方法
From correlation to communication:
Disentangling hidden factors from
functional connectivity changes
Yuhua Yu1
, Caterina Gratton1,2,3, and Derek M. Smith1,4
1心理学系, Northwestern University, Evanston, 伊尔, 美国
2神经内科, Northwestern University, Evanston, 伊尔, 美国
3心理学系, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, 美国
4神经内科, Division of Cognitive Neurology/ Neuropsychology,
The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 巴尔的摩, 医学博士, 美国
开放访问
杂志
关键词: Functional connectivity, Neural correlations, Neural coupling, BOLD variance
抽象的
While correlations in the BOLD fMRI signal are widely used to capture functional connectivity
(FC) and its changes across contexts, its interpretation is often ambiguous. The entanglement of
multiple factors including local coupling of two neighbors and nonlocal inputs from the rest
of the network (affecting one or both regions) limits the scope of the conclusions that can
be drawn from correlation measures alone. Here we present a method of estimating the
contribution of nonlocal network input to FC changes across different contexts. To disentangle
the effect of task-induced coupling change from the network input change, we propose a new
metric, “communication change,” utilizing BOLD signal correlation and variance. 与一个
combination of simulation and empirical analysis, we demonstrate that (1) input from the rest
of the network accounts for a moderate but significant amount of task-induced FC change and
(2) the proposed “communication change” is a promising candidate for tracking the local
coupling in task context-induced change. 此外, when compared to FC change across
three different tasks, communication change can better discriminate specific task types. Taken
一起, this novel index of local coupling may have many applications in improving our
understanding of local and widespread interactions across large-scale functional networks.
介绍
The brain is a dynamic system that integrates information in order to fulfill its functions. 这
system is characterized by both short- and long-range connections between regions that are
organized into large-scale brain networks (Fox et al., 2005; Power et al., 2011; Yeo et al.,
2011). The coupling in activity between different brain regions is referred to as functional
连接性 (FC). FC is typically measured by indices of statistical dependence, such as cor-
relation coefficients, 连贯性, or transfer entropy (Baccalá & Sameshima, 2001; Patel et al.,
2006; Vicente et al., 2011). In human functional magnetic resonance imaging (功能磁共振成像), 这
Pearson correlation derived from blood oxygen level–dependent (大胆的) time series is the
dominant means of quantifying FC and drawing inferences on neural coupling (Biswal
等人。, 1995; David et al., 2004; Fiecas et al., 2013; Mahadevan et al., 2021).
引文: 于, Y。, Gratton, C。, & 史密斯,
D. 中号. (2023). From correlation to
沟通: Disentangling hidden
factors from functional connectivity
变化. 网络神经科学, 7(2),
411–430. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a
_00290
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00290
支持信息:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00290
已收到: 18 行进 2022
公认: 2 十一月 2022
利益争夺: 作者有
声明不存在竞争利益
存在.
通讯作者:
Yuhua Yu
yyu@u.northwestern.edu
处理编辑器:
Richard Betzel
版权: © 2022
麻省理工学院
在知识共享下发布
归因 4.0 国际的
(抄送 4.0) 执照
麻省理工学院出版社
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
/
/
t
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
Local coupling:
The causal interaction between two
neural regions that can be modulated
by influences such as the synaptic
strength or entrained oscillation.
Shared input:
Neural signals that simultaneously
affect two coupled network regions.
Unshared input:
Neural signals that are idiosyncratic
to one of the coupled network
地区.
While functional connectivity patterns are largely stable over time within an individual
(Gordon et al., 2017; Gratton et al., 2018; Laumann et al., 2015), there is evidence that task
context induces subtle changes in FC (Arbabshirani et al., 2013; Cole et al., 2014; 冈萨雷斯-
Castillo & Bandettini, 2018; Gratton et al., 2016, 2018; Krienen et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2021).
Changes in FC between rest and task can help us understand how functional networks support
human cognition (Allen et al., 2014; 张 & Glover, 2010; Kietzmann et al., 2019), but its
interpretation remains controversial (Hutchison et al., 2013; 劳曼 & 斯奈德, 2021). 一些
authors have raised concerns about interpreting change in correlation as change in neural cou-
pling (Behseta et al., 2009; 弗里斯顿, 2011; Laumann et al., 2017). 例如, Duff et al.
(2018) suggested that many changes in BOLD correlation can be attributable to “additive sig-
nal change,” where a separate signal is added to one or both neighboring nodes. Additive
signal changes include changes in noise levels or the amplitude of a common signal driving
correlation between two nodes. By making an inference from the relationship between
changes in variances and covariances, the authors distinguished additive signal changes from
changes involving more complex combination of increases and decreases in the strength of
existing signal components. A conjunction analysis incorporating variances and covariances
was also proposed by Cole et al. (2016) to investigate the shared and unshared signals in
BOLD data.
In the current project, we distinguish two types of factors driving the change in BOLD FC
(数字 1). The first type involves a change in the local coupling between two nodes. 当地的
coupling can be altered by increased synaptic strength (例如, Yao et al., 2007; Zucker &
Regehr, 2002) in the cortical circuit. Local coupling can also be instantiated when the oscil-
lation in a neuronal group is entrained by the oscillation from another group (薯条, 2005).
Local coupling is closely related to the causal influence1 (弗里斯顿, 2011) between two regions,
regardless of anatomical distance.
The second type of factor involves input from the rest of the network that modulates the FC
of an edge even in the absence of causal interaction between two neighbors. We differentiate
two subtypes here. The first subtype, shared input, is comprised of neural signals that simul-
taneously affect both neighbors. 例如, increases in the correlation between visual
regions during visual stimulation could be explained by shared activity induced by the same
刺激. There is also considerable evidence showing that the ascending arousal system plays
a role in modulating functional network topography globally (Guedj et al., 2017; Shafiei et al.,
2019; Shine, 2019; Shine et al., 2016, 2018; Turchi et al., 2018). Projections from critical neu-
romodulatory sources like the dorsal raphe, locus coeruleus, ventral tegmental area, pedun-
culopontine nucleus, and the cholinergic basal forebrain are widespread, thus making these
regions potential sources of shared input (Aston-Jones & 科恩, 2005; Mena-Segovia &
Bolam, 2017; Morales & Margolis, 2017; Selden et al., 1998; Shine, 2019). The second sub-
类型, unshared input, involves neural signals that are idiosyncratic to each node of a pair. 为了
例子, signals from different modalities (例如, visual and auditory) can project to separate,
but coupled regions in the higher order cortex (Sepulcre et al., 2012). Unshared input can also
include local neural noises (Daunizeau et al., 2012) or measurement errors. Regardless of the
机制, the two subtypes of input are exogenous to the causal interaction between two
neighbors, and we collectively refer to them as (nonlocal) network input.
While neither network input, nor local coupling can be observed directly with fMRI, 我们
aim to estimate their contributions to task-induced FC change by incorporating information
regarding the temporal variance of the BOLD signal. 具体来说, we focus on the FC changes
1 虽然, in the present work, we do not attempt to address the directionality in the local coupling.
网络神经科学
412
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
数字 1. A schematic representation. Different factors can drive functional connectivity across contexts. When the BOLD correlation
between two regions increases, it can stem from stronger coupling between the two regions (direct synaptic input, ETC。), higher shared input
from the rest of the network, or decreased unshared input that is idiosyncratic to one of the regions.
Communication change:
The part of task-induced functional
connectivity change that cannot be
attributed to the change of shared
and unshared input.
Partial correlation:
The correlation between a pair of
time series after the portion of their
variance explained by all other
observed time series is removed.
under “task state” that occur over a block (multiple minute period). Like correlation, 大胆的
signal variance can change over contexts. 例如, resting-state BOLD data has been
reported to have a higher variance than task BOLD data (Ito et al., 2020). 然而, for a pair
of neighbors, the shared and unshared input variance can have opposing effects. Shared sig-
nals are expected to increase the correlation between the pair while unshared signals to atten-
uate the correlation. We aim to quantify this relationship in both simulation and real data by
estimating the network input variance from BOLD signals.
Our simulation not only highlights the multiple factors modulating FC but also provides
insight on a new measure, communication change, that may be used to track local coupling
more closely than FC. Communication change utilizes a learned relationship between FC and
input variances during a baseline (or resting state) to mitigate the impact of input fluctuation on
task-induced FC change. 然而, unlike correlation or partial correlation (见下文) 那
can be used as a “standalone” metric for functional connectivity, communication change is
a method to disentangle factors underlying FC changes across task contexts.2
Previous work has used partial correlation (Marrelec et al., 2006) to disentangle the impact
of local coupling from the mutual dependencies on other brain regions. Partial correlation cal-
culates the correlation between a pair of time series after the portion of their variance
explained by all other observed time series is removed. 因此, partial correlation can capture
a similar concept as communication change, but with a different modeling and computational
方法. To evaluate how partial correlation is affected by the changes in local coupling and
network input, we analyzed it in the same simulation framework.
2 In the following text, we may omit the word change when we refer to communication change for
方便.
网络神经科学
413
From correlation to communication
When examining empirical data, we found that network input account for a significant but
moderate amount of task-induced FC change. To estimate FC and input variances during task
表现, we used BOLD signal residuals after removing evoked task effects following the
“background connectivity” approach (Al-Aidroos et al., 2012; Fair et al., 2007). 根据
the task context, communication change sometimes suggests local coupling changes in the
opposite direction as FC change would indicate. To gain insights into the informational con-
tent of communication change, we investigated whether it can reliably discriminate task type.
结果
FC Is Sensitive to Both Local Coupling and Network Input, While Communication Tracks Local Coupling
More Closely
We simulated two interacting neural regions with a two-dimensional Wilson–Cowan dynamic
模型 (Wilson & 考恩, 1972.) to illustrate that (1) FC change is sensitive to local coupling as
well as to network input and (2) the impact of network input is dampened in the communi-
cation change measure where the contribution from the rest of the network, proxied by shared
and unshared input variance (see section Communication Change in Methods), is removed
from FC changes. BOLD signals were computed from simulated neural activity via the
Balloon–Windkessel model (Friston et al., 2003). FC and communication changes were
derived from the simulated BOLD series. Throughout, we refer to the temporal correlation
between nodes in this model as “FC,” in equivalence to BOLD correlation.3
With all parameters (including the local coupling weight and shared/unshared neural signal
variances) fixed, we computed baseline FC, baseline input variance for each node, and regres-
sion coefficients of FC with respect to the input variance. These values will be used to compute
communication change in the following scenarios: (1) after varying local coupling strength, (2)
after varying shared neural signal variance, 和 (3) after varying unshared neural signal
方差.
正如预期的那样, simulated FC is sensitive to both changes in local coupling and changes in
input signal variance, confounding the interpretation of FC measures. Both stronger local cou-
pling (图2A) and amplified shared signal (图2B) lead to higher FC, while amplified
unshared signals (Figure 2C) lowered the FC. In comparison, communication change shows a
dampened relationship to shared and unshared signal variance while remaining sensitive to
the local coupling (Figure 2A–C). 例如, with the current simulation setup, both FC and
communication were equally sensitive to local coupling. 然而, FC was 5 times more sen-
sitive to shared signal (the slope in Figure 2B) 和 2.5 times more sensitive to unshared signal
(the slope in Figure 2C) relative to communication.
Given that partial correlation is often used to account for the mutual dependence on other
brain regions, we computed partial correlation in the same three scenarios (参见方法). 我们
问: Does partial correlation have less dependence on input signal variance than FC? 在里面
current simulation framework, partial correlation had similar sensitivity as FC with respect to
changes in local coupling and input signals (Figure 2D–F). 所以, it cannot discriminate
among those factors.
This analysis suggests that communication, compared to correlation and partial correlation,
more closely tracks changes in local coupling relative to changes in input variance. Commu-
nication diminishes the impact of network input by using the statistical relationship between
3 虽然, as discussed in the Introduction, correlation is only one of many possible ways to estimate func-
tional connectivity.
网络神经科学
414
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
t
/
/
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
.
t
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
/
/
t
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
数字 2. The sensitivity of FC, 沟通. and partial correlation with respect to model parameters. (A) Both FC and communication
increase with the coupling strength ( 瓦). (乙) FC (but not communication) increases sharply with increases in shared signal variance. (C) FC
decreases with unshared signal variance while the effect is dampened for communication. (D–F) FC and partial correlation in the same sim-
ulated scenarios display similar behavior to one another.
FC and variance in a baseline state. 尤其, this is an imperfect proxy because input signals
are unobservable in real data. 所以, communication is not guaranteed to have zero sen-
sitivity to shared and unshared input variances, but their impact is considerably dampened as
observed from the simulation. This property allows us to infer the change in local coupling
from real data when neither the coupling nor input signals are directly observable.
FC Is Related to Shared and Unshared Input Variance in Real fMRI Data
To compare communication and FC in empirical data, we used Midnight Scan Club (MSC)
dataset (Gordon et al., 2017), a precision fMRI dataset with 10 sessions of fMRI rest and task
data from nine participants. See Methods for BOLD data acquisition, preprocessing, FC, 和
variance calculation for resting-state and task data. We computed BOLD FC, shared input var-
iance, and unshared input variance for each of the 10 resting-state scans. We fit a linear model
for each edge with the FC as the dependent variable and shared/unshared input variance as
independent variables. 正如预期的那样, FC increased with shared input variance and decreased
with unshared input variance in the MSC data. 数字 3 shows the distribution of the regression
coefficients of all participants. Network input fluctuations jointly explained 24.7 ± 0.1% (adj.
R2) of the FC variability across resting-state sessions. Results from individual participants all
show a similar pattern and are included in Supporting Information Figure S1, indicating robust-
ness of the results.
因此, the empirical relationship between FC and network input is consistent with what we
observed in the simulation: FC increases with the shared input variance and decreases with
unshared variance idiosyncratic to one of the nodes. We next use the regression coefficients
网络神经科学
415
From correlation to communication
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
/
/
t
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
数字 3. Distribution of regression coefficients of FC against the unshared and unshared input
variance in the Midnight Scan Club. The regression is run for each edge across resting-state scans
for each participant. Error bars show a standard error across edges. Data from all participants and
edges are plotted here. Results for individual participants are shown in Supporting Information
Figure S1.
learned from resting state to estimate the contribution of input variance in FC during task-
induced contexts.
Task-Induced FC Changes Are Related to Changes in Network Inputs
下一个, we asked whether task-induced FC change can be predicted by network inputs, esti-
mated from variance changes. We computed the predicted FC changes during task perfor-
mance from the observed input variance change under (方程 1). There was a significant
correlation between the predicted and actual FC changes across tasks for the majority of the
边缘 (见图 4 for the aggregated group distribution of the correlations, mean is 0.152 ±
0.001; individual participants are shown in Figure S2). This indicates that input fluctuation
explains a small but significant proportion of task-evoked FC changes. The prediction error,
那是, communication change in our definition, is the part of the FC change not explained by
input fluctuation.
predicted ΔFC ¼ β0 * Δshared input variance þ β1 * Δunshared input variance1
þ β2 * Δunshared input variance2
(1)
where β’s are computed from resting-state data (数字 3) and input variance changes are mea-
sured from the comparison in variances between task and rest.
Communication Change Differs From FC Change and Shows Improved Sensitivity to Task Information
Since the previous simulations suggest that communication tracks local coupling while damp-
ening the impact of input fluctuation, we next directly compare FC and communication mea-
sures in real data. We compared the task-induced changes in each measure, grouping edges
网络神经科学
416
From correlation to communication
数字 4. Ability of changes in input variance to predict FC changes during tasks in real fMRI data
from the Midnight Scan Club. For each edge, accuracy was measured using the correlation coeffi-
cients between actual FC changes and those predicted by task-induced input variance changes
across 30 task conditions (10 sessions for categorization tasks, 10 for motor task, 和 10 for mixed
任务). Data plotted here is based on the group average; individual participant relationships are
shown in Figure S2.
according to a widely used large-scale network designation (Gordon et al., 2016). 康尼尼-
cation and FC changes during tasks differed in many networks and these differences were
modulated by the task type (数字 5). Sometimes communication was higher magnitude than
FC, and other times it was lower; on occasion it even showed relationships in the reverse
方向. 例如, when participants performed the categorization task4 (Figure 5A),
FC decreased on average within the ventral attention network, while communication (计算-
lated from FC after removing the contribution from input variance, 参见方法, 方程 2)
增加. This highlights the danger of interpreting an FC decrease directly as reduced inter-
regional interaction.
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
/
/
t
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
Intraclass correlation coefficient:
The percentage of variance
accounted for by the group
difference out of the overall
population variance.
To probe the informational content in the communication measure, we ask to what extent it
can discriminate among tasks with distinct task demands. 为此, we computed the intra-
class correlation coefficient (ICC; 参见方法) between task types for each edge across all
scan sessions (4 task types including the resting state with 10 sessions each). ICC measures
the percentage of variance accounted for by task types among all changes observed for the
same edge (Bartko, 1976; 麦格劳 & 黄, 1996). If communication change contains task
信息, ICC5 is expected to be closer to 1 (rather than to 0).
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
Across all edges and all participants, the average communication ICC = 0.699 ± 0.001,
和 71% of the edges showing a a p value less than 0.05 (F test), indicating that most edges
contain reliable information about task type (数字 6). 用于比较, we computed the ICC
for FC changes using the same methodology. Similar to communication, FC changes reliably
represent task information (ICC = 0.462 ± 0.001; 数字 6). 然而, the ICC based on the
4 This task was called “memory task” in previous publications using the MSC dataset. We label it as “catego-
rization” here to better reflect the nature of the task; 参见方法.
5 Although ICC is theoretically bounded by 0 和 1, in practice it is possible to obtain negative, unbounded
values due to sampling issues (Bartko, 1976). 例如, when the sample size is small, the estimated
within-group variance may exceed the variance of the whole population. It is often recommended that neg-
ative values be cut off. Although the threshold can affect the ICC statistics, our conclusion does not change
whether or not we apply the cutoffs.
网络神经科学
417
From correlation to communication
我
D
哦
w
n
哦
A
d
e
d
F
r
哦
米
H
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
我
r
e
C
t
.
米
我
t
.
t
/
/
e
d
你
n
e
n
A
r
t
我
C
e
–
p
d
我
F
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
A
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
F
乙
y
G
你
e
s
t
t
哦
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
米
乙
e
r
2
0
2
3
数字 5. Group averaged, task-induced changes in FC and communication for all edges grouped by network affiliation during (A) catego-
rization task, (乙) motor task, 和 (C) mixed task (dot coherence and semantic) relative to rest in the MSC. Error bar indicates the standard error
across edges. Data from individual participants are shown in Figure S3. DMN = default mode; Vis = Visual; FP = fronto-parietal; DAN = dorsal
注意力; VAN = ventral attention; Sal = salience; CO = cinguloopercular; SM-Dors = dorsal somatomotor; SM-Lat = lateral somatomotor;
Aud = auditory; PMN = parietal memory; RSP = retrosplenial; between = between-network connectivity.
网络神经科学
418
From correlation to communication
数字 6. Distribution of intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC), across all edges and participants,
for discriminating among task conditions. The ICC between task types was computed for each edge
across 40 scans (10 each for rest, 分类, mixed, and motor). Data from individual partic-
ipants are shown in Figure S4.
communication measure is significantly higher than the ICC of FC (z = 140, p < .00001), sug-
gesting that communication change increases the discrimination of task type.
The same pattern was observed consistently for all participants (Figure S4). The difference
between FC and communication in terms of task discrimination is not solely driven by the
distinction between the resting state and any task state. ICC among the three task states exhibits
a similar pattern (although the difference is narrower, z = 77, p < .00001; see Figure S5) indi-
cating that communication change contains more task-specific information relative to FC.
In summary, communication changes during tasks exhibit significant differences from FC in
various networks of the brain. Importantly, communication contains more informational con-
tent to discriminate specific task types, compared to FC.
DISCUSSION
While BOLD correlation is widely used to capture both static functional connectivity and its
changes across contexts, the interpretation of this measure is often ambiguous. The entangle-
ment of multiple latent factors including local coupling of two neighbors and network inputs
from the rest of the network limits the scope of the conclusions that can be drawn from cor-
relation measures alone. Here, we developed a method to compute the shared and unshared
input variance to an edge and quantify their impact on task-induced FC changes. We further
proposed a metric, termed “communication change,” that dampens the impact of input signals
and thereby provides a promising candidate for more closely tracking local coupling changes.
We first analyzed local and network impact on FC by conducting a simulation based on a
well-established model of neural dynamics (Wilson & Cowan, 1972). The model suggests that
FC can be driven by multiple factors. Increased local coupling as well as increased variance of
shared inputs result in higher FC, while increases in the variance of unshared (idiosyncratic)
input has the opposite effect. Although the conclusion seems intuitive, explicit modeling of the
Network Neuroscience
419
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
.
t
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
three latent factors leads to a novel metric, communication, that dampens the impact of both
shared and unshared inputs using only observable, statistical relationships. Our simulation
shows that communication change is more likely to track local coupling.
Interestingly, partial correlation, a metric often used to disentangle the impact of local cou-
pling from the mutual dependencies on other brain regions (Marrelec et al., 2006; Smith,
2012), shows similar sensitivity as FC change in the current simulation framework. Partial cor-
relation removes the contemporaneous signal dependence by using a linear residualization.
However, as the BOLD signal exhibits temporal nonlinearity, the transformation from neural
activity to BOLD signal (via Ballon–Windkessel model) is state and path dependent (Buxton
et al., 2004; Friston et al., 2000). It is possible, therefore, that the correlation between residua-
lized signals is still affected by the network input through their “history.” We are not aware of
other simulative studies illustrating the (lack of ) efficacy of partial correlation, except for Cole
et al. (2016), where the authors also reported that partial correlation retains limitations of the
FC measure. In comparison, to compute communication change, we first aggregated inputs
from the rest of the network into three terms, shared variance and two unshared variances
idiosyncratic to each node. Communication then removes the input contribution via a
“learned” statistical relationship between FC and variance at baseline (here operationalized
as the average resting-state signal). Therefore, the method makes few assumptions about the
underlying structure of the input signals.
With empirical data, we computed the sensitivity of FC with respect to shared and unshared
input during resting-state scans. As expected, FC in real BOLD data increased with the vari-
ance of shared input and decreased with unshared input. We then analyzed task-induced FC
changes using the coefficients learned from the resting state. Input fluctuation explained a sig-
nificant, but moderate, amount of FC changes during task performance. After removing the
input related contributions, the remaining FC change, that is, communication change, often
had the opposite sign of the original FC change in various tasks and brain networks. This high-
lights the ambiguity of interpreting task-induced FC change by itself. If the correlation of an
edge increases, for example, it can be driven by an increase in shared input from the rest of the
network, a decrease in an idiosyncratic input affecting only one of the nodes, an enhanced
neural coupling between the nodes, or any combination of the above factors.
Although we cannot directly observe how communication differentiates mechanisms
behind FC changes in empirical data, evidence suggests that it shows better discrimination
across the task types relative to the standard FC measure. Both simulation and empirical anal-
ysis suggest that communication may be an improved measure (from FC) for tracking changes
in causal interactions between regions across task contexts. Such changes can occur at the
synaptic level due to short-term plasticity (Krystal et al., 2003; Yao et al., 2007; Zucker &
Regehr, 2002), such as an elevated dopamine tone in the same cortical circuit (Vijayraghavan
et al., 2007) and via increased coherence or phase locking (Fries, 2005).
While FC is strongly related to shared and unshared network inputs, communication shows
a diminished relationship to these latent signals. Shared input can arise from signals with wide-
spread influence stemming from subcortical projections (Raut et al., 2021; Shine, 2019), while
unshared (idiosyncratic) input can come from, for example, unique inputs from different
modalities (visual or somatosensory) that only project to selected regions. The communication
metric dampens the impact of network input changes using a statistical relationship and
remains agnostic to the underlying biophysiological mechanism. This allows contextual fac-
tors to guide the interpretation of the difference between FC and communication across a vari-
ety of states.
Network Neuroscience
420
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
.
t
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
Using BOLD Variance to Augment the Interpretation of FC
Other researchers have also explored the idea of incorporating variance (or covariance) to
disambiguate the interpretation of FC (Duff et al., 2018; Cole et al., 2016). For example, Duff
et al. (2018) combined correlation and variance to classify FC changes into cases attributable
to “additive signal change” (a type of “shared or unshared input”) and changes arising from
modifying existing signals. While the current analysis echoes the importance of augmenting
FC with variance/covariance, we propose a framework to quantitatively decompose FC
changes into contributions from different underlying mechanisms.
Communication shows higher intraclass correlation across tasks compared to FC, suggest-
ing that the proposed measure better discriminates task types. This suggests that local coupling
is modulated by specific task demands, while overall signal variance contains less task-specific
information. Previous work has shown a simultaneous task-induced reduction in FC and var-
iance (Ito et al., 2020). It is possible that task performance, relative to the resting state, reduces
synchronized, spontaneous activity that affects a large region of the brain and leads to an FC
reduction in a pattern that is similar across task types. By extracting the “excess” FC change
after accounting for the variance-related effects, we can gain more insight into how specific
task demand modulates the brain network.
Furthermore, the current framework provides a way to decompose exogenous network
inputs into shared and unshared variance. One powerful approach may be to use several of
these metrics in combination. For example, an increase in communication implies stronger
local coupling. If a decrease in FC is simultaneously observed, we can ask whether the
discrepancy is due to a reduction of shared signal or an increase of idiosyncratic inputs by
examining changes in shared and unshared variance simultaneously. Therefore, examining
communication together with FC may allow researchers to draw inferences regarding how a
particular region adjusts its interactions with its neighbors across a range of different contexts,
providing an improved handle on whether local or shared inputs are at work in any given
scenario.
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
.
t
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Limitations and Future Directions
One pitfall of our approach is that the interpretation of our simulation results is dependent on
the validity of our computational model. The communication change metric was inspired by a
simplified neural dynamic simulation. Although Wilson–Cowan dynamics have been widely
used and have had a major impact on computational neuroscience, it is a simplification of the
underlying biological process (Chow & Karimipanah, 2020). Many extensions and generaliza-
tions have been proposed since its initial introduction in 1972. To highlight the basic interac-
tion between several statistical measures of interest, we adopted the simplest form of the model
in a two interacting nodes setup. By doing this, we imposed the assumption that any effect
from the rest of the system could be represented by Gaussian signals. Furthermore, we fixed
the mean of the input signals and focus on the impact of their variance/amplitude. Because the
neural dynamics involve sigmoid activation functions, the shift in the average level of the input
signal would have a nonlinear impact on the resulting fMRI BOLD signals (Ito et al., 2020).
Future work will explore how the average activity level interacts with the relationship between
FC and input variances discussed in this article.
When it comes to empirical data, a key premise of our analysis is based on evidence that
fMRI signals are indirectly related to underlying neural population activity. Such a view is
widely supported in the literature (Logothetis et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2016), but certain MRI
artifacts can ambiguate the results (Friston et al., 1996). We employed multiple artifact
Network Neuroscience
421
From correlation to communication
mitigation measures (scrubbing, motion regression, global signal regression, white matter sig-
nal regression, and ventricle signal regression) to minimize the impact of artifactual factors.
Moreover, the BOLD signal analyses utilized nine highly sampled subjects from the MSC
dataset (Gordon et al., 2016). This is a small sample, but there is evidence that results from
MSC data extend to other independent datasets (Gordon et al., 2018). Importantly, the single
subject results presented here are consistently observed in all individuals, demonstrating a cer-
tain degree of robustness. However, communication change should be examined in other
datasets in future work, especially with task conditions that impose varying degrees of cogni-
tive demand and a larger number of subjects.
There are unanswered questions that are beyond the scope of the current investigation that
pertains to communication change. Task-induced communication increases in some regions
(e.g., visual network, dorsal attention network), and the magnitude of the changes seems to be
modulated by the type of task. In addition, the spatial pattern of communication exhibits sig-
nificant individual differences (Figure S3), consistent with findings from previous work (Gratton
et al., 2018; Porter et al., 2023). An intriguing question for future research is how differential
communication is related to task performance.
Finally, task discrimination is only one possible venue to explore the informational content
in communication and its advantage over FC. Research directly modifying potentially wide-
spread signals such as arousal (shared input), signals that might be expected to be more local
such as selective stimulus-response features (unshared input), or manipulation at the local syn-
aptic level (coupling) may help to validate the interpretation of communication change.
Conclusions
FC, computed as a correlation between paired BOLD signals, is not only determined by local
coupling between pairs of regions but also by inputs from the rest of the network, among other
factors. Utilizing the statistical relationship between FC and input variance, we developed a
method to extract a component (communication change) from FC change with a dampened
influence of network input. Examining changes in communication, along with FC, allows us to
draw inferences regarding how the interaction between a particular region with its neighbors
changes across different contexts.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
We used a combination of numerical simulations and empirical analysis to examine the effects
of local interactions and input fluctuations on BOLD correlation and variance. These results led
us to develop a new measure, communication change, that more closely tracks the change in
coupling between two neural regions compared to the standard FC measure (BOLD correlation)
across task contexts. Below, we first describe our modeling approach, then the derivation of the
communication metric, and finally we include a description of a dataset and analyses used to
empirically examine differences in communication change and correlation.
Compute Communication Change in Both Simulated and Real Data
We developed a novel metric to better capture the interregional coupling shift in task-induced
FC change, which we termed communication change. To compute the communication
change of an edge, we first need to estimate the shared and unshared network input variance.
Shared/unshared input variance is computed from the variance of the BOLD signals from
common and idiosyncratic neighbors. Specifically, assume node1 and node2 are the nodes of
Network Neuroscience
422
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
the edge under consideration. Ns is the set of neighboring nodes that connect to both node1
and node2. N1 is the set of nodes that only connect to node1, and N2 is the set of nodes that
only connect to node2. That is, Ns, N1, and N2 are mutually exclusive. Shared input variance
is the total variance of the BOLD signals from Ns, computed by the trace of the covariance
matrix. We residualized all the signals in N1 against all signals in Ns. Unshared input variance1
is the total variance of the residualized signals in N1. Likewise, unshared input variance2 is the
total variance of the residualized signals in N2 against Ns.
Communication change is computed with the following equation:
ΔComm ¼ ΔFC − β0 * Δshared input variance − β1 * Δunshared input variance1
− β2 * Δunshared input variance2
(2)
Where all the changes are based on comparisons of task-rest, or for the simulation, a scenario
with parameter shock—baseline.
The coefficients β0, β1, β2 are then computed from resting state (or the baseline simulation
scenario) using linear regression:
FC ¼ β0 * shared input variance þ β1 * unshared input variance1
þ β2 * unshared input variance2 þ constant þ noise
(3)
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
All edges and neighbors in empirical data (including rest and task) are defined by 5% spar-
sity based on the average resting-state correlation. We reevaluate the key findings using 10%
sparsity and reached similar conclusions (Figure S6).
Numerical Simulation of BOLD Signals From Two Interacting Neural Regions
We examined the effects of local coupling and network inputs on BOLD correlation with a
numerical simulation. A two-dimensional (two node) Wilson–Cowan type (Wilson & Cowan,
1972) dynamic was used to describe the activity of two interacting neural regions:
τ dx1 tð Þ
dt
τ dx2 tð Þ
dt
¼ −x1 tð Þ þ f w11x1 tð Þ þ w12x2 tð Þ þ s tð Þ þ I1 tð Þ
ð
Þ
;
¼ −x2 tð Þ þ f w21x1 tð Þ þ w22x2 tð Þ þ s tð Þ þ I2 tð Þ
ð
Þ
where xi(t) represents the excitatory level of a neural group.
(cid:1)
(cid:3)
The matrix W ¼ w11 w12
w21 w22
describes the information exchange within and between
regions. In particular, the off-diagonal terms, w21, w12, determine the strength of interregion
coupling. For the baseline scenario, the matrix was set to W ¼
: The diagonal entries
(cid:1)
(cid:3)
2 1
1 2
t
/
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
were fixed throughout the simulation. The parameter values (and those in the following) are
adapted from Ito et al. (2020) and adjusted to obtain a realistic baseline correlation.
Shared input s(t) is a Gaussian signal with shared signal variance Vsh, which was set to Vsh =
0.25 in the baseline scenario. Vsh indexes the magnitude of the common exogenous input from
the rest of the brain network to both regions.
I1(t), I2(t) are two independent Gaussian signals representing idiosyncratic signals with
unshared signal variance parameter Vun, which was set to 0.35 in the baseline. Vun indexes
the magnitude of the idiosyncratic input to one of the neighbors.
f is the sigmoid input-output activation defined as f (x) = 1/(1 + e^(−k * x)), where k is fixed
at 0.5 throughout the simulation.
Network Neuroscience
423
From correlation to communication
Neural activities simulated from the above equations were transformed to BOLD signals
using the Balloon–Windkessel model (Friston et al., 2003). The transformation assumes
normalized deoxyhemoglobin content, normalized blood inflow, resting oxygen extraction
fraction, and normalized blood volume. State equations and parameters were taken from pre-
vious work (Friston et al., 2003). Correlation (FC), partial correlation, and communication
change were then computed on the simulated BOLD signals as specified below. Each simu-
lation (1 run) generated a pair of BOLD time series with 2,000 steps each.
For the baseline simulation, we ran the scenario with fixed parameters 500 times (500 runs).
We obtained the average FC, average shared/unshared input variance, and the regression coef-
ficients as in Equation 3. The baseline simulation mimics the resting state as in real data.
Next, we computed the sensitivity of correlation, communication, and partial correlation
with respect to the input signal variance and local coupling parameters. To this end, we carried
out the same simulation as above, but now independently varied the shared signal variance
Vsh, unshared signal variance Vun, and the coupling parameter w12 (and symmetrically w21).
We used inputs for Vsh ranging from 0.15 to 0.35, Vun ranging from 0.25 to 0.45, and w12 from
0.4 to 1.6 (centered around baseline 1). The range was chosen such that the parameters had
comparable impact on FC. For each scenario, the model is run 500 times to obtain the mean
and standard deviation of all quantities of interest.
Correlation (FC) was then computed between a pair of simulated BOLD timeseries.
Communication change was computed as described above. All the variances required in
the formula were computed from the simulated BOLD signals. To simulate the input BOLD
signals, we applied the Balloon–Windkessel model to transform the shared signal s(t) and
the idiosyncratic signals I1(t ), I2(t ).
Partial correlation was computed as the correlation between a pair of residualized series,
after removing the common variance from the rest of the network. In the current simulation
framework, inputs from the rest of the network are represented by the shared/unshared signals.
Therefore, we residualized the BOLD signals derived from x1(t), x1(t), against the input BOLD
signals derived from I1(t), I2(t), and s(t), and computed the correlation between the residualized
signals.
MSC Dataset
In the second portion of our investigation, we examined the relationship between FC, vari-
ance, and communication in an empirical dataset. We used the Midnight Scan Club (MSC),
a precision fMRI dataset (Gordon et al., 2017) that contains large quantities of both task and
resting-state data was selected, as large amounts of data are required to achieve highly reliable
FC measures (Gordon et al., 2017; Laumann et al., 2015). The dataset includes data from 10
participants who each participated in 10 separate fMRI sessions. During each session, each
participant completed three different tasks (a motor task, a categorization task, and a mixed
dot coherence/verbal discrimination task) and rest. This dataset is described in detail else-
where (see Gordon et al., 2017), but we will review parameters relevant to this study below.
In addition, a dataset of 120 healthy adults ( WashU-120) was used as a reference set for the
network definition and the calculation of graph theoretic metrics (Power et al., 2011; Power
et al., 2013). This dataset has been described in detail in Power et al. (2013).
Task design. Each resting-state session was 30 min, during which participants were asked to lie
still while fixating on a white cross presented against a black background. Each motor task
session included two runs (7.8 min total) of a blocked motor task adapted from the Human
Network Neuroscience
424
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
Connectome Project (Barch et al., 2013). In each block (15.4 s in duration), participants were
cued to move either their left or right hand, left or right foot, or tongue. Each run included two
blocks of each type of movement, as well as three fixation blocks (15.4 s). Each mixed task
session included two runs (14.2 min total) that each included four blocks, two of the semantic
tasks and two of the coherence tasks. Both tasks had a mixed block/event-related design, mod-
eled after the tasks in Dubis et al. (2016). During the semantic task, 30 individual trials were
presented, consisting of words presented for 0.5 s with jittered 1.7–8.3 s intervals. Participants
were asked to respond whether the words were nouns or verbs (50% nouns and 50% verbs
were included). Forty-four second fixation periods separated blocks. During the coherence
task (also 30 trials), individual trials consisted of arrays of Glass patterns (Glass, 1969) that
were varied in how concentrically they were arranged (50% or 0% coherence to a concentric
arrangement, displayed with equal frequency). Participants were asked to respond whether
dots were arranged concentrically or randomly. Each categorization task session included
three runs of an event-related categorization task with implicit repetitions, with a separate
run per stimulus type (face, scenes, and words). Within each run, participants viewed 24
images, repeated three times and made binary decisions.
MRI acquisition parameters. High-resolution T1-weighted (224 slices, 0.8 mm3 isotropic reso-
lution, TE = 3.74 ms, TR = 2,400 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 8 degrees), T2-weighted
(224 slices, 0.8 mm3 isotropic resolution, TE = 479 ms, TR = 3,200 ms) both with 0.8 isotropic
resolution, and resting-state BOLD data were collected on a Siemens 3T Magnetom Tim Trio
with a 12-channel head coil (Gordon et al., 2017). Functional scans were collected with a
gradient-echo EPI sequence, isotropic 4-mm3 voxels, TE of 27 ms, and TR of 2,200 ms
(Gordon et al., 2017). The MSC dataset is considered a deep (precision) dataset with 5 hours
of resting-state data per subject in 30-min blocks over 10 separate sessions (Gordon et al., 2017).
General preprocessing. The T1-weighted images were processed via automatic segmentation
of the gray matter, white matter, and ventricles in Freesurfer 5.3 (Fischl et al., 2002). The
default recon-all command in Freesurfer was then applied to produce the anatomical surface
for each subject (Dale et al., 1999). These surfaces were manually edited to improve the qual-
ity of the registration and the surfaces were registered to the fs_LR_32k surface space via the
procedure outlined in Glasser et al. (2013). Functional data preprocessing began with field
inhomogeneity distortion correction using the mean field map, which was applied to all ses-
sions. Slice timing correction was implemented using sinc interpolation to account for tempo-
ral misalignment in slice acquisition time. This step was followed by motion correction that
was conducted within and across BOLD runs (aligned to the first frame of the first run) through
a rigid body transformation. Then whole-brain intensity values across each BOLD run were
normalized to achieve a mode value of 1,000 (Miezin et al., 2000). Functional BOLD data
was first registered to a T2-weighted image and then to the T1. An affine transformation
was employed for registration. The T1-weighted image was aligned to a template atlas
(Lancaster et al., 1995) conforming to Talairach stereotactic atlas space (Talairach & Tournoux,
1988) using an affine transformation. All computed transformations and resampling to 3-mm
isotropic voxels were simultaneously applied at the end of these steps.
Functional connectivity preprocessing. Steps were taken to attenuate the impact of artifacts on
BOLD time series. The effect of nuisance signals was mitigated through regression of average
signal from the white matter, ventricles, whole brain (global signal), motion parameters, and
derivatives and expansion terms of the motion parameters (Friston, 1998; Power et al., 2014).
Removal of frames with FD > 0.2 毫米, in addition to sequences containing less than five con-
tiguous low-motion frames, the first 30-s of each run, and runs with < 50 low-motion frames
Network Neuroscience
425
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
t
/
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
was performed as an additional safeguard against the impact of motion (Power et al., 2014). A
special filtering procedure was applied to two MSC subjects (MSC03 and MSC10) with respi-
ratory contamination in their motion parameters. This procedure involved applying a low-pass
filtered at 0.1 Hz to reduce the effects of respiratory artifacts on motion estimates stemming
from the short-TR multiband acquisition, all before censoring high-motion frames (Fair et al.,
2020; Gratton et al., 2018; Laumann et al., 2017). Then a filtered FD threshold of 0.1 mm was
applied to censor frames. In all cases, flagged head motion frames were removed and the time
points were replaced with interpolated data using a power-spectral matched approach (Power
et al., 2014), and this was followed by the application of a bandpass filter (0.009–0.08 Hz).
Previous research has shown that ~30 min of low-motion data is necessary to achieve high
reliability of FC (Gordon et al., 2017). We therefore removed a participant (MSC08) that exhib-
ited high degree of motion (Gordon et al., 2017). The excluded participant also reported a high
level of drowsiness (Gordon et al., 2017; Laumann et al., 2017; Tagliazucchi & Laufs, 2014).
The processed BOLD data were mapped to each individual’s native midthickness surface
via the ribbon-constrained sampling procedure (Marcus et al., 2013) Then, the mapped data
were registered to the fsaverage surface in one step using the deformation map generated from
the ribbon-constrained sampling procedure described in Glasser et al. (2013). This was
followed by spatial smoothing conducted via a geodesic Gaussian smoothing kernel to the
surface registered data (FWHM = 6 mm, sigma = 2.55) (Gordon et al., 2016; Marcus et al.,
2011). Next, temporally interpolated frames were removed (prior to functional connectivity
analysis).
Regions and networks. We examined a previously published set of 333 brain parcels associated
with 12 networks somatomotor dorsal, somatomotor lateral, visual, auditory, cinguloopercular,
fronto-parietal, dorsal attention, ventral attention, salience, default, parietal memory, and retro-
splenial (Gordon et al., 2016).
In the case of the task scans, task related activation was captured
Task functional connectivity.
with a general linear model (GLM) using in-house software written in IDL (Research Systems,
Inc.), as described in Gratton et al. (2018), using a finite impulse response approach for trial-
level regressors (Ollinger et al., 2001). Functional connectivity was then measured for the task
scans following the “background connectivity” method (Al-Aidroos et al., 2012; Fair et al.,
2007) with the residuals from the GLM analysis being used for BOLD time series correlations.
The residuals were subjected to the aforementioned connectivity processing pipeline prior to
functional connectivity calculation. There are multiple methods to measure task FC, that here
we focus on background connectivity because of its simplicity and ability to track task changes
at the resolution we are interested in, and that it is well suited for comparison with a resting
baseline.
Intraclass correlation coefficients to quantify task discrimination. We used the Python package
Pingouin (Vallat, 2018) to evaluate how much variability in FC and communication across
scans is attributable to task type. Specifically, we used the function intraclass_corr, where
the “targets” are the task types (rest, mixed, categorization, motor), raters are the session index
(1–10). We used “ICC1k” output indicating that the target (task type) is rated by different raters
(the session) and the raters are selected at random. This is equivalent to the one-way ANOVA
fixed effects model (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Derek M. Smith would like to thank The Therapeutic Cognitive Neuroscience Fund.
Network Neuroscience
426
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
t
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Supporting information for this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00290.
Midnight Scan Club data is publicly available (https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds000224).
Code related to the analysis in this paper is located at https://github.com/ DijunQuant
/communication_public/.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Yuhua Yu: Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Visualization;
Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing. Caterina Gratton: Funding acquisition;
Supervision; Writing – review & editing. Derek M. Smith: Conceptualization; Data curation;
Funding acquisition; Methodology; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing.
FUNDING INFORMATION
Derek M. Smith, National Institutes of Health (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000002), Award
ID: T32NS047987. Caterina Gratton, National Science Foundation (https://dx.doi.org/10
.13039/100000001), Award ID: CAREER2048066. Caterina Gratton, National Institutes of
Health (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000002), Award ID: R01MH118370.
REFERENCES
Al-Aidroos, N., Said, C. P., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2012). Top-down
attention switches coupling between low-level and high-level
areas of human visual cortex. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(36),
14675–14680. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1202095109,
PubMed: 22908274
Allen, E. A., Damaraju, E., Plis, S. M., Erhardt, E. B., Eichele, T., &
Calhoun, V. D. (2014). Tracking whole-brain connectivity
dynamics in the resting state. Cerebral Cortex, 24(3), 663–676.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs352, PubMed: 23146964
Arbabshirani, M. R., Havlicek, M., Kiehl, K. A., Pearlson, G. D., &
Calhoun, V. D. (2013). Functional network connectivity during
rest and task conditions: A comparative study. Human Brain
Mapping, 34(11), 2959–2971. https://doi.org/10.1002/ hbm
.22118, PubMed: 22736522
Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of
locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and
optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28,
403–450. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604
.135709, PubMed: 16022602
Baccalá, L. A., & Sameshima, K. (2001). Partial directed coherence:
A new concept in neural structure determination. Biological
Cybernetics, 84(6), 463–474. https://doi.org/10.1007
/PL00007990, PubMed: 11417058
Barch, D. M., Burgess, G. C., Harms, M. P., Petersen, S. E.,
Schlaggar, B. L., Corbetta, M., Glasser, M. F., Curtiss, S., Dixit,
S., Feldt, C., Nolan, D., Bryant, E., Hartley, T., Footer, O., Bjork,
J. M., Poldrack, R., Smith, S., Johansen-Berg, H., Snyder, A. Z., &
Van Essen, D. C. (2013). Function in the human connectome:
Task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. NeuroImage,
80, 169–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05
.033, PubMed: 23684877
Bartko, J. J. (1976). On various intraclass correlation reliability coef-
ficients. Psychological Bulletin, 83(5), 762–765. https://doi.org
/10.1037/0033-2909.83.5.762
Behseta, S., Berdyyeva, T., Olson, C. R., & Kass, R. E. (2009). Bayes-
ian correction for attenuation of correlation in multi-trial spike
count data. Journal of Neurophsiology, 101(4), 2186–2193.
https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90727.2008, PubMed: 19129297
Biswal, B., Zerrin Yetkin, F., Haughton, V. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1995).
Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human
brain using echo-planar MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine,
34(4), 537–541. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910340409,
PubMed: 8524021
Buxton, R. B., Uludağ, K., Dubowitz, D. J., & Liu, T. T. (2004).
Modeling the hemodynamic response to brain activation. Neuro-
Image, 23(Suppl. 1), S220–S233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2004.07.013, PubMed: 15501093
Chang, C., & Glover, G. (2010). Time-frequency dynamics of
resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuro-
Image, 50(1), 81–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009
.12.011, PubMed: 20006716
Chow, C. C., & Karimipanah, Y. (2020). Before and beyond the
Wilson–Cowan equations. Journal of Neurophsiology, 123(5),
1645–1656. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00404.2019, PubMed:
32186441
Cole, M. W., Bassett, D. S., Power, J. D., Braver, T. S., & Petersen,
S. E. (2014). Intrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of
the human brain. Neuron, 83(1), 238–251. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014, PubMed: 24991964
Cole, M. W., Yang, G. J., Murray, J. D., Repovš, G., & Anticevic, A.
(2016). Functional connectivity change as shared signal dynam-
ics. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 259, 22–39. https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.11.011, PubMed: 26642966
Network Neuroscience
427
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
/
t
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical
surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruc-
tion. NeuroImage, 9(2), 179–194. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg
.1998.0395, PubMed: 9931268
Daunizeau, J., Stephan, K. E., & Friston, K. J. (2012). Stochastic
dynamic causal modelling of fMRI data: Should we care about
neural noise? NeuroImage, 62(1), 464–481. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.061, PubMed: 22579726
David, O., Cosmelli, D., & Friston, K. J. (2004). Evaluation of differ-
ent measures of functional connectivity using a neural mass
model. NeuroImage, 21(2), 659–673. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2003.10.006, PubMed: 14980568
Dubis, J. W., Siegel, J. S., Neta, M., Visscher, K. M., & Petersen, S. E.
(2016). Tasks driven by perceptual information do not recruit
sustained bold activity in cingulo-opercular regions. Cerebral
Cortex, 26(1), 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu187,
PubMed: 25150283
Duff, E. P., Makin, T., Cottaar, M., Smith, S. M., & Woolrich, M. W.
(2018). Disambiguating brain functional connectivity. Neuro-
Image, 173, 540–550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2018.01.053, PubMed: 29476911
Fair, D. A., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Snyder, A. Z., Perrone, A.,
Earl, E. A., Van, A. N., Koller, J. M., Feczko, E., Tisdall, M. D.,
van der Kouwe, A., Klein, R. L., Mirro, A. E., Hampton, J. M.,
Adeyemo, B., Laumann, T. O., Gratton, C., Greene, D. J.,
Schlaggar, B. L., Hagler, D. J., … Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2020).
Correction of respiratory artifacts in MRI head motion estimates.
NeuroImage, 208, 116400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2019.116400, PubMed: 31778819
Fair, D. A., Schlaggar, B. L., Cohen, A. L., Miezin, F. M.,
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Wenger, K. K., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z.,
Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (2007). A method for using
blocked and event-related fMRI data to study “resting state”
functional connectivity. NeuroImage, 35(1), 396–405. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.051, PubMed: 17239622
Fiecas, M., Ombao, H., van Lunen, D., Baumgartner, R., Coimbra,
A., & Feng, D. (2013). Quantifying temporal correlations: A
test-retest evaluation of functional connectivity in resting-state
fMRI. NeuroImage, 65, 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2012.09.052, PubMed: 23032492
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove,
C., van der Kouwe, A., Killiany, R., Kennedy, D., Klaveness, S.,
Montillo, A., Makris, N., Rosen, B., & Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole
brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical
structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X, PubMed: 11832223
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen,
D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically
organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(27),
9673–9678. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102,
PubMed: 15976020
Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal
communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cogni-
tive Sciences, 9(10), 474–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics
.2005.08.011, PubMed: 16150631
Friston, K. J. (1998). Modes or models: A critique on independent
component analysis for fMRI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10),
373–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01227-3,
PubMed: 21227247
Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: A
review. Brain Connectivity, 1(1), 13–36. https://doi.org/10.1089
/brain.2011.0008, PubMed: 22432952
Friston, K. J., Harrison, L., & Penny, W. (2003). Dynamic causal
modelling. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1273–1302. https://doi.org/10
.1016/S1053-8119(03)00202-7, PubMed: 12948688
Friston, K. J., Mechelli, A., Turner, R., & Price, C. J. (2000). Nonlin-
ear responses in fMRI: The Balloon model, Volterra kernels, and
other hemodynamics. NeuroImage, 12(4), 466–477. https://doi
.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0630, PubMed: 10988040
Friston, K. J., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R. S. J., &
Turner, R. (1996). Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 35(3), 346–355. https://doi.org
/10.1002/mrm.1910350312, PubMed: 8699946
Glass, L. (1969). Moiré effect from random dots. Nature, 223(5206),
578–580. https://doi.org/10.1038/223578a0, PubMed: 5799528
Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S.,
Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., Xu, J., Jbabdi, S., Webster, M.,
Polimeni, J. R., Van Essen, D. C., & Jenkinson, M. (2013). The
minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome
Project. NeuroImage, 80, 105–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2013.04.127, PubMed: 23668970
Gonzalez-Castillo, J., & Bandettini, P. A. (2018). Task-based
dynamic functional connectivity: Recent findings and open ques-
tions. NeuroImage, 180(Pt B), 526–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2017.08.006, PubMed: 28780401
Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Adeyemo, B., Huckins, J. F., Kelley,
W. M., & Petersen, S. E. (2016). Generation and evaluation of a
cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cerebral
Cortex, 26(1), 288–303. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu239,
PubMed: 25316338
Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Gilmore, A. W., Newbold, D. J.,
Greene, D. J., Berg, J. J., Ortega, M., Hoyt-Drazen, C., Gratton, C.,
Sun, H., Hampton, J. M., Coalson, R. S., Nguyen, A. L., McDermott,
K. B., Shimony, J. S., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., Petersen, S. E.,
Nelson, S. M., & Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2017). Precision functional
mapping of individual human brains. Neuron, 95(4), 791–807.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011, PubMed: 28757305
Gordon, E. M., Lynch, C. J., Gratton, C., Laumann, T. O., Gilmore,
A. W., Greene, D. J., Ortega, M., Nguyen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L.,
Petersen, S. E., Dosenbach, N. U. F., & Nelson, S. M. (2018).
Three distinct sets of connector hubs integrate human brain
function. Cell Reports, 24(7), 1687–1695. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.celrep.2018.07.050, PubMed: 30110625
Gratton, C., Laumann, T. O., Gordon, E. M., Adeyemo, B., &
Petersen, S. E. (2016). Evidence for two independent factors that
modify brain networks to meet task goals. Cell Reports, 17(5),
1276–1288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2016.10.002,
PubMed: 27783943
Gratton, C., Laumann, T. O., Nielsen, A. N., Greene, D. J., Gordon,
E. M., Gilmore, A. W., Nelson, S. M., Coalson, R. S., Snyder,
A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., Dosenbach, N. U. F., & Petersen, S. E.
(2018). Functional brain networks are dominated by stable group
and individual factors, not cognitive or daily variation. Neuron,
98(2), 439–452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.035,
PubMed: 29673485
Network Neuroscience
428
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
/
t
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
.
t
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
Guedj, C., Monfardini, E., Reynaud, A. J., Farnè, A., Meunier, M., &
Hadj-Bouziane, F. (2017). Boosting norepinephrine transmission
triggers flexible reconfiguration of brain networks at rest. Cerebral
Cortex, 27(10), 4691–4700. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor
/bhw262, PubMed: 27600848
Hutchison, R. M., Womelsdorf, T., Allen, E. A., Bandettini, P. A.,
Calhoun, V. D., Corbetta, M., Della Penna, S., Duyn, J. H.,
Glover, G. H., Gonzalez-Castillo, J., Handwerker, D. A.,
Keilholz, S., Kiviniemi, V., Leopold, D. A., de Pasquale, F.,
Sporns, O., Walter, M., & Chang, C. (2013). Dynamic functional
connectivity: Promise, issues, and interpretations. NeuroImage,
80, 360–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05
.079, PubMed: 23707587
Ito, T., Brincat, S. L., Siegel, M., Mill, R. D., He, B. J., Miller, E. K.,
Rotstein, H. G., & Cole, M. W. (2020). Task-evoked activity
quenches neural correlations and variability across cortical
areas. PLoS Computational Biology, 16(8), e1007983. https://
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007983, PubMed: 32745096
Kietzmann, T. C., McClure, P., & Kriegeskorte, N. (2019). Deep neural
networks in computational neuroscience. In Oxford research
encyclopedia of neuroscience (pp. 1–28). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190264086.013.46
Krienen, F. M., Yeo, B. T. T., & Buckner, R. L. (2014). Reconfigurable
task-dependent functional coupling modes cluster around a core
functional architecture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369(1653),
20130526. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0526, PubMed:
25180304
Krystal, J. H., D’Souza, D. C., Mathalon, D., Perry, E., Belger, A., &
Hoffman, R. (2003). NMDA receptor antagonist effects, cortical
glutamatergic function, and schizophrenia: Toward a paradigm
shift in medication development. Psychopharmacology, 169(3–4),
215–233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1582-z, PubMed:
12955285
Lancaster, J. L., Glass, T. G., Lankipalli, B. R., Downs, H., Mayberg,
H., & Fox, P. T. (1995). A modality-independent approach to spa-
tial normalization of tomographic images of the human brain.
Human Brain Mapping, 3(3), 209–223. https://doi.org/10.1002
/hbm.460030305
Laumann, T. O., Gordon, E. M., Adeyemo, B., Snyder, A. Z., Joo,
S. J., Chen, M. Y., Gilmore, A. W., McDermott, K. B., Nelson,
S. M., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Schlaggar, B. L., Mumford, J. A.,
Poldrack, R. A., & Petersen, S. E. (2015). Functional system
and areal organization of a highly sampled individual human
brain. Neuron, 87(3), 657–670. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron
.2015.06.037, PubMed: 26212711
Laumann, T. O., & Snyder, A. Z. (2021). Brain activity is not only for
thinking. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 40, 130–136.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.04.002
Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Mitra, A., Gordon, E. M., Gratton,
C., Adeyemo, B., Gilmore, A. W., Nelson, S. M., Berg, J. J.,
Greene, D. J., McCarthy, J. E., Tagliazucchi, E., Laufs, H.,
Schlaggar, B. L., Dosenbach, N. U. F., & Petersen, S. E. (2017).
On the stability of BOLD fMRI correlations. Cerebral Cortex,
27(10), 4719–4732. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/ bhw265,
PubMed: 27591147
Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., & Oeltermann,
A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the
fMRI signal. Nature, 412(6843), 150–157. https://doi.org/10
.1038/35084005, PubMed: 11449264
Ma, Y., Shaik, M. A., Kozberg, M. G., Kim, S. H., Portes, J. P.,
Timerman, D., & Hillman, E. M. C. (2016). Resting-state hemo-
dynamics are spatiotemporally coupled to synchronized and
symmetric neural activity in excitatory neurons. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, 113(52), E8463–E8471.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1525369113, PubMed: 27974609
Mahadevan, A. S., Tooley, U. A., Bertolero, M. A., Mackey, A. P., &
Bassett, D. S. (2021). Evaluating the sensitivity of functional con-
nectivity measures to motion artifact in resting-state fMRI data.
N e u ro I m a g e , 2 4 1 , 11 8 4 0 8 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / j
.neuroimage.2021.118408, PubMed: 34284108
Marcus, D. S., Harms, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., Jenkinson, M., Wilson,
J. A., Glasser, M. F., Barch, D. M., Archie, K. A., Burgess, G. C.,
Ramaratnam, M., Hodge, M., Horton, W., Herrick, R., Olsen, T.,
McKay, M., House, M., Hileman, M., Reid, E., Harwell, J., … Van
Essen, D. C. (2013). Human Connectome Project informatics:
Quality control, database services, and data visualization. Neu-
roImage, 80, 202–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2013.05.077, PubMed: 23707591
Marcus, D. S., Harwell, J., Olsen, T., Hodge, M., Glasser, M. F.,
Prior, F., Jenkinson, M., Laumann, T., Curtiss, S. W., & Van Essen,
D. C. (2011). Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for
the Human Connectome Project. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 5, 4.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2011.00004, PubMed: 21743807
Marrelec, G., Krainik, A., Duffau, H., Pélégrini-Issac, M., Lehéricy,
S., Doyon, J., & Benali, H. (2006). Partial correlation for func-
tional brain interactivity investigation in functional MRI. Neuro-
Image, 32(1), 228–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2005.12.057, PubMed: 16777436
McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about
some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods,
1(1), 30–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.30
Mena-Segovia, J., & Bolam, J. P. (2017). Rethinking the pedunculo-
pontine nucleus: From cellular organization to function. Neuron,
94(1), 7–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.027,
PubMed: 28384477
Miezin, F. M., Maccotta, L., Ollinger, J. M., Petersen, S. E., &
Buckner, R. L. (2000). Characterizing the hemodynamic
response: Effects of presentation rate, sampling procedure, and
the possibility of ordering brain activity based on relative timing.
NeuroImage, 11(6), 735–759. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000
.0568, PubMed: 10860799
Morales, M., & Margolis, E. B. (2017). Ventral tegmental area:
Cellular heterogeneity, connectivity and behaviour. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 18(2), 73–85. https://doi.org/10.1038
/nrn.2016.165, PubMed: 28053327
Ollinger, J. M., Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). Separating
processes within a trial in event-related functional MRI: II.
Analysis. NeuroImage, 13(1), 218–229. https://doi.org/10.1006
/nimg.2000.0711, PubMed: 11133324
Patel, R. S., Bowman, F. D. B., & Rilling, J. K. (2006). A Bayesian
approach to determining connectivity of the human brain.
Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 267–276. https://doi.org/10.1002
/hbm.20182, PubMed: 16092131
Porter, A., Nielsen, A., Dorn, M., Dworetsky, A., Edmonds, D., &
Gratton, C. (2023). Masked features of task states found in
Network Neuroscience
429
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
t
/
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
.
t
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
From correlation to communication
individual brain networks. Cerebral Cortex, 33(6), 2879–2900.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac247, PubMed: 35802477
Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Barnes, K. A.,
Church, J. A., Vogel, A. C., Laumann, T. O., Miezin, F. M., Schlaggar,
B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2011). Functional network organization of
the human brain. Neuron, 72(4), 665–678. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006, PubMed: 22099467
Power, J. D., Mitra, A., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar,
B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2014). Methods to detect, characterize,
and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage,
84, 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08
.048, PubMed: 23994314
Power, J. D., Schlaggar, B. L., Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N., & Petersen,
S. E. (2013). Evidence for hubs in human functional brain net-
works. Neuron, 79(4), 798–813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuron.2013.07.035, PubMed: 23972601
Raut, R. V., Snyder, A. Z., Mitra, A., Yellin, D., Fujii, N., Malach, R.,
& Raichle, M. E. (2021). Global waves synchronize the brain's
functional systems with fluctuating arousal. Science Advances,
7(30), eabf2709. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abf2709,
PubMed: 34290088
Selden, N. R., Gitelman, D. R., Salamon-Murayama, N., Parrish,
T. B., & Mesulam, M. M. (1998). Trajectories of cholinergic path-
ways within the cerebral hemispheres of the human brain. Brain,
121(12), 2249–2257. https://doi.org/10.1093/ brain/121.12
.2249, PubMed: 9874478
Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Yeo, T. B., Liu, H., & Johnson, K. A.
(2012). Stepwise connectivity of the modal cortex reveals the
multimodal organization of the human brain. Journal of Neuro-
science, 32(31), 10649–10661. https://doi.org/10.1523
/JNEUROSCI.0759-12.2012, PubMed: 22855814
Shafiei, G., Zeighami, Y., Clark, C. A., Coull, J. T., Nagano-Saito, A.,
Leyton, M., Dagher, A., & Mišić, B. (2019). Dopamine signaling
modulates the stability and integration of intrinsic brain net-
works. Cerebral Cortex, 29(1), 397–409. https://doi.org/10.1093
/cercor/bhy264, PubMed: 30357316
Shine, J. M. (2019). Neuromodulatory influences on integration and
segregation in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(7),
572–583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.002, PubMed:
31076192
Shine, J. M., Bissett, P. G., Bell, P. T., Koyejo, O., Balsters, J. H.,
Gorgolewski, K. J., Moodie, C. A., & Poldrack, R. A. (2016).
The dynamics of functional brain networks: Integrated network
states during cognitive task performance. Neuron, 92(2),
544–554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.018,
PubMed: 27693256
Shine, J. M., van den Brink, R. L., Hernaus, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., &
Poldrack, R. A. (2018). Catecholaminergic manipulation alters
dynamic network topology across cognitive states. Network Neu-
roscience, 2(3), 381–396. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00042,
PubMed: 30294705
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in
assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420–428.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420, PubMed: 18839484
Smith, S. M. (2012). The future of fMRI connectivity. NeuroImage,
62(2), 1257–1266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012
.01.022, PubMed: 22248579
Tagliazucchi, E., & Laufs, H. (2014). Decoding wakefulness levels
from typical fMRI resting-state data reveals reliable drifts between
wakefulness and sleep. Neuron, 82(3), 695–708. https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.020, PubMed: 24811386
Talairach, J., & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of
the human brain. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers.
Turchi, J., Chang, C., Ye, F. Q., Russ, B. E., Yu, D. K., Cortes, C. R.,
Monosov, I. E., Duyn, J. H., & Leopold, D. A. (2018). The basal
forebrain regulates global resting-state fMRI fluctuations. Neuron,
97(4), 940–952. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.032,
PubMed: 29398365
Vallat, R. (2018). Pingouin: Statistics in Python. Journal of Open
Source Software, 3(31), 1026. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss
.01026
Vicente, R., Wibral, M., Lindner, M., & Pipa, G. (2011). Transfer
entropy-a model-free measure of effective connectivity for the
neurosciences. Journal of Computational Neuroscience, 30(1),
45–67. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-010-0262-3, PubMed:
20706781
Vijayraghavan, S., Wang, M., Birnbaum, S. G., Williams, G. V., &
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2007). Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor
actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory.
Nature Neuroscience, 10(3), 376–384. https://doi.org/10.1038
/nn1846, PubMed: 17277774
Wilson, H. R., & Cowan, J. D. (1972). Excitatory and inhibitory
interactions in localized populations of model neurons. Biophys-
ical Journal, 12(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3495(72)
86068-5, PubMed: 4332108
Wu, L., Caprihan, A., & Calhoun, V. (2021). Tracking spatial
dynamics of functional connectivity during a task. NeuroImage,
239, 118310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021
.118310, PubMed: 34175424
Yao, H., Shi, L., Han, F., Gao, H., & Dan, Y. (2007). Rapid learning
in cortical coding of visual scenes. Nature Neuroscience, 10(6),
772–778. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1895, PubMed: 17468750
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari,
D., Hollinshead, M., Roffman, J. L., Smoller, J. W., Zöllei, L.,
Polimeni, J. R., Fischl, B., Liu, H., & Buckner, R. L. (2011). The
organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic
functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3),
1125–1165. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011, PubMed:
21653723
Zucker, R. S., & Regehr, W. G. (2002). Short-term synaptic plasticity.
Annual Review of Physiology, 64, 355–405. https://doi.org/10
.1146/annurev.physiol.64.092501.114547, PubMed: 11826273
Network Neuroscience
430
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
t
/
/
e
d
u
n
e
n
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
l
f
/
/
/
/
/
7
2
4
1
1
2
1
1
8
4
5
5
n
e
n
_
a
_
0
0
2
9
0
p
d
t
.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3