研究

研究

Changes in structural network topology correlate
with severity of hallucinatory behavior in
Parkinson’s disease

1,2

Julie M. 大厅
Joseph R. Phillips

1,2

, Claire O’Callaghan

2,3

2
, Alana J. 穆勒

, Ahmed A. Moustafa

, Simon J. G. Lewis

1,4

, Kaylena A. Ehgoetz Martens
2
, and James M. Shine

2

,

2

开放访问

杂志

1School of Social Sciences and Psychology, Western Sydney University, Milperra, NSW, 澳大利亚
2Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, 澳大利亚
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge,
剑桥, 英国

3

4

MARCS Institute, Western Sydney University, Milperra, NSW, 澳大利亚

关键词: Parkinson’s disease, Visual hallucinations, Diffusion tensor imaging, Network topology,
Graph theory, Connectomics

抽象的

Inefficient integration between bottom-up visual input and higher order visual processing
regions is implicated in visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease (PD). 这里, 我们
investigated white matter contributions to this perceptual imbalance hypothesis. Twenty-nine
PD patients were assessed for hallucinatory behavior. Hallucination severity was correlated
to connectivity strength of the network using the network-based statistic approach. 这
results showed that hallucination severity was associated with reduced connectivity within a
subnetwork that included the majority of the diverse club. This network showed overall
greater between-module scores compared with nodes not associated with hallucination
severity. Reduced between-module connectivity in the lateral occipital cortex, insula, 和
pars orbitalis and decreased within-module connectivity in the prefrontal, somatosensory,
and primary visual cortices were associated with hallucination severity. 反过来,
hallucination severity was associated with increased between- and within-module
connectivity in the orbitofrontal and temporal cortex, as well as regions comprising the
dorsal attentional and default mode network. These results suggest that hallucination severity
is associated with marked alterations in structural network topology with changes in
participation along the perceptual hierarchy. This may result in the inefficient transfer of
information that gives rise to hallucinations in PD.

作者总结

Inefficient integration of information between external stimuli and internal perceptual
predictions may lead to misperceptions or visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease (PD).
In this study, we show that hallucinatory behavior in PD patients is associated with marked
alterations in structural network topology. Severity of hallucinatory behavior was associated
with decreased connectivity in a large subnetwork that included the majority of the diverse
club, nodes with a high number of between-module connections. 此外, 变化
in between-module connectivity were found across brain regions involved in visual
加工, top-down prediction centers, and endogenous attention, including the occipital,
眶额, and posterior cingulate cortex. 一起, these findings suggest that impaired

引文: 大厅, J. M。, O’Callaghan, C。,
穆勒, A. J。, Ehgoetz Martens, K. A。,
Phillips, J. R。, Moustafa, A. A。, Lewis,
S. J. G。, & Shine, J. 中号. (2019). 变化
in structural network topology
correlate with severity of hallucinatory
behavior in Parkinson’s disease.
网络神经科学, 3(2), 521–538.
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00078

DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00078

支持信息:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00078
https://github.com/juliemaehall/
topology

已收到: 10 八月 2018
公认: 21 十二月 2018

利益争夺: 作者有
声明不存在竞争利益
存在.

通讯作者:
Julie M. 大厅
julie.hall@sydney.edu.au

处理编辑器:
Danielle Bassett

版权: © 2019
麻省理工学院
在知识共享下发布
归因 4.0 国际的
(抄送 4.0) 执照

麻省理工学院出版社

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

t

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

integration across different sides across different perceptual processing regions may result in
inefficient transfer of information.

介绍

Visual hallucinations (VHs) in Parkinson’s disease (PD) exist on a spectrum ranging from simple
misperceptions to complex well-formed images (巴恩斯 & 大卫, 2001). With disease progres-
sion and loss of insight, VHs constitute a major source of distress for the patient (Goetz, 2009;
Schrag, 2004) and comprise a high degree of burden for caregivers (Aarsland et al., 2007).
Risk factors of VHs include older age and disease duration, sleep and mood disturbances, 作为
well as cognitive decline (巴恩斯 & 大卫, 2001; Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon, & Ziégler, 2000;
Lenka, Hegde, Arumugham, & 朋友, 2017). 此外, previous work has shown that patients
with VHs show disruptions in attentional processing (Hall et al., 2016), reduced performance
on visuoperceptive tasks (巴恩斯, Boubert, 哈里斯, 李, & 大卫, 2003; Gallagher et al., 2011;
Ramírez-Ruiz, Junqué, Martí, Valldeoriola, & Tolosa, 2006), and decreased visual contrast sen-
sitivity, color discrimination (Diederich et al., 1998), and acuity (Matsui et al., 2006). 当前的
models of VHs have therefore focused on the interaction of perceptual and attentional dysfunc-
的 (for a review, see Muller, Shine, Halliday, & Lewis, 2014). Specifically, it has been pro-
posed that failure to effectively integrate information from different processing sites across the
perceptual hierarchy is likely to contribute to VHs and misperceptions in PD (Collerton, Perry,
& McKeith, 2005; Diederich, Goetz, & Stebbins, 2005; Muller et al., 2014; Shine, Halliday,
Carlos, Naismith, & Lewis, 2012).

Attention, prior experience, and expectations strongly influence perception. Perceptual pre-
措辞, generated from a myriad of modalities across the brain, guide perceptual processes
to facilitate the interpretation of noisy and ambiguous input (Bar, 2009; 恩格尔, 薯条, & 歌手,
2001; Summerfield et al., 2006). The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) processes coarse informa-
tion projected from the visual cortex and provides an “initial guess” of an object’s identity
(Summerfield & Koechlin, 2008). Previous work in PD patients with VHs has shown that the
accumulation of sensory evidence is slow and inefficient, which may result in an overreliance
on these top-down predictions (O’Callaghan et al., 2017). 重要的, top-down visual pro-
cessing regions can modulate neural activity in early visual regions, with expected stimuli
leading to reduced activity (迈耶 & 奥尔森, 2011). 此外, activity within the default
mode network (DMN), a network involved in mediating endogenous perception, 已经表明
to be increased during a misperception in this patient population (Shine, Halliday, 等人。, 2014).
所以, VHs may arise when perceptual input is not properly integrated and internally gen-
erated images interfere with the perceptual process (弗莱彻 & Frith, 2008; Intaite, Noreika,
Soliunas, & Falter, 2013; O’Callaghan et al., 2017; 权力, Kelley, & Corlett, 2016).

While functional neuroimaging studies have made significant contributions to our
理解 (Hepp, Foncke, Olde Dubbelink, 等人。, 2017; Ramírez-Ruiz et al., 2008; Shine,
Halliday, 等人。, 2014; Shine, 穆勒, 等人。, 2015; Yao et al., 2014), less is known about the
involvement of white matter changes in the manifestation of VHs in PD. Experiments using dif-
fusion tensor imaging (DTI) have reported altered white matter integrity in the optic nerve and
optic radiation (李等人。, 2016) as well as ascending tracts from the cholinergic nucleus basalis
of Meynert to parietal and occipital cortical regions (Hepp, Foncke, 贝伦德塞, 等人。, 2017).
然而, given the involvement of large-scale brain networks in perception, unique insights
into white matter changes associated with VHs can be gained by investigating whole-brain
network topology. Topological features of the human connectome allow us to describe the

网络神经科学

522

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

t

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

t

.

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

arrangement of connections within and between segregated submodules (布莫尔 & 斯波恩斯,
2009). Specifically, nodes that integrate these specialist communities are crucial for incor-
porating information streams of different modalities, which is essential for processes such as
洞察力 (Bertolero, 杨, & D’Esposito, 2015; 穆勒, O’Callaghan, 沃尔顿, Shine, & Lewis,
2017). 所以, investigating network topology can provide novel insights in changes across
different perceptual hierarchies.

The current study aimed to examine whether VHs are associated with changes in structural
network topology. To identify hallucinatory behavior in patients with PD, we assessed perfor-
mance on a computerized task capable of inducing misperceptions (Shine et al., 2012), 在
combination with a validated questionnaire that evaluates hallucinatory behavior in PD pa-
tients (Shine, 米尔斯, 等人。, 2015). We aimed to circumvent the sole reliance on self-reported
symptom occurrence while controlling for the possibility of misclassifying hallucinators as non-
hallucinators, when patients did not experience VHs during their clinic visit or when “passage”
hallucinations go unreported. 此外, by creating this composite score, we are able to
assess visual hallucinatory severity, rather than classifying patients into artificial dichotomous
patient groups.

We hypothesized that the severity of hallucinatory behavior would be associated with
ineffective information processing as shown by reduced between-module scores in visual net-
作品, reflecting reduced visual input to integration centers. 此外, increased between-
module scores across top-down perceptual prediction areas and the DMN could indicate an
overreliance on regions involved in the generation of internal percepts (Shine, O’Callaghan,
Halliday, & Lewis, 2014).

方法

Twenty-nine patients with idiopathic PD were included in this study. Demographic information
including age, disease duration, and levodopa equivalence daily dose (LEDD) were obtained
for all participants. All patients were assessed on the Hoehn & Yahr clinical stage (Hoehn &
Yahr, 1998) and the motor aspect of the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Dis-
ease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) part III (Goetz et al., 2008). Global cognition was assessed
using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Robins, & Helzer, 1983), and set-
shifting performance was assessed using the Trail Making Test Part B minus Part A (TMTB−A;
Tombaugh, 2004). The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Sydney
and was in accordance with the principles of the Helsinki Declaration. Written informed con-
sent was obtained from all participants before participation.

Bistable Percept Paradigm

All patients performed the bistable percept paradigm (BPP; Shine et al., 2012), a behavioral
task capable of inducing misperceptions in susceptible patients. In this task, patients were
presented with either single or bistable percepts (IE。, “hidden” images as shown in Figure 1) 为了
a maximum of 30 s in a randomized order. The participant had to decide whether the stimulus
was a single or hidden image by a button press and describe to the examiner what they had
见过. The recorded responses included the following: (A) correct single or correct hidden, (乙)
“missed,” recorded when the subject perceived a single image when a bistable percept was
提出, 和 (C) “misperceptions,” recorded when a subject incorrectly identified a single
image as a bistable image, 那是, incorrectly reported an image that was not presented on the
屏幕.

MDS Unified Parkinson’s Disease
Rating Scale:
A commonly used measure of
Parkinson’s disease clinical symptom
severity.

Bistable percept paradigm:
A computer-based task using bistable
percepts to measure visual
misperceptions.

网络神经科学

523

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

/

t

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

Psychosis and Hallucinations
Questionnaire:
A 20-item self-reported questionnaire
to measure hallucinatory
phenomenology.

数字 1. Example of single and hidden images of the BPP (Shine et al., 2012).

Psychosis and Hallucinations Questionnaire

All patients completed the Psychosis and Hallucinations Questionnaire (PsycH-Q; Shine, 米尔斯,
等人。, 2015). The PsycH-Q (Part A) consists of three subscales including (A) visual mispercep-
系统蒸发散, which includes questions about the presence of VHs, passage hallucinations, 和三个
frequently reported contents of VHs including people, 动物, 和物体; (乙) sensory mis-
perceptions, including audition, 触碰, olfaction, and gustation; 和 (C) disordered thought
and psychotic behavior. Participants rated the frequency of their symptoms on a 5-point Likert
规模, 范围从 0 (“never experienced”) 到 4 (“experienced daily”). The total score was
calculated by summing the responses (Shine, 米尔斯, 等人。, 2015; see Supporting Information,
Hall et al., 2019). Part B of the PsycH-Q assesses symptoms related to VHs (IE。, attention and
睡觉) and was not included in this study.

Composite Score

The percentage of misperceptions on the BPP (“indirect” measure of VH) and the total score
on the PsycH-QA (“direct” measure of hallucinatory behavior) were standardized and then
summed to create a composite score that reflected the severity of hallucinatory behavior (这里-
after referred to as the hallucination severity score, HSS). The HSS was correlated with the
demographic variables using parametric or nonparametric correlations depending on the dis-
tribution of the variables and was used as a correlate in the imaging analysis.

MRI Acquisition

All participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) using a 3-Tesla General Elec-
tric Discovery MR750 scanner (GE Medical Systems) with an 8-channel phased array head
coil. Diffusion-weighted images (DWI) were obtained by using echo-planar imaging sequences
和 61 different motion-probing gradient directions (TR/TE: 7,025/80 多发性硬化症, 55 transverse slices,
slice thickness: 2.5 毫米, 矩阵: 256 × 256, FOV: 240 × 240 毫米). The effective diffusion
2
weighting was b = 1,000 s/mm
)
were obtained at the beginning of each diffusion sequence. 3-D T1-weighted, anatomical im-
ages were obtained (TR/TE/TI: 7.2/2.7/450 多发性硬化症, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 毫米, 196 transverse slices,
256 × 256 矩阵, FOV: 256 × 256 毫米, flip angle 12
). The 3-D T1 images were used for
individual registration between T1-weighted anatomical and the DWI images and cortical
parcellation using FreeSurfer (version 5.3; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu).

, and four volumes with no diffusion weighting (b = 0 s/mm

2

网络神经科学

524

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

/

t

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

t

.

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

纤维束成像术:
Three-dimensional representation of
the brain’s white matter tracts derived
from diffusion MRI data.

Network-based statistic:
A statistical method to identify
connections and networks
comprising the connectome
associated with behavior.

Diffusion Tensor Imaging Preprocessing and Deterministic Fiber Tracking

DTI preprocessing was performed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL, http://fsl.fmrib.
ox.ac.uk). The preprocessing steps were as follows: (A) DTI images were corrected for sus-
ceptibility, head motion, and eddy current–induced geometrical distortions using FSL’s tool
eddy; (乙) a binary brain mask was created using bet; (C) images were realigned using a rigid
body registration to the b = 0 图像; 然后 (d) a tensor was fitted in each voxel (张, 琼斯, &
皮尔保利, 2005), 其次是 (e) the computation of the fractional anisotropy (F A) level based
on the eigenvalues for each voxel, in order to determine the preferred diffusion direction within
a voxel. FA thus serves as a surrogate measure of white matter integrity, with lower levels of
FA reflecting reduced white matter integrity (孙等人。, 2003; van den Heuvel & 斯波恩斯, 2011;
Verstraete et al., 2010). The preferred diffusion direction information was then used to recon-
struct the white matter tracts of the brain using a deterministic tracking approach based on
the fiber assignment by continuous tracking (FACT) algorithm (森, Crain, Chacko, & van
Zijl, 1999). Deterministic tractography yields less false positive tracts compared with proba-
bilistic methods (Bastiani, Shah, Goebel, & Roebroeck, 2012). False positives are detrimental
in network modularity as they occur more prevalently between than within modules (扎莱斯基
等人。, 2016). A streamline was started from eight seeds within each voxel of the brain (gray
and white matter) following the main diffusion direction of the voxel and stopped when (A)
the FA value < 0.1; (b) the traced fiber made a turn > 45○
; 或者 (C) the tract left the brain mask.
The images were acquired when reverse phase-encoding direction approaches were not the
standard procedure within acquisition protocols, which could have influenced the registra-
tion of diffusion and anatomical images. 所以, anatomically constrained tractography was
not applied (史密斯, 图尔尼耶, 卡拉曼特, & 康纳利, 2012). The atlas presented by Cammoun
等人. (2012) was used, 包括 219 cortical regions and 14 subcortical regions. The weighted
brain network was calculated for each participant, and consistency thresholding at 50% 曾是
applied (IE。, including the tracts found in 50% of the patients; de Reus & van den Heuvel,
2013). The mean density of the thresholded group matrix was 8.7%. To verify the results were
not skewed by the choice of threshold, we also applied the thresholding method that retained
most consistent edges across subjects but controlling for their distance (IE。, the consistency of
edges within “bins” based on their length to avoid preferential retention of short edges; Misic
等人。, 2015). The mean density of the group matrix using this threshold was 13.2%.

Network-Based Statistic

A network-based statistic (NBS) analysis was applied to investigate whether the HSS was asso-
ciated with altered connectivity strength in an interconnected subnetwork of the brain (扎莱斯基,
假如, & 布莫尔, 2010). NBS is a nonparametric method for connectome-wide analysis,
which aims to detect specific pairs of brain regions showing a significant effect of interest, 尽管
controlling for family-wise error (FWE) 速度. 重要的, no inferences of individual connec-
tions are made; instead the null hypothesis can only be rejected at the subnetwork level.
像这样, NBS is similar to the cluster-based multiple-comparison approaches used in stan-
dard functional MRI analysis. To identify changes in subnetworks associated with the HSS, 这
t statistic was set at 1.7, determined using the critical value of the t distribution for our sam-
ple size (Field, 2009). 此外, we verified the robustness of the results by controlling for
disease severity using the MDS-UPDRS III (motor part) as covariate. Connections were deemed
significant at FWE-corrected p value < 0.05 (one-sided) using 5,000 permutations. To investigate whether the subnetwork involved particular functional networks, we inves- tigated whether nodes in the subnetwork that correlated with the HSS overlapped with previ- ously defined resting-state networks. To this end, seven canonical resting-state networks from Network Neuroscience 525 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 3 2 5 2 1 1 0 9 2 7 0 4 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 7 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease the Yeo et al. (2011) atlas were overlaid with the structural parcellation and the percentage of nodes from each network included within the structural subnetwork that inversely related to HSS was calculated for each resting-state network. To analyze whether this overlap occurred significantly above chance, we randomly permuted the resting-state network identity of each region (5,000 iterations) and used the overlap between the randomized vector and the original node assignment to populate a null distribution. To test whether each individual resting-state network overlapped with the significant subnetwork, their overlap was compared with the null distributions. A resting-state network was identified as targeted if the true overlap was more than the 97.5th percentile of null distribution (i.e., the top 2.5%). A network was considered not to be associated with the HSS if the overlap was less than the 2.5th percentile of the null distribution. Graph Theoretical Analysis The graph organizational measures were computed using the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (http:// www.brain-connectivity-toolbox.net; Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). The thresholded, weighted brain networks were then partitioned into modules, which are nonoverlapping groups of highly connected nodes that are only sparsely connected with other modules, using the Louvain algo- rithm (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). To account for the stochastic nature of the Louvain algorithm, a consensus partition was identified by calculating the module assignment for each node 500 times. To define an appropriate value for the resolution parameter (γ), the Louvain algorithm was iterated 100 times across a range of values (0.5–2.0 in steps of 0.1) of the group mean con- nectivity matrix and then the similarity of the resultant partitions was estimated using mutual information. The γ parameter of 1.9 provided the most robust estimates of topology across the iterations and was used to determine the optimal resolution of the network modularity. After the nodes were assigned to their modules, their intra- and intermodular connectivity were calculated. Intramodular connectivity was calculated using the module degree z-score Wi (see Equation 1), in which a positive score reflects high within-module connections (compared with the node’s average number of connections), and negative z-scores denote the opposite. In- termodular connectivity was calculated using the participation coefficient Bi (see Equation 2). Low Bi values indicate few between-module connections, whereas high Bi values indicate uniformly distributed connections across modules (Hall, 2018). High Wi and high Bi scores are not mutually exclusive (Guimerà & Nunes Amaral, 2005). Wi = κi − κsi σκsi (1) Module degree z-score: A measure of diversity of intramodular connections of individual nodes. Participation coefficient: A measure of diversity of intermodular connections of individual nodes. Equation 1: Module degree z-score Wi, where κi is the strength of the connections of region i to other regions in its module si, κsi is the average of κ over all the regions in si, and σκsi is the standard deviation of κ in si. Bi = 1 − ∑nM s=1 (cid:2) (cid:3)2 κis κi (2) Equation 2: Participation coefficient Bi, where κ is is the strength of the positive connections of region i to regions in module s, and κ i is the sum of strengths of all positive connections of region i. The participation coefficient of a region is therefore close to 1 if its connections are uniformly distributed among all the modules and 0 if all of its links are within its own module. To test whether nodes within the subnetwork identified using the NBS analysis differed from nodes not included in the subnetwork, the average Wi and Bi of the subnetwork were contrasted against the average Wi and Bi of the nodes not included in the subnetwork using nonparametric permutation testing. Network Neuroscience 526 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 3 2 5 2 1 1 0 9 2 7 0 4 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 7 8 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease Diverse club: A subset of nodes in a network with high participation coefficients that exhibit properties consistent with an integrative network function. To test whether the HSS correlated with the Wi and Bi nodes across the whole-brain con- nectome, a Spearman’s rho correlation was performed followed by a nonlinear permutation test using 5,000 iterations to control for multiple comparisons (Nichols & Holmes, 2002), using an alpha of 0.05. This approach was repeated using the different threshold (Misic et al., 2015), and the outcome was correlated to the Wi and Bi using the original threshold. Both the Wi and the Bi scores calculated using the aforementioned threshold highly correlated with the Wi and Bi scores calculated with the consensus threshold (r = 0.92 and r = 0.94, respectively), indicating that the results were not biased by the chosen thresholding method. Diverse Club Analysis We identified the “diverse club” of the network, which comprised the top 20% of Bi nodes (Bertolero, Yeo, & D’Esposito, 2017). These nodes play an important role in network inte- gration, and changes to these nodes could affect between-module communication (Bertolero et al., 2017). We normalized the diverse club coefficient in reference to a null model: A random vector with a preserved modular structure was created by randomizing the mean participation coefficient of each node for 5,000 iterations. The diverse club was identified as those regions with a participation coefficient greater than the 95th percentile of the permuted distribution. We investigated whether the number of diverse club nodes was significantly higher within the subnetwork associated with the HSS, compared with nodes that were not included in the subnetwork identified using the NBS analysis. RESULTS Demographics Table 1 presents the descriptive variables of the 29 patients. The mean percentage of misper- ceptions on the BPP was 18.48 (range: 0–49), and the mean score on the PsycH-QA was 9.48 (range: 0–34, max score = 52), highlighting a diverse range of hallucinatory behavior in the patient cohort. The PsycH-QA and the BPP score showed a positive, significant correlation (r = 0.52, p = 0.004). Finally, to verify the severity score to the “gold standard,” we corre- lated the HSS in a large cohort of patients with PD and Lewy body dementia (n = 75) to the MDS-UPDRS item 2 and found a correlation of r = 0.53 (p < 0.001). However, given higher construct validity (Shine, Mills, et al., 2015), we opted to include the PsycH-QA and BP scores in the composite score (HSS) for the remainder of our analysis. Table 1. Demographics and clinical variables Mean (range) 66.8 ± 8 (51 – 84) 5.8 ± 4 (1.2–16) 617.8 ± 392 (125–1548) Variable Age (y) Duration (y) LEDD H & Y MDS-UPDRS III MMSE TMTB−A BPP % misperceptions PsycH-QA LEDD = levodopa equivalence daily dose; MDS-UPDRS III = motor part of the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; H & Y = Hoehn and Yahr; MMSE = Mini-Mental = Trail Making Test Part B – Part A; BPP = bistable percept paradigm; State Examination; TMTB−A PsycH-QA Correlation with HSS r (p value) 0.18 (0.345) −0.05 (0.785) −0.06 (0.767) 0.01 (0.949) 0.29 (0.125) −0.06 (0.762) 0.33 (0.080) − − 1.9 ± 0.5 (1–3) 30.0 ± 14 (7–55) 28.5 ± 2 (25–30) 78.9 ± 65 (−1−123) 18.5 ± 17 (0–49) 9.5 ± 8 (0–34) = Psychosis and Hallucinations Questionnaire, Part A. Network Neuroscience 527 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 3 2 5 2 1 1 0 9 2 7 0 4 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 7 8 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease The HSS showed a positive correlation trending towards significance with the TMTB−A (r = 0.33, p = 0.08). No significant correlations were observed between the HSS and other demographic and clinical variables. The HSS Correlated With Decreased Connectivity in a Large Subnetwork As illustrated in Figure 2, the NBS analysis revealed a subnetwork comprising 183 edges (8% of the edges in the thresholded connectivity matrix) and 127 nodes with reduced FA-based connectivity strength correlated to the HSS (p < 0.05). Using disease severity as a covariate, the NBS analysis revealed a similar subnetwork comprising 177 edges and 135 nodes that showed a correlation with the HSS that was trending towards significance (p = 0.059). The effects presented with a fairly liberal threshold, suggesting the changes related to the HSS are subtle yet topological extended (Zalesky et al., 2010). The size of networks identified using a range of t statistics are presented in the Supporting Information (Hall et al., 2019). No significant subnetwork was identified in the opposite direction (positive correlation between the HSS and connectivity strength). Furthermore, the group average Bi score within the subnetwork was 0.506, which was significantly higher (p < 0.05) than nodes outside this network, which show a group average of 0.310. The group average Wi score of the nodes within the network was higher than the group average Wi score of nodes outside the network (0.149 and −0.178, respectively), yet this difference did not remain significant when controlling for disease severity (p = 0.104). l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 3 2 5 2 1 1 0 9 2 7 0 4 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 7 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 2. NBS analysis reveals a subnetwork, comprising 183 edges and 127 nodes with reduced connectivity strength correlated to increased HSS (p < 0.05). Figure visualized with BrainNet Viewer (Xia, Wang, & He, 2013). Network Neuroscience 528 Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease Figure 3. Overlap between the identified structural subnetwork and functional resting-state net- works. (A) The percentage of nodes included in the subnetwork for each resting-state network. DMN = default mode network; VAN = ventral attentional network; DAN = dorsal attentional net- work. (B) The functional resting-state networks of the Yeo et al. (2011) atlas. The Subnetwork Includes All Subcortical Nodes but Did Not Target a Specific Cortical Resting-State Network The subnetwork that showed decreased connectivity strength correlated with the HSS included all 14 subcortical nodes (p < 0.05). As illustrated in Figure 3, the subnetwork further included nodes across the cortex. However, none of the other the resting-state networks were signifi- cantly correlated with the HSS (p > 0.05), yet the somatomotor network was relatively spared
(p < 0.05). Nodes in the Subnetwork Show High Participation Scores Eighteen nodes were included in the diverse club (see Supporting Information, Hall et al., 2019). Seventeen of the eighteen nodes (94%) of the diverse club were included in the afore- mentioned subnetwork, which was deemed significantly above chance (p < 0.001). As illustrated in Figure 4, nodes with high participation coefficients were more often part of the subnetwork. The HSS Correlated With Wi and Bi Scores When investigating the whole structural connectome, the HSS positively correlated to regional Bi (i.e., higher participation scores were associated with higher severity values) for nodes in the left medial OFC, a node in the right anterior and left posterior cingulate, precuneus, and the caudal middle frontal gyrus. Furthermore, nodes in the right occipital, pars orbitalis, and insula showed negative correlations between the HSS and participation coefficient (i.e., lower participation scores were associated with higher scores on the HSS; see Table 2 and Figure 5). However, when controlling for disease severity, the insula and medial OFC were only trending towards significance, while the lateral occipital cortex and anterior posterior cingulate did not remain significant. Increased HSS scores were further associated with increased module degree z-scores in the right thalamus, bilateral lingual, left medial OFC, pars opercularis, supramarginal gyrus, and superior temporal cortices. The right lateral occipital cortex also showed a positive correlation, but this did not remain significant after controlling for disease severity. Decreased HSS scores were associated with increased module degree z-scores in the bilateral precuneus, left parts triangularis, rostral middle frontal, and superior parietal cortex. The right pericalcarine and Structural connectome: Brain connectivity graphs obtained from diffusion MRI and tractography. Network Neuroscience 529 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 3 2 5 2 1 1 0 9 2 7 0 4 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 7 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 3 2 5 2 1 1 0 9 2 7 0 4 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 7 8 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 4. Nodes ranked according to the Bi scores. Blue: nodes included in the subnetwork; gray: nodes not included in the subnetwork correlated to the HSS. Table 2. Spearman’s rho correlation between the participation coefficient and the HSS (p < 0.05; permutation test) X Y Z Rho Rho* Subnetwork Node Positively correlated Frontal ctx-lh-medialorbitofrontal_2 ctx-rh-caudalmiddlefrontal_2 Parietal ctx-lh-precuneus_2 Cingulate ctx-lh-posteriorcingulate_2 ctx-rh-superiorfrontal_3 Negatively correlated Frontal ctx-rh-parsorbitalis_1 Occipital ctx-rh-lateraloccipital_5 Insular ctx-rh-insula_1 * Rho after controlling for disease severity; right hemisphere. −5 40 33 15 −20 39 0.37 0.43 # 0.35 0.53 −10 −44 −8 11 −43 40 46 21 40 0.40 0.37 0.57 0.36 0.53 0.31ˆ 43 −10 −0.46 −0.46 −73 2 −0.39 −0.21ˆ 43 47 36 −18 # p < 0.1; ˆ p > 0.1. ctx-lh = left hemisphere; ctx-rh =

−0.40

−0.32

13

(西德:2)

#

(西德:2)
-

(西德:2)

(西德:2)
(西德:2)

(西德:2)

(西德:2)

网络神经科学

530

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

t

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

数字 5. Significant changes in between- and within-module scores associated with the HSS.
(A) Nodes with a significant correlation between participation coefficients and the HSS. (乙) Nodes
with a significant correlation between the module degree z-score and HSS. Green indicates a posi-
tive correlation, red indicates a negative correlation. Larger nodes are part of the subnetwork. 数字
visualized with BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013).

precentral gyrus also a showed negative correlation with the HSS, yet this did not remain
significant after controlling for disease severity (见表 3 和图 5).

讨论

The aim of this study was to determine whether changes in structural network topology were
associated with hallucinatory behavior in PD. We showed that severity of hallucinatory behav-
ior was negatively correlated with connectivity across a bilateral subnetwork. Regions within
this subnetwork showed higher participation compared with regions outside this network. 这
loss of connectivity strength may force the system to adapt and reroute information across less
efficient pathways, impeding the standard sensory integration process. 重要的, 94% 的
nodes in the diverse club were included in this subnetwork. This community of high participa-
tion nodes is thought to control the integration of relatively segregated regions (Bertolero et al.,
2017). 的确, the diverse connectivity pattern of these nodes makes them crucial for the func-
tional coordination of brain regions during tasks, and activity in these nodes predicts changes
in the coupling of other regions (Bertolero et al., 2017). Severity of hallucinatory behavior may
thus be the result of impaired integration and segregation of brain networks or “modules,”
affecting effective information transfer. 最后, we showed regional changes in participation
associated with hallucination severity (the HSS score), with a positive correlation between par-
ticipation scores in the medial OFC, cingulate, precuneus, and middle frontal gyrus and the
HSS and negative correlation with participation scores in the lateral occipital cortex, pars or-
bitalis, and insula. These findings suggest a reweighting of the regions along the perceptual
等级制度, which may give rise to hallucinations.

网络神经科学

531

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

桌子 3. Spearman’s rho correlation between the module degree z-score and the HSS (p < 0.05; permutation test) X Y Z Rho Rho* Subnetwork Node Positively correlated Subcortical Right-Thalamus-Proper Frontal ctx-lh-medialorbitofrontal_1 ctx-lh-parsopercularis_2 Parietal ctx-lh-supramarginal_3 Temporal ctx-rh-superiortemporal_4 Occipital ctx-rh-lateraloccipital_3 ctx-lh-lingual_2 ctx-rh-lingual_1 Negatively correlated Frontal ctx-lh-parstriangularis_1 ctx-lh-rostralmiddlefrontal_5 ctx-rh-precentral_6 Parietal ctx-lh-precuneus_2 ctx-lh-superiorparietal_6 ctx-rh-precuneus_2 Occipital ctx-rh-pericalcarine_1 13 −17 7 0.49 0.45 −7 −47 52 14 −11 15 0.40 0.41 0.43 0.44 −55 −34 35 0.45 0.53 46 −6 −11 0.57 0.47 −94 −70 −83 33 56 −17 −44 −76 −54 −6 0 −7 2 2 67 46 29 26 0.39 0.42 0.41 −0.50 −0.57 −0.39 −0.46 −0.41 −0.39 # 0.34 0.50 0.37 −0.43 −0.50 −0.35 # −0.43 −0.47 −0.43 27 −10 14 −43 −31 22 −10 −22 12 15 (cid:2) − − − − − (cid:2) − (cid:2) (cid:2) (cid:2) (cid:2) (cid:2) (cid:2) * Rho after controlling for disease severity; right hemisphere. −75 # p < 0.1; ˆ p > 0.1. ctx-lh = left hemisphere; ctx-rh =

−0.24ˆ

−0.38

12

(西德:2)

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

t

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Lower participation of the lateral occipital cortex may reflect reduced early visual process-
英, resulting in ineffective accumulation of visual information from the environment. 以前的
work using a Bayesian drift diffusion model has demonstrated that accumulation speed and
quality of perceptual information are reduced in PD patients with VH (O’Callaghan et al.,
2017). 此外, reduced quality or integration of visual stimuli may increase perceptual
uncertainty, a suggestion that aligns with increased participation in the dorsal anterior cingu-
late cortex (Keri, Decety, Roland, & Gulyas, 2004). Perceptual uncertainty may place excessive
emphasis on top-down prediction centers, which subsequently could lead to a reduced activity
in early visual regions (迈耶 & 奥尔森, 2011). 然而, the reduced participation score in
the occipital and anterior cingulate cortex may not be specific to hallucinatory behavior, 作为
the negative correlations did not remain significant after controlling for disease severity.

This emphasis on top-down visual processing centers is supported by the increased partici-
pation coefficient and module degree z-score in the medial OFC. The OFC has an integrative
function across brain networks, as evidenced by its high participation coefficient. 此外,
this region is thought to facilitate recognition during visual perception by integrating incom-
ing sensory information with previous experiences and expectations (Panichello, 张, &
Bar, 2012). During typical visual perception, the OFC is activated early in response to vi-
sual stimuli, receiving low spatial frequency signals from the visual cortex (Bar et al., 2006).
尤其, only stimuli resembling known objects are shown to activate the OFC, which in turn

网络神经科学

532

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

generates a semantic association and provides a predictive signal
to the visual system
(Chaumon, Kveraga, Barrett, & Bar, 2014). 反过来, visual stimuli that carry no meaningful
association do not activate the OFC in healthy individuals. 因此, it could be speculated that
because of decreased quality of visual input, inappropriate recruitment of the OFC occurs,
which may result in ascribing false associative information to visual stimuli.

The manifestation of VHs has previously been recognized as a dysfunction between the
attentional networks (Shine, 穆勒, 等人。, 2015). Specifically, patients with VHs are shown to
be less able to recruit the dorsal attentional network (DAN), which enables the selection of
appropriate sensory stimuli (科尔贝塔, Patel, & 舒尔曼, 2008). With reduced control of this
网络, ambiguous stimuli might instead be interpreted by the ventral attentional network
(VAN) and the DMN, which are less well equipped for this task. Our results showed increased
participation in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), a key hub of the DMN. PCC activity has
been implicated in regulating the focus of attention, specifically the shift from the external
world into internal mentation (Weissman, 罗伯茨, Visscher, & Woldorff, 2006). 此外,
the PCC is involved in mind wandering and supports internally directed cognition (水蛭 &
锋利的, 2013). A failure to suppress PCC activity may lead to the intrusion of internal thoughts
into task performance (Sonuga-Barke & Castellanos, 2007). 而且, a positive correlation
was found between the HSS and the module degree z-score of the left pars opercularis, A
node in the VAN, a network that is activated when expectations in perception are violated
(科尔贝塔 & 舒尔曼, 2002; Shine, Halliday, 等人。, 2014). 反过来, a negative correlation
between the HSS scores and module degree score and participation coefficients was found
in other nodes of the VAN, namely the left pars triangularis, the right pars orbitalis, 并在-
sula. The left pars triangularis supports resolving competition between simultaneously active
陈述 (Badre & 瓦格纳, 2007), while the insula plays an important role in dynami-
cally shifting attention between the attentional control networks (Menon & Uddin, 2010). 这
anterior insula has previously been shown to be reduced in volume in PD patients with VH
(Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al., 2010; Shine, Halliday, 等人。, 2014). 一起, these results suggest that
ineffective communication between attentional networks in the brain may predispose an in-
dividual to hallucinate. 出奇, the participation coefficient of a node within the DAN
(“ctx-rh-caudalmiddlefrontal_2”) showed a positive correlation with the HSS. This node was
not part of the subnetwork, and it may be possible that this is a compensatory response to
the loss of connectivity strength in the other DAN regions. 尤其, the connectivity matrix
shows between module connections of this region with nodes in the somatomotor and the
frontoparietal network, but not with the DMN or VAN.

最后, all nodes that showed negative correlations with the HSS were included in the
subnetwork. Decreased within-module scores were found across the prefrontal and the
somatosensory association cortex, as well as in the primary visual cortex, while there was
a positive correlation between the HSS and the bilateral secondary visual cortex, perhaps as a
result of the decreased visual input from V1. 此外, the supramarginal gyrus, a node that
has been shown to be functionally active during spatial perception but also during visual im-
agery (Ganis, 汤普森, & Kosslyn, 2004), showed an increased module degree z-score with
increasing severity of VHs. 此外, a positive correlation with the HSS and the module
degree score in the superior temporal cortex, a region involved in auditory processing, 曾是
成立. It could be speculated that increased visual uncertainty may stimulate other sensory
processing areas. 反过来, previous work in schizophrenia has shown increased activation
in the superior temporal cortex during a mismatch between expected and perceived auditory
feedback (Fu et al., 2008). The HSS also included hallucinations of other sensory modali-
领带, and it could be speculated that a greater emphasis is being placed on predictions of

网络神经科学

533

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

/

t

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

auditory stimuli, as reflected as increased within-module connectivity in the superior temporal
cortex.

This study has several limitations worth noting. 第一的, the DWI data were acquired without
EPI distortion correction. This may have affected the accuracy of registration between DWI and
T1 images in the frontal and temporal cortices. Because of relatively low diffusion weighting
used in the current MRI protocol, it was chosen to employ DTI rather than more sophisticated
methods such as constrained spherical deconvolution, an algorithm that more adequately deals
with multiple fiber directions within one voxel than DTI. 此外, after controlling for dis-
ease severity, the identified subnetwork was only trending towards significance. The correlation
between the HSS and the participation coefficients of the lateral occipital and anterior cingulate
did not remain significant, while the OFC and insula were only trending towards significance,
and similar patterns were found for the module degree z-score: correlation with the HSS and
nodes in the occipital and precentral regions did not remain significant. 然而, this may
be expected given the close relationship between overall symptom severity and phenotypic
symptom severity. 重要的, the correlation coefficients showed notable consistency across
the analyses, suggesting the HSS was able to distinguish interindividual variability. While the
current study included PD patients with no self-reported hallucinations (n = 6), no data from a
healthy control group were acquired, which could have aided in disassociating the significant
network measures and disease burden. The LEDD score was not associated with the HSS in
the current study, and the relation between hallucinations and a LEDD-related side effect has
been dismissed in previous work (Fénelon et al., 2000; Graham, Grünewald, & Sagar, 1997;
Sanchez-Ramos, Ortollm, & Paulson, 1996; Shergill, 沃克, & Le Katona, 1998). 此外,
hallucinations may occur in the absence of dopaminergic treatment in PD or dementia with
Lewy bodies (Ala, 哪个, 宋, & 弗雷, 1997). 所以, the current study chose not to include
LEDD score as a covariate. 此外, the investigation was conducted in a relatively small
group of PD patients, and future studies should replicate our findings in a larger sample size.
最后, this study chose to include the PsycH-Q to assess hallucinations. This is an in-depth
questionnaire with high construct validity; 然而, patients have to report their hallucinations
他们自己. 理想情况下, a trained neurologist or qualified researcher, who can probe the patients
when in doubt, should assist with this process.

结论

We cautiously conclude that hallucinatory behavior in PD patients is associated with marked
alterations in structural network topology. Severity of hallucinatory behavior was associated
with decreased connectivity in a large subnetwork that included the majority of the diverse
club. These changes may result in an inefficient rerouting of information across less efficient
pathways, which may lead to impaired visual integration processes. 此外, nodes within
the orbitofrontal cortex and temporal lobes showed increased participation scores, 而
visual association cortex, insula, and middle frontal gyrus showed decreased scores associ-
ated with the HSS score. These findings suggest that impaired integration across different re-
gions along the perceptual hierarchy may result in inefficient transfer of information. A failure
to effectively switch between attentional networks and the intrusion of internal percepts could
give rise to perceptual glitches, such as misperceptions and hallucinations.

致谢

We thank the patients and their families who contribute to our research at the Parkinson’s Dis-
ease Research Clinic. 我们感谢博士. Váša for sharing his thresholding code (https://github.com/

网络神经科学

534

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

/

t

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

frantisekvasa/matlab_general). The DWI data were processed during the 10kin1day initiative at
the Dutch Connectome Lab. This research was supported by Sydney Informatics Hub, funded
by the University of Sydney.

作者贡献

Julie M. 大厅: 概念化; 数据管理; 形式分析; 调查; Methodol-
奥吉; 项目管理; 可视化; Writing – original draft. Claire O’Callaghan: 数据
curation; 调查; 方法; 写作——复习 & 编辑. Alana Justine Muller: 数据
curation; 写作——复习 & 编辑. Kaylena A. Ehgoetz Martens: 写作——复习 & edit-
英. Joseph R. Phillips: 写作——复习 & 编辑. Ahmed A. Moustafa: 写作——复习 &
编辑. Simon J. G. Lewis: 资金获取; 调查; 监督; 写作——复习
& 编辑. 詹姆斯·M. Shine: 概念化; 数据管理; 形式分析; 调查;
方法; 监督; 写作——复习 & 编辑.

资金信息

Claire O’Callaghan, National Health and Medical Research Council Neil Hamilton Fairley
Fellowship, 澳大利亚, 奖项ID: 1091310. Simon J. G. Lewis, National Health and Medical
Research Council of Australia, 奖项ID: 1037746. Simon J. G. Lewis, National Health and
Medical Research Council of Australia, 奖项ID: 1095127. Simon J. G. Lewis, 国家的
Health and Medical Research Council of Australia and the Australian Research Council, 的-
mentia Fellowship, AWARD ID: 1110414. 詹姆斯·M. Shine, National Health and Medical Re-
search Council Project Grant, 澳大利亚, 奖项ID: 1156536. 詹姆斯·M. Shine, The University
of Sydney Robinson Fellowship.

参考

Aarsland, D ., Bronnick, K., Ehrt, U。, De Deyn, 磷. P。, Tekin, S。, Emre,
M。, & Cummings, J. L. (2007). Neuropsychiatric symptoms in pa-
tients with Parkinson’s disease and dementia: Frequency, profile
and associated care giver stress. Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
surgery, and Psychiatry, 78(1), 36–42. https://doi.org/10.1136/
jnnp.2005.083113

Ala, 时间. A。, 哪个, K. H。, 宋, J. H。, & 弗雷, 瓦. H. (1997). Hallucina-
tions and signs of parkinsonism help distinguish patients with de-
mentia and cortical Lewy bodies from patients with Alzheimer’s
杂志
disease at presentation: A clinicopathological study.
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 62(1), 16–21.

Badre, D ., & 瓦格纳, A. D.

(2007). Left ventrolateral prefrontal
cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia,
45(13), 2883–2901. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2007.06.015

Bar, M。, Kassam, K. S。, Ghuman, A. S。, Boshyan,

Bar, 中号. (2009). The proactive braerin: Memory for predictions. Philo-
sophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
364(1521), 1235–1243. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0310
J。, Schmid,
A. M。, 戴尔, A. M。, . . . 哈尔格伦, 乙. (2006). Top-down fa-
cilitation of visual recognition. 国家会议录
Academy of Sciences, 103(2), 449–454. https://doi.org/10.1073/
普纳斯.0507062103

巴恩斯, J。, Boubert, L。, 哈里斯, J。, 李, A。, & 大卫, A. S. (2003). 关于-
ality monitoring and visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease.
Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 565–574.

巴恩斯, J。, & 大卫, A. (2001). Visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s
疾病: A review and phenomenological survey. Journal of Neu-
rology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 70(6), 727–733.

Bastiani, M。, Shah, 氮. J。, Goebel, R。, & Roebroeck, A. (2012). 胡-
man cortical connectome reconstruction from diffusion weighted
MRI: The effect of tractography algorithm. 神经影像, 62(3),
1732–1749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.002
Bertolero, 中号. A。, 杨, 乙. 时间. T。, & D’Esposito, 中号. (2015). The modu-
lar and integrative functional architecture of the human brain.
美国国家科学院院刊, 112(49),
E6798–E6807. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1510619112

Bertolero, 中号. A。, 杨, 乙. 时间. T。, & D’Esposito, 中号. (2017). The diverse
club. Nat Commun, 8(1), 1277. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-
017-01189-w

布莫尔, E., & 斯波恩斯, 氧. (2009). 复杂的大脑网络: 图形
结构和功能系统的理论分析. 自然
评论 神经科学, 10(3), 186.

卡蒙, L。, 巨人, X。, Meskaldji, D ., Thiran, J. P。, 斯波恩斯,
奥。, 做, K. Q., . . . 哈格曼, 磷. (2012). Mapping the human
connectome at multiple scales with diffusion spectrum MRI. Jour-
nal of Neuroscience Methods, 203(2), 386–397. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.09.031

张, L. C。, 琼斯, D. K., & 皮尔保利, C. (2005). RESTORE: Ro-
bust estimation of tensors by outlier rejection. Magnetic Reso-
nance in Medicine, 53(5), 1088–1095. https://doi.org/10.1002/
mrm.20426

网络神经科学

535

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

t

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

t

.

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

Chaumon, M。, Kveraga, K., Barrett, L. F。, & Bar, 中号. (2014). 维-
sual predictions in the orbitofrontal cortex rely on associa-
tive content. 大脑皮层, 24(11), 2899–2907. https://doi.org/
10.1093/cercor/bht146

Collerton, D ., Perry, E., & McKeith, 我. (2005). Why people see things
that are not there: A novel Perception and Attention Deficit model
for recurrent complex visual hallucinations. Behavioral and Brain
科学, 28(6), 737—757; discussion 757–794. https://doi.org/
10.1017/S0140525X05000130

科尔贝塔, M。, Patel, G。, & 舒尔曼, G. L. (2008). The reorienting sys-
tem of the human brain: From environvment to theory of mind. 新-
ron, 58(3), 306–324.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017
科尔贝塔, M。, & 舒尔曼, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed
and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. 自然评论
神经科学, 3(3), 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755

de Reus, 中号. A。, & van den Heuvel, 中号. 磷.

(2013). Estimating
false positives and negatives in brain networks. 神经影像, 70,
402–409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.066
Diederich, 氮. J。, Goetz, C. G。, Raman, R。, Pappert, 乙. J。, Leurgans,
S。, & Piery, V. (1998). Poor visual discrimination and visual hal-
lucinations in Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Neuropharmacology,
21(5), 289–295.

Diederich, 氮. J。, Goetz, C. G。, & Stebbins, G. 时间. (2005). Repeated
visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease as disturbed exter-
nal/internal perceptions: Focused review and a new integrative
模型. Movement Disorders, 20(2), 130–140. https://doi.org/10.
1002/mds.20308

恩格尔, A. K., 薯条, P。, & 歌手, 瓦. (2001). Dynamic predictions:
Oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Re-
views Neuroscience, 2, 704. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094565
Fénelon, G。, Mahieux, F。, Huon, R。, & Ziégler, 中号. (2000). Halluci-
nations in Parkinson’s disease: Prevalence, phenomenology and
risk factors. Brain, 123(4), 733–745.

Field, A.

(2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. 伦敦, 英国:

SAGE Publications.

弗莱彻, 磷. C。, & Frith, C. D. (2008). Perceiving is believing:
A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of
schizophrenia. 自然评论神经科学, 10, 48. https://
doi.org/10.1038/nrn2536

Folstein, 中号. F。, Robins, L. N。, & Helzer, J. 乙. (1983). The mini-mental
state examination. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40(7), 812.
福, C. H. Y。, Brammer, 中号. J。, Yágüez, L。, 艾伦, P。, Matsumoto,
K., Johns, L。, . . . van Haren, 氮. (2008). Increased superior
temporal activation associated with external misattributions of
self-generated speech in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research,
100(1), 361–363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.023
加拉格尔, D. A。, Parkkinen, L。, O’Sullivan, S. S。, Spratt, A。, Shah,
A。, Davey, C. C。, . . . Lees, A. J. (2011). Testing an aetiologi-
cal model of visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease. Brain,
134(11), 3299–3309.

Ganis, G。, 汤普森, 瓦. L。, & Kosslyn, S. 中号. (2004). Brain areas
underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An fMRI
学习. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(2), 226–241. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.02.012

Goetz, C. G. (2009). Scales to evaluate psychosis in Parkinson’s
疾病. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 15(Suppl. 3),
S38–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1353-8020(09)70777-1

Goetz, C. G。, Tilley, 乙. C。, Shaftman, S. R。, Stebbins, G. T。, Fahn, S。,
Martinez-Martin, P。, . . . Dodel, 右. (2008). Movement Disorder
Society–sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease
Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): Scale presentation and clinimetric
testing results. Movement Disorders, 23(15), 2129–2170.

Graham, J. M。, Grünewald, 右. A。, & Sagar, H. J. (1997). Hallucinosis
in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
surgery, and Psychiatry, 63(4), 434–440.

Guimerà, R。, & Nunes Amaral, L. A. (2005). Functional cartogra-
phy of complex metabolic networks. 自然, 433, 895. https://
doi.org/10.1038/nature03288

大厅, J. 中号. (2018). Topology: Structural network topology code for
“Alterations in structural network topology contribute to freez-
ing of gait in Parkinson’s disease,” GitHub. https://github.com/
juliemaehall/topology

大厅, J. M。, O’Callaghan, C。, 穆勒, A. J。, Ehgoetz Martens, K. A。,
(2019). Sup-
Phillips, J. R。, Moustafa, A. A。, . . . Shine, J. 中号.
porting information for “Changes in structural network topology
correlate with severity of hallucinatory behavior in Parkinson’s
disease.” Network Neuroscience, 3(2), 521–538. https://doi.org/
10.1162/netn_a_00078

大厅, J. M。, O’Callaghan, C。, Shine, J. M。, 穆勒, A. J。, Phillips, J. R。,
沃尔顿, C. C。, . . . Moustafa, A. A. (2016). Dysfunction in atten-
tional processing in patients with Parkinson’s disease and visual
hallucinations. Journal of Neural Transmission (维也纳), 123(5),
503–507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-016-1528-3

Hepp, D. H。, Foncke, 乙. 中号. J。, 贝伦德塞, H. W., Wassenaar, 时间. M。,
Olde Dubbelink, K. 时间. E., Groenewegen, H. J。, . . . Schoonheim,
中号. 中号.
(2017). Damaged fiber tracts of the nucleus basalis of
Meynert in Parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucina-
系统蒸发散. 科学报告, 7(1), 10112.

Hepp, D. H。, Foncke, 乙. 中号. J。, Olde Dubbelink, K. 时间. E., van de Berg,
瓦. D. J。, 贝伦德塞, H. W., & Schoonheim, 中号. 中号. (2017). Loss of
functional connectivity in patients with Parkinson disease and vi-
sual hallucinations. Radiology, 285(3), 896–903. https://doi.org/
10.1148/radiol.2017170438

Hoehn, 中号. M。, & Yahr, 中号. D. (1998). Parkinsonism: Onset, progres-

锡安, and mortality. Neurology, 50(2), 318–318.

Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N。, Ramírez-Ruiz, B., Junqué, C。, Martí, 中号. J。,
Valldeoriola, F。, Bargallo, N。, . . . Tolosa, 乙. (2010). Differen-
tial progression of brain atrophy in Parkinson’s disease with and
Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
without visual hallucinations.
surgery, and Psychiatry, 81(6), 650–657. https://doi.org/10.1136/
jnnp.2009.179655

Intaite, M。, Noreika, 五、, Soliunas, A。, & Falter, C. 中号. (2013). Interac-
tion of bottom-up and top-down processes in the perception of
ambiguous figures. 视觉研究, 89, 24–31. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.visres.2013.06.011

Keri, S。, Decety, J。, Roland, 磷. E., & Gulyas, 乙. (2004). Feature uncer-
tainty activates anterior cingulate cortex. 人脑图谱,
21(1), 26–33. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10150

李, J.-Y., Yoon, 乙. J。, 李, 瓦. W., Kim, 是. K., 李, J.-Y., & Jeon, 乙.
(2016). Lateral geniculate atrophy in Parkinson’s with visual hal-
lucination: A trans-synaptic degeneration? Movement Disorders,
31(4), 547–554. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.26533

水蛭, R。, & 锋利的, D. J. (2013). The role of the posterior cingulate

cortex in cognition and disease. Brain, 137(1), 12–32.

网络神经科学

536

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

/

/

t

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

Lenka, A。, Hegde, S。, Arumugham, S. S。, & 朋友, 磷. K. (2017). 图案
of cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease and
psychosis: A critical review. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders,
37, 11–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.12.025
Matsui, H。, Udaka, F。, Tamura, A。, Oda, M。, Kubori, T。, Nishinaka,
K., & Kameyama, 中号.
Impaired visual acuity as a risk
factor for visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease. 杂志
Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 19(1), 36–40. https://土井.
org/10.1177/0891988705284739

(2006).

Menon, 五、, & Uddin, L. 问.

(2010). Saliency, switching, 注意力
and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struc-
ture and Function, 214(5–6), 655–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s00429-010-0262-0

迈耶, T。, & 奥尔森, C. 右. (2011). Statistical learning of visual
transitions in monkey inferotemporal cortex. 诉讼程序
美国国家科学院, 108(48), 19401–19406. https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1112895108

Misic, B., 贝策尔, 右. F。, Nematzadeh, A。, Goni,

J。, Griffa, A。,
哈格曼, P。, . . . 斯波恩斯, 氧. (2015). Cooperative and competitive
spreading dynamics on the human connectome. 神经元, 86(6),
1518–1529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.035
森, S。, Crain, 乙. J。, Chacko, V. P。, & van Zijl, 磷. C. (1999). Three-
dimensional tracking of axonal projections in the brain by mag-
网络共振成像. Annals of Neurology, 45(2), 265–269.
穆勒, A. J。, O’Callaghan, C。, 沃尔顿, C. C。, Shine, J. M。, & Lewis,
S. J. (2017). Retrospective neuropsychological profile of patients
with Parkinson disease prior to developing visual hallucinations.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 30(2), 90–95.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988716686830

穆勒, A. J。, Shine, J. M。, Halliday, G. M。, & Lewis, S. J. (2014).
Visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: Theoretical models.
Movement Disorders, 29(13), 1591–1598.

Nichols, 时间. E., & Holmes, A. 磷.

(2002). Nonparametric permuta-
tion tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples.
人脑图谱, 15(1), 1–25.

O’Callaghan, C。, 大厅, J. M。, 托马西尼, A。, 穆勒, A. J。, Walpola,
我. C。, Moustafa, A. A。, . . . Lewis, S. J. G. (2017). Visual hallu-
cinations are characterized by impaired sensory evidence ac-
cumulation: Insights from hierarchical drift diffusion modeling
in Parkinson’s disease. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuro-
science and Neuroimaging, 2(8), 680–688. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.bpsc.2017.04.007

Panichello, 中号. F。, 张, 氧. S。, & Bar, 中号. (2012). Predictive feed-
back and conscious visual experience. 心理学前沿,
3, 620. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00620

权力, A. R。, Kelley, M。, & Corlett, 磷. 右. (2016). Hallucinations as
top-down effects on perception. Biological Psychiatry: 认知的
Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1(5), 393–400. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.04.003

Ramírez-Ruiz, B., Junqué, C。, Martí, 中号. J。, Valldeoriola, F。, & Tolosa,
乙. (2006). Neuropsychological deficits in Parkinson’s disease pa-
tients with visual hallucinations. Movement Disorders, 21(9),
1483–1487. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20965

Ramírez-Ruiz, B., Martí, 中号. J。, Tolosa, E., Falcón, C。, Bargalló, N。,
Valldeoriola, F。, & Junqué, C. (2008). Brain response to complex
visual stimuli in Parkinson’s patients with hallucinations: A func-

tional magnetic resonance imaging study. Movement Disorders,
23(16), 2335–2343.

鲁比诺夫, M。, & 斯波恩斯, 氧. (2010). 复杂的网络措施
大脑连接: 用途和解释. 神经影像, 52(3),
1059–1069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003
Sanchez-Ramos, J. R。, Ortollm, R。, & Paulson, G. 瓦. (1996). 维-
sual hallucinations associated with Parkinson disease. Archives
of Neurology, 53(23), 1265–1268.

Schrag, A.

(2004). Psychiatric aspects of Parkinson’s disease: 一个
update. Journal of Neurology, 251(7), 795–804. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s00415-004-0483-3

Shergill, S. S。, 沃克, Z。, & Le Katona, C. (1998). A preliminary in-
vestigation of laterality in Parkinson’s disease and susceptibility
to psychosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychia-
尝试, 65(4), 610–611.

Shine, J. M。, Halliday, G. H。, Carlos, M。, Naismith, S. L。, & Lewis,
S. J. (2012). Investigating visual misperceptions in Parkinson’s dis-
舒适: A novel behavioral paradigm. Movement Disorders, 27(4),
500–505.

Shine, J. M。, Halliday, G. M。, Gilat, M。, Matar, E., Bolitho, S. J。,
Carlos, M。, . . . Lewis, S. J. (2014). The role of dysfunctional at-
tentional control networks in visual misperceptions in Parkinson’s
疾病. 人脑图谱, 35(5), 2206–2219. https://土井.
org/10.1002/hbm.22321

Shine, J. M。, 米尔斯, J. 中号. Z。, Qiu, J。, O’Callaghan, C。, Terpening,
Z。, Halliday, G. M。, . . . Lewis, S. J. G. (2015). 验证

the psychosis and hallucinations questionnaire in non-
demented patients with Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disor-
ders Clinical Practice, 2(2), 175–181. https://doi.org/10.1002/
mdc3.12139

Shine,

J. M。, 穆勒, A.

J。, O’Callaghan, C。, Hornberger, M。,
Halliday, G. M。, & Lewis, S. J. (2015). Abnormal connectivity
between the default mode and the visual system underlies the
manifestation of visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: A
task-based fMRI study. NPJ Parkinson’s Disease, 1, 15003. https://
doi.org/10.1038/npjparkd.2015.3

Shine, J. M。, O’Callaghan, C。, Halliday, G. M。, & Lewis, S. J. (2014).
Tricks of the mind: Visual hallucinations as disorders of attention.
Progress in Neurobiology, 116, 58–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.pneurobio.2014.01.004

史密斯, 右. E., 图尔尼耶, J. D ., 卡拉曼特, F。, & 康纳利, A. (2012).
Anatomically constrained tractography:
Improved diffusion
MRI streamlines tractography through effective use of anatom-
ical information. 神经影像, 62(3), 1924–1938. https://土井.
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.005

Sonuga-Barke, 乙. J. S。, & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous
attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological
状况: A neurobiological hypothesis. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(7), 977–986. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.neubiorev.2007.02.005

Summerfield, C。, Egner, T。, Greene, M。, Koechlin, E., Mangels, J。, &
Hirsch, J. (2006). Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in
the frontal cortex. 科学, 314(5803), 1311–1314. https://土井.
org/10.1126/science.1132028

Summerfield, C。, & Koechlin, 乙. (2008). 神经表征
of prior information during perceptual inference. 神经元, 59(2),
336–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.021

网络神经科学

537

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

t

/

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

Changes in structural network topology are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease

Sun, Z。, 王, F。, Cui, L。, Breeze, J。, Du, X。, 王, X。, . . .
张, D.
(2003). Abnormal anterior cingulum in patients
with schizophrenia: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuro-
报告, 14(14), 1833–1836. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.
0000094529.75712.48

Tombaugh, 时间. 氮. (2004). Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data
stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy-
chology, 19(2), 203–214.

车工, 时间. H。, Cookson, J. C。, Wass, J. A。, Drury, 磷. L。, Price, 磷. A。,
& Besser, G. 中号. (1984). Psychotic reactions during treatment of
pituitary tumours with dopamine agonists. British Medical Jour-
纳尔 (Clinical Research Ed.), 289(6452), 1101–1103.

van den Heuvel, 中号. P。, & 斯波恩斯, 氧. (2011). Rich-club organ-
ization of the human connectome. 神经科学杂志,
31(44), 15775–15786.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-
11.2011

Verstraete, E., van den Heuvel, 中号. P。, Veldink, J. H。, Blanken, N。,
Mandl, 右. C。, Hulshoff Pol, H. E., & van den Berg, L. H. (2010).
Motor network degeneration in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: A
structural and functional connectivity study. PLoS ONE, 5(10),
e13664. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013664

Weissman, D. H。, 罗伯茨, K., Visscher, K., & Woldorff, 中号. (2006).

The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. 自然
神经科学, 9(7), 971.

Xia, M。, 王, J。, & 他, 是. (2013). BrainNet Viewer: A network
visualization tool for human brain connectomics. 公共图书馆一号,
8(7), e68910. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068910
Yao, N。, Shek-Kwan Chang, R。, 张, C。, Pang, S。, Lau, K. K.,
Suckling, J。, . . . Chua, S. 乙. (2014). The default mode network
is disrupted in Parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations.
人脑图谱, 35(11), 5658–5666.

杨, 乙. T。, 克里宁, F. M。, 墓, J。, 肥皂, 中号. R。, 军队,
D ., 霍林斯黑德, M。, . . . 巴克纳, 右. L. (2011). The organization
of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional
神经生理学杂志, 106(3), 1125–1165.
连接性.
https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011

扎莱斯基, A。, 假如, A。, & 布莫尔, 乙. 时间. (2010). 基于网络
statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. 神经影像,
53(4), 1197–1207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.
06.041

扎莱斯基, A。, 假如, A。, Cocchi, L。, Gollo, L. L。, van den Heuvel,
中号. P。, & Breakspear, 中号. (2016). Connectome sensitivity or speci-
ficity: Which is more important? 神经影像, 142, 407–420.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.035

D

w
n

A
d
e
d

F
r


H

t
t

p

:
/
/

d

r
e
C
t
.


t
.

t

/

/

e
d

n
e
n
A
r
t

C
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

/

3
2
5
2
1
1
0
9
2
7
0
4
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
0
7
8
p
d

.

t

F


y
G

e
s
t

t


n
0
8
S
e
p
e


e
r
2
0
2
3

网络神经科学

538研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象
研究形象

下载pdf