 579–594.
Fabiani, M., Friedman, D., & Cheng, J. C. (1998). Individual
differences in P3 scalp distribution in older adults, and
their relationship to frontal lobe function. Psychophysiology,
35, 698–708.
Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2005). Electrophysiological and
optical measures of cognitive aging. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg,
& D. Park (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking
cognitive and cerebral aging (pp. 85–106). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Fabiani, M., Low, K. A., Wee, E., Sable, J. J., & Gratton, G.
(2006). Reduced suppression or labile memory?
Mechanisms of inefficient filtering of irrelevant information
in older adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18,
637–650.
Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J. W., Rissman, J., & DʼEsposito, M.
(2005). Top–down suppression deficit underlies working
memory impairment in normal aging. Nature Neuroscience,
8, 1298.
Gordon, B., Rykhlevskaia, E., Brumback, C. R., Lee, Y., Elavsky,
S., Konopack, J. F., et al. (2008). Anatomical correlates
of aging, cardiopulmonary fitness level, and education.
Psychophysiology, 45, 825–838.
Grady, C. L., Maisog, J. M., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G.,
Mentis, M. J., Salerno, J. A., et al. (1994). Age-related
changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual
processing of faces and location. Journal of Neuroscience,
14, 1450–1462.
Grady, C. L., Yu, H., & Alain, C. (2008). Age-related differences
in brain activity underlying working memory for spatial
and nonspatial auditory information. Cerebral Cortex,
18, 189–199.
Gratton, G., & Fabiani, M. (2001). Shedding light on brain
function: The event-related optical signal. Trends in
Cognitive Science, 5, 357–363.
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. (2008). Inhibitory
mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. Conway,
C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. Towse (Eds.),
Variation in working memory (pp. 227–249). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory,
comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view.
Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 22, 193–225.
Henson, R. N. A., Burgess, N., & Frith, C. D. (2000).
Recoding, storage, rehearsal and grouping in verbal
short-term memory: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia,
38, 426–440.
Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E. E., Lauber, E. J.,
Awh, E., Minoshima, S., et al. (1997). Verbal working
memory load affects regional brain activation as measured
by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 462–475.
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. W.
(2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory
capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
44, 169–183.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity,
proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on
long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26,
336–358.
La Pointe, L. B., & Engle, R. W. (1990). Simple and complex
word spans as measures of working memory capacity.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 16, 1118–1133.
Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Intellectual functioning
in old and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the
Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 12, 410–432.
Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H.
(2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and
cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data
sets. Neuroimage, 19, 1233–1239.
Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., Tessitore, A., Hariri, A. R., Berman,
K. F., Das, S., et al. (2006). Neurophysiological correlates
of age-related changes in working memory capacity.
Neuroscience Letters, 392, 32–37.
Mayeux, R., Stern, Y., Rosen, J., & Leventhal, J. (1981).
Depression, intellectual impairment, and Parkinson
disease. Neurology, 31, 645–650.
McIntosh, A. R., Sekuler, A. B., Penpeci, C., Rajah, M. N.,
Grady, C. L., Sekuler, R., et al. (1999). Recruitment of
unique neural systems to support visual memory in
normal aging. Current Biology, 9, 1275–1278.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or
minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing
information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
668
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 22, Number 4
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
f
.
t
.
.
/
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of
handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia,
9, 97–113.
Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S.,
Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial
and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology
and Aging, 17, 299–320.
Park, D. C., Welsh, R. C., Marshuetz, C., Gutchess, A. H.,
Mikels, J., Polk, T. A., et al. (2003). Working memory
for complex scenes: Age differences in frontal and
hippocampal activations. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 15, 1122–1134.
Payer, D., Marshuetz, C., Sutton, B., Hebrank, A., Welsh, R. C.,
& Park, D. C. (2006). Decreased neural specialization in
old adults on a working memory task. NeuroReport, 17,
487–491.
Raz, N., Gunning, F. M., Head, D., Dupuis, J. H., McQuain, J.,
Briggs, S. D., et al. (1997). Selective aging of the human
cerebral cortex observed in vivo: Differential vulnerability
of the prefrontal gray matter. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 268–282.
Resnick, S. M., Goldszal, A. F., Davatzikos, C., Golski, S.,
Kraut, M. A., Metter, E. J., et al. (2000). One-year age changes
in MRI brain volumes in older adults. Cerebral Cortex, 10,
464–472.
Rettmann, M. E., Kraut, M. A., Prince, J. L., & Resnick,
S. M. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses
of anatomical sulcal changes associated with aging.
Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1584–1594.
cognitive strategy and neural efficiency using event-related
fMRI. Cortex, 41, 582–594.
Rypma, B., & DʼEsposito, M. (1999). The roles of prefrontal
brain regions in components of working memory: Effects of
memory load and individual differences. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 96, 6558–6563.
Rypma, B., & DʼEsposito, M. (2000). Isolating the neural
mechanisms of age-related changes in human working
memory. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 509–515.
Rypma, B., & DʼEsposito, M. (2001). Age-related changes in
brain–behavior relationships: Evidence from event-related
functional MRI studies. European Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 13, 235–256.
Rypma, B., Eldreth, D. A., & Rebbechi, D. (2007). Age-related
differences in activation-performance relations in
delayed-response task: A multiple component analysis.
Cortex, 43, 65–76.
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of
adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review,
103, 403–428.
Spearman, C. E. (1927). The abilities of man. New York:
MacMillan.
Stern, Y., Habeck, C., Moeller, J., Scarmeas, N., Anderson,
K. E., Hilton, H. J., et al. (2005). Brain networks associated
with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults.
Cerebral Cortex, 15, 394–402.
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human
memory. Science, 153, 652–654.
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive
Todd, J. J., & Marois, R. (2005). Posterior parietal cortex
aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 177–182.
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Lustig, C. (2005). Brain aging:
Reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind. Current
Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 245–251.
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Marshuetz, C., Jonides, J., & Smith,
E. E. (2001). Neurocognitive aging of storage and executive
processes. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
13, 257–258.
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Stanczak, L., & Miller, A. C. (1999).
Neural recruitment and aging: Two hemispheres are better
than one, especially as you age. Psychological Science, 10,
494–500.
Riecker, A., Gröschel, K., Ackermann, H., Steinbrink, C.,
Witte, O., & Kastrup, A. (2006). Functional significance
of age-related differences in motor activation patterns.
Neuroimage, 32, 1345–1354.
Rypma, B., Berger, J. S., & DʼEsposito, M. (2002). The
influence of working-memory demand and subject
performance on prefrontal cortical activity. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 721–731.
Rypma, B., Berger, J. S., Genova, H. M., Rebbechi, D., &
DʼEsposito, M. (2005). Dissociating age-related changes in
activity predicts individual differences in visual short-term
memory capacity. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral
Neuroscience, 5, 144–155.
Veltman, D. J., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Dolan, R. J. (2003).
Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory
revisited: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 18, 247–256.
Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of
age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear
and nonlinear age effects and structural models.
Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231–249.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.
New York: Psychological Corporation.
Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E., Beckmann, C. F.,
Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. M. (2004). Multilevel linear
modelling for fMRI group analysis using Bayesian
inference. Neuroimage, 21, 1732–1747.
Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., Marrett, S., & Neelin, P. (1992).
A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation
studies in human brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow
and Metabolism, 12, 900–918.
Zarahn, E., Rakitin, B., Abela, D., Flynn, J., & Stern, Y. (2007).
Age-related changes in brain activation during a delayed
item recognition task. Neurobiology of Aging, 28, 784–798.
Schneider-Garces et al.
669
D
o
w
n
l
o
a
d
e
d
l
l
/
/
/
/
j
f
/
t
t
i
t
.
:
/
/
f
r
o
m
D
h
o
t
w
t
n
p
o
:
a
/
d
/
e
m
d
i
f
t
r
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
i
l
d
v
i
e
r
e
r
c
c
t
.
h
m
a
i
r
e
.
d
u
c
o
o
m
c
/
n
j
a
o
r
c
t
i
n
c
/
e
a
-
p
r
d
t
i
2
c
2
l
4
e
-
6
p
5
d
5
f
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
7
4
1
/
4
6
o
5
c
5
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
4
2
4
1
0
2
3
/
0
j
o
p
c
d
n
.
b
2
y
0
g
0
u
9
e
.
s
t
2
o
1
n
2
3
0
0
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
f
e
m
b
y
b
e
g
r
u
2
e
0
2
s
3
t
/
j
/
t
.
.
.
f
o
n
1
8
M
a
y
2
0
2
1