Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural

Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural
Efficiency of Working Memory

Gilbert Preston1, Erik Anderson1, Claudio Silva1
Terry Goldberg2, 和埃里克·M. Wassermann3

抽象的

■ Working memory ( WM) has been described as short-term
retention of information that is no longer accessible in the en-
环境, and the manipulation of this information for sub-
sequent use in guiding behavior. WM is viewed as a cognitive
process underlying higher-order cognitive functions. 证据
supports a critical role for PFC in mediating WM performance.
Studies show psychomotor processing speed and accuracy
account for considerable variance in neural efficiency (Ne). 这
study compared the relative effects of active and sham 10 赫兹
rTMS applied to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) on indi-

ces of Ne in healthy participants performing a WM paradigm
that models the association between WM load and task be-
行为 [Sternberg, S. High-speed scanning in human memory.
科学, 153, 652–654, 1966]. Previous studies identified a rela-
tionship between diminished Ne and impaired WM across a
broad array of clinical disorders. In the present study, 这
authors predicted there would be a main effect of stimulation
团体 (STM) on accuracy (SCR) and processing speed (RT),
因此, Ne. We observed a main effect of STM for RT without an
effect on SCR; even so, there was a robust effect of STM on Ne.

介绍

Working memory ( WM) has been described as short-
term retention of information that is no longer accessible
in the environment, and the manipulation of this informa-
tion for subsequent use in guiding behavior (DʼEsposito,
Postle, & Rypma, 2000). WM is widely viewed as a cognitive
process that underlies an array of higher order cogni-
主动功能, such as reasoning (Rypma, Prabhakaran,
德斯蒙德, & Gabrieli, 2001), 规划 (Goel & Grafman,
1995), and problem solving (Duncan & 欧文, 2000), 为了
实例. Over the past few decades, such studies have de-
veloped a substantial body of convergent evidence that
supports a critical role for prefrontal cortex (全氟碳化物) in medi-
ating WM performance. 例如, in a study aimed to
identify the relationship between WM demand and task
表现, Rypma, Berger, and DʼEsposito (2002) 成立
that task factors exert their effects largely during WM
encoding. Subject factorsʼ influence on task performance
occurred mainly during WM retrieval. In an fMRI study
(Rypma et al., 2002), subjects performed a WM task that
required them to maintain from one to eight letters over
a brief delay. Neural activity was measured during en-
编码, 维护, and retrieval task phases. With in-
creasing memory load, the researchers observed that RT
increased and accuracy (SCR) decreased. 此外, A
decline in ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) activation was seen

1University of Utah, 2Hillside-Zucker Hospital, Glen Oaks, 纽约, 3Na-
tional Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda, 医学博士

during encoding, whereas dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) 交流电-
tivation increased during maintenance and retrieval. 这
authors concluded that VLPFC mediates WM storage and
that DLPFC mediates memory organization processes that
facilitate supra-capacity WM storage. 有趣的是, 高的-
performing subjects in toto showed less activation than
low-performing subjects, suggesting that high performers
utilized fewer neural resources in resolving task require-
评论. In comparison to low performers, such task perfor-
mance could plausibly be seen as an example of greater
neural efficiency (Ne). 即使是这样, higher-scoring performers
showed activation increases with increasing memory load
during maintenance and retrieval in lateral PFC. 全面的,
low-performing subjects showed more activation than
high-performing subjects, but high performers showed
minimal activation increases in DLPFC with increasing
memory load. The authors suggested that their results in-
dicate that individual differences in Ne and cognitive strat-
egy mediate individual differences in WM performance
(Rypma et al., 2002). In a subsequent study comparing
WM performance in younger and older adults, Rypma,
Berger, Genova, Rebbechi, and DʼEsposito (2005) 用过的
event-related fMRI to measure brain activity while younger
and older adults performed an item-recognition task with
set size varying between one and eight letters. Both groups
experienced a decline in Ne indexed as RT increases and
accuracy decreases with increasing memory set size. 这
authors also found related differences in neural activation
with increasing memory set size in PFC. Analysis of in-
dividual subjectsʼ performance relative to cortical activity

© 2009 麻省理工学院

认知神经科学杂志 22:3, PP. 447–456

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

F

/

.

.

.

t


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

suggested that speed and accuracy accounted for con-
siderable variance in dorsal and ventral PFC activity during
encoding and retrieval. The authors concluded that their
results suggested support to a model of cognitive slowing
in which processing rate is related to Ne (Rypma et al.,
2005). 与此相类似, Altamura et al. (2007) used fMRI
to dissociate the effects of Sternberg WM demands on
DLPFC. The effect of varying loads of three, 五, or eight
letters on WM maintenance was determined by using two
time delays (1 sec and 6 秒) between the letter set and
the probe stimuli. The authors observed strongly activated
DLPFC with load manipulation. 尤其, regions of right
DLPFC were exclusively affected by load. The authors con-
cluded that their results suggested the possibility that top–
down modulation of cognitive control during encoding or
response to probe stimulus could be mediated by these
地区 (Altamura et al., 2007).

日益, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) 是
used as a research tool for mapping brain function and
modification of neural processes during WM tasks that en-
gage PFC. TMS has been shown to be a safe, noninvasive
means of stimulating the awake and alert human cere-
bral cortex in carefully screened subjects ( Wassermann,
1998). 例如, Koch et al. (2005) used a repetitive
TMS (rTMS) approach to disambiguate the spatial distribu-
tion and reciprocal interactions of different regions of the
parieto-frontal network in healthy human participants per-
forming a spatial WM task. These researchers compared
the effect of 25 Hz rTMS on neural activity within PFC
和后顶叶皮质 (个人电脑) during the delay and
decision phases of the task. Trains of rTMS at 25 Hz were
applied to PPC, premotor cortex, and right DLPFC alterna-
tively during the two phases. They found that TMS dur-
ing the delay phase in both parietal cortex and DLPFC
interfered with performance. When 25-Hz rTMS trains
were applied during the decision phase, they observed in-
terference in DLPFC, but not in PPC or premotor cortex. A
local neural network subserving decisional processes and
a second neural population functionally interconnected
with PPC that was activated when spatial information was
maintained temporarily in WM (Koch et al., 2005). Brandt,
Ploner, 迈耶, Leistner, and Villringer (1998) investigated
the role of DLPFC and PPC in a visuospatial delayed-
response task. Using high-frequency rTMS, the researchers
interfered temporarily with cortical activity in DLPFC and
PPC during the maintenance period in order to determine
whether rTMS to DLPFC or PPC during maintenance af-
fects accuracy of memory-guided saccades. Stimulation
over DLPFC significantly impaired accuracy of amplitude
and direction of saccades. They concluded that, 之内
this network, DLPFC mediates the mnemonic representa-
tion and PPC with the sensory representation of spatially
defined perceptual information (Brandt et al., 1998).
Studies of the chronometrics of WM phase evolution have
used TMS in conjunction with fMRI to induce a temporary
lesion that is intended to interfere with task performance,
因此, identifying a causal role for the targeted cortical

region in mediating WM task performance. Such studies
have shown that the effects of rTMS on cognitive perfor-
mance are dependent on domain, experimental design,
cortical target, stimulation frequency, 定时, and duration
of TMS application relative to the stage of task processing.
最后, most studies of WM and the role of PFC
have been aimed to identify whether and precisely when
functional disruption of PFC would degrade WM perfor-
曼斯. 例如, Mull and Seyal (2001) aimed to deter-
mine whether transient functional disruption of DLPFC
would impair performance in a WM task in which par-
ticipants were shown sequences of letters and asked to
decide if the letter just displayed was the same as the letter
presented three trials back. Single-pulse TMS was applied
over DLPFC between letter presentations. TMS to left
DLPFC caused increased errors relative to controls. 这
authors concluded that their results supported the idea
that DLPFC played a crucial role in this paradigm. Delete-
rious effects of TMS to LPFC on WM were also observed
by Osaka et al. (2007) in a study using paired-pulse TMS.
Albeit fewer in number, studies showing beneficial effects
of TMS on WM have also observed a critical role for stim-
ulation frequency and timing relative to the phase of
task evolution. 例如, individual power and peak
frequency attributes of alpha band oscillations have been
shown to predict performance on a WM task (Luber et al.,
2007; Klimesch, Sauseng, & 格尔洛夫, 2003). In a previous
学习, the present authors designed a combined EEG–
TMS study of the interaction between WM Ne, alpha band
oscillations, 和 10 Hz rTMS applied to DLPFC prior to
the onset of a Sternberg task (安德森, 普雷斯顿, & 席尔瓦,
2007). We predicted first that compared with sham active
10 Hz rTMS would significantly reduce psychomotor pro-
cessing speed indexed as RT. 第二, we predicted that
improvement in RT would be associated with a 10-Hz
rTMS induced increase in pretask alpha power and pretask
alpha band phase synchrony. We identified a robust as-
sociation between 10 Hz rTMS perturbed individual mean
peak alpha frequency and alpha spectral phase synchrony
on task performance.

Frequency and target-specific effects have also been
observed in studies of cognitive domains other than WM:
例如, enhancement of picture naming and posterior
superior temporal cortex (Mottaghy, Sparing, & Topper,
2006); enhancement of episodic memory and left inferior
全氟碳化物 (Kohler, Paus, 巴克纳, & Milner, 2004); analogic rea-
soning and left DLPFC (Boroojerdi et al., 2001); 精神的
rotation of 3-D objects and mesial frontal and right parietal
cortex (Klimesch et al., 2003). 最后, Luber et al. (2007)
showed that TMS could enhance WM. They predicted
that healthy subjects’ performance on a delayed match-to-
sample task would be enhanced when TMS of 1, 5, 或者 20 赫兹
was applied to either left dorsolateral prefrontal or midline
parietal cortex during the delay phase of the task. 他们
成立 5 赫兹, 但不是 1 Hz or 20 赫兹, stimulation to the
midline parietal site during delay resulted in a significant de-
crease in RT without a corresponding decrease in accuracy

448

认知神经科学杂志

体积 22, 数字 3

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

t

.

.

.

F

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

(SCR). In a second experiment, 5 Hz rTMS was applied to
the parietal site during the delay or during presentation of
the probe. RT enhancement occurred only with stimulation
during the delay phase. The authors concluded TMS could
enhance WM performance under specific conditions of
时间, 目标, and stimulation frequency.

The present study was designed to determine the effects
的 10 Hz rTMS directed to right or left DLPFC on psycho-
motor processing speed (RT), 准确性 (SCR), and indices
of Ne, on a WM task (Sternberg, 1966). In view of the
numerous studies that showed a critical role for DLPFC
in processing WM tasks, yet a minority of studies that show
performance enhancement when rTMS was directed to
DLPFC during evolutionary stages of a WM task, we enter-
tained the notion that subject distraction might play a role
in such an apparent discrepancy. Relevant in this regard
is the study of Abler et al. (2005). In order to determine
the relation between TMS-induced distraction and perfor-
曼斯, Abler et al. asked healthy subjects to evaluate the
discomfort caused by TMS during a verbal WM task. Sub-
jects were first studied using fMRI to identify specific cor-
tical regions engaged in task processing. 随后,
left DLPFC, inferior frontal, 顶叶, and temporo-parietal
cortices were identified and stimulated during the delay
phase of a delayed match-to-sample task. Task perfor-
mance and subjective disturbance due to side effects were
monitored. Subjectsʼ level of discomfort was correlated
with their error rates: The higher the discomfort, the more
errors they made. Abler et al. concluded that TMS sensory–
motor side effects induced during task evolution may
bias task performance and lead to misinterpretation of
结果. Our predictions were also influenced by earlier
work that showed DLPFC plays an important role in WM.
例如, Hamidi, 托诺尼, and Postle (2009) used high-
frequency rTMS to evaluate the role of DLPFC in memory-
guided response to two different types of spatial WM tasks:
one requiring a recognition decision about a probe stimu-
lus using a YES–NO button press, another requiring recall
of the stimulus location. In half the trials, randomly dis-
tributed rTMS was applied to DLPFC; in a separate session,
the same stimulation was directed to the superior parietal
lobule (声压级), a brain area implicated in spatial WM stor-
年龄. In each condition, a 3-sec train of 10 Hz at 110% 的
motor threshold was delivered at the onset of the response
时期. DLPFC, but not SPL, rTMS affected performance;
rTMS to right DLPFC decreased SCR of delayed-recall trials;
rTMS to left DLPFC enhanced SCR on delayed-recognition
试验. The authors concluded that their results suggested
DLPFC plays an important role in memory-guided re-
sponse and that the nature of such a role varies with the
processes required for making a response (Hamidi et al.,
2009).

The concept of Ne in human studies is largely derived
from fMRI studies of the relationship between cognitive
task performance and neural activation indexed by the
signal magnitude of blood oxygen level-dependent BOLD
utilization. An array of similar investigations lends sup-

port to the finding that as item-load increases, 大胆的
signal amplitude increases. Such increases have been re-
peatedly shown to be inversely associated with task per-
formance. 例如, 古尔德, 棕色的, 欧文, ffytche, 和
霍华德 (2003) used event-related fMRI to measure PFC
activity while younger and older adults performed an
item-recognition task in which the memory set size varied
between one and eight letters. RT increased and accuracy
decreased with increasing memory set size. Variance in RT
and BOLD signal in dorsal and ventral PFC accounted
for most of the variance. The authors concluded that their
results support a model of cognitive slowing with age in
which a decline in processing speed is related to a decline
in Ne (古尔德等人。, 2003).

In the present rTMS study, we predicted first that com-
pared with sham active 10 Hz rTMS applied to either right
or left DLPFC 10 sec prior to task onset would enhance the
neural efficiency (Ne) of WM operationally indexed by the
relationship between RT and SCR, whereby Ne = work/
时间; 因此, Ne = accuracy/RT. Guided, 部分地, by earlier
work that suggested the auditory and sensory stimulation
of rTMS could degrade cognitive performance in partici-
pants who are distracted by such extraneous stimuli (Abler
等人。, 2005), we directed 10 Hz rTMS to either right or left
DLPFC serially 10 sec prior to task onset with the intention
of allowing time for resolution of the putative disruptive
effects of stimulation. 第二, we posited that if sensori-
motor side effects of rTMS biased our results, we would
see equal or better performance in the sham-stimulated
group compared with the actively stimulated group.

方法

Experimental Design

This was a single-blind, sham-controlled study (数字 1).
在 32 healthy participants, active or sham 10 Hz rTMS
was directed to either right or left DLPFC 10 sec prior
to the onset of a Sternberg short-term verbal recognition
memory paradigm. Five 5-sec trains of 10 Hz rTMS (for a
total of 250 pulses) were applied to DLPFC ending 10 秒
prior to task onset. Stimulation was followed by 48 单身的
trials of 6- and 8-item strings of capital letters that were
randomized and counterbalanced. 下一个, 250 additional
pulses of 10 Hz rTMS were delivered to DLPFC of the same
边, followed by a second set of 48 single trials of 6- 和
8-item strings of capital letters. After a 15- to 20-min rest,
the same sequence of events was repeated on side-2.

Outcome measures were pre- and poststimulation accu-

活泼的 (SCR), RT, and Ne by stimulation group (STM).

参加者

The Investigational Review Boards for the University of
Utah and Veterans Health Affairs of the Salt Lake City
Health Care System approved this study. Thirty-four medi-
cally healthy, right-handed participants, ages 18–55 years,

Preston et al.

449

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

/

.

t

F

.

.


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

from the University of Utah and the Salt Lake City commu-
nity were admitted to the study free of exclusionary condi-
tions for TMS (Wassermann, 1998) and without a past or
present psychiatric disorder. All participants had a negative
screen for drugs of abuse on the morning of the study.
Participants were instructed not to drink caffeinated bev-
erages for 12 hours prior to testing. Participants were com-
pensated for their time. Group assignment to stimulation
(active or sham) and side first stimulated (S-1) were pseudo-
randomized and counterbalanced.

Paradigm Training

Participants were trained and tested between 11:00 a.m.
和 1:00 下午. They were familiarized with the task and
response procedures by practicing to use their right first
finger to press one of two buttons (YES or NO) on a hand-
held keypad as comfortably quick as possible while attend-
ing to a fixation cue at the center of a 19-inch computer
monitor. Baseline values of SCR and RT were derived from
20 randomized and counterbalanced 5- and 7-item single
trials of the Sternberg task.

The Sternberg Paradigm

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Task stimuli and computation of RTs and accuracy (SCR)
were derived using Presentation 9.90 软件 (Neuro-
behavioral Systems, 旧金山, CA). A single trial of
this task was composed of four phases: 刺激 (A 5-
or 7-item string of uppercase consonants and vowels
was presented at the center of a 19-inch video monitor
为了 1.5 秒), delay (participants focused on a fixation marker
appearing at the center of the computer monitor while
maintaining the previous string in memory for 1.5 秒),
probe (a single uppercase letter appeared at center screen
为了 3 秒), 和回应 (participants were instructed to
decide as quickly and accurately as possible whether the
probe was present in the previous string, and to respond
by pressing one of two keypad buttons; if there was a re-
sponse within 3 秒, it was recorded, and the next trial
began. If there was no response, the trial was marked as
an error, and the next trial began). A minimum baseline
SCR of 80% on the 5-item, 和 70% on the more difficult
7-item task was required to continue. If necessary, partici-
pants were allowed to repeat each of the training para-
digms once and were disqualified if they failed to make
the minimum SCR. Training and testing phase item-strings
had equal numbers of randomized and counterbalanced
true positive and true negative probes.

实验过程中, participants were comfortably
seated in an individually adjustable chair. Participantsʼ
single-pulse TMS resting motor threshold (RMT) 曾是
determined by identification of the lowest level of TMS
machine output that would induce a visible contraction
of the first dorsal interosseous muscle of the dominant
hand on 5 的 10 pulses delivered with a 10-sec interpulse
间隔. A MagStimTM Super Rapid stimulator with physi-
cally identical sham and active air-cooled figure-of-eight
coils (Magstim, Whitland, Dyfed UK) was used to deliver
TMS pulses directed to either left or right DLPFC. 这
sound generated by capacitor discharge was identical in
the two coils; the sham coil did not generate a sensori-
motor stimulus. A custom-designed mechanical coil holder
was used to maintain contact between the midpoint of
the 70-mm figure-eight coil and the location of the fron-
tal scalp overlying right or left DLPFC. To determine the
scalp correlates for targeting of DLPFC, we used the mea-
sures 10–20 EEG system. Positioning of the coil on the
scalp was determined by the measured position of EEG
sensors F3 (左边) and F4 (正确的). Participants used dis-
posable in-ear sound protection during the experiment.
Side stimulated first (S-1) was randomly assigned and
counterbalanced.

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

F

.

.

.

/

t


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

数字 1. 实验设计.

450

认知神经科学杂志

体积 22, 数字 3

Subject Stimulation and Testing

A brief period of acclimation to the laboratory environment
was followed by the onset of five 5-sec trains of 10 Hz rTMS
with an intertrain interval of 10 sec at 100% of RMT for
a total of 250 pulses of 10 Hz rTMS. Stimulation ended
10 sec prior to the onset of the first 48 single trials of the
任务. In order to minimize the effects of practice on per-
formance, single trials in the testing phase were composed
of more difficult 6- or 8-item uppercase letters pseudo-
randomly presented and counterbalanced for true positive
and true negative probes.

This sequence was repeated once more on S-1. 下列的
A 15- to 20-min rest period, the entire sequence was re-
peated on S-2; 因此, the study totaled 192 single trials
和 1000 pulses of 10 Hz rTMS.

Planned Analyses

Skewness and kurtosis in the distribution of dependent
variables were identified and normalized with square
root transformation. Repeated measures ANOVA with
Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was used
to identify the main effects of STM, S-1, and interactions
between STM and S-1. Dependent variables were covaried
with RMT and age. Pairwise comparisons were used to de-
termine the within-subject effects of active and sham 10 赫兹
rTMS on SCR and RT by task load.

结果

Thirty-four right-handed men and 12 women recruited from
the Salt Lake City, 犹他州, community participated in this study.
Two subjects in the actively stimulated group were unable to
complete the study due to the development of moderate
muscle contraction headaches. In both cases, the headaches
spontaneously resolved. There were no adverse events in the
sham group. All participants achieved the minimum 5- 和
7-item training SCR on their first attempt.

Demographics

There was no difference in mean age between stimulation
groups or by sex. Mean age by Stimulation group: active =
28.6 (标准差= 5.68); sham = 30.88 (标准差= 11.2); 独立的-
samples two-tailed t test for equality of means; equal
variance not assumed: t(31) = −0.707, p = .478. 意思是
age by Sex: male = 28.75 年; female = 29.50 年;
two-tailed t test: t(30) = −0.25, p = .80. There was equal
distribution of the sexes between groups [Pearson un-
corrected chi-square test: χ2(1, n = 32) = 0.139, p = .710].

Resting Motor Threshold

The groups were evenly matched for the intensity of stim-
ulation indexed as RMT and expressed as a percent of max-

imum machine output [active RMT = 67%, sham RMT =
66%; two-tailed t(34) = 0.252, p = .4484].

Results of the Repeated Measures ANOVA

There was no main effect of S-1, and no interaction be-
tween S-1 and STM. Measures of dependent variables did
not covary with RMT, 性别, or age. In the absence of a main
effect of S-1, prestimulation and poststimulation values of
dependent variables were collapsed across side for subse-
quent analysis. There were no prestimulation differences
between groups regarding SCR, RT, or Ne. 有, 如何-
曾经, a robust main effect of STM on poststimulation RT
and Ne, but not SCR, as presented in Table 1.

In the present study, the authors directed 10 Hz rTMS
to right or left DLPFC 10 sec prior to task onset, predicting
there would be a main effect of STM on SCR, RT, and Ne.
We observed a robust main effect of STM in the compari-
son of poststimulation RT and Ne, but not SCR. The mean
poststimulation RT in the active group was 219 毫秒
(±0.16) faster than the prestimulation baseline, 然而
the mean poststimulation RT in the sham group was
30 毫秒 (±0.16) slower than the mean baseline RT. Pair-
wise comparisons of pre- and poststimulation RT, SCR, 和
Ne were computed by comparing the relatively easier 5-
and 7-item (prestimulation) measures with the 6- 和
8-item (poststimulation) 措施. Pairwise comparisons
的 5- and 7-item training RT data (桌子 2) show a small
but nonsignificant within-subject difference in both groups
for this comparison. 具体来说, pairwise comparisons in
the actively treated group showed that compared with the
larger 6-item and 8-item poststimulation loads, the 5-item
prestimulation RT was slower when the converse would
be expected. Similar results were seen when the compari-
son of the prestimulation 7-item RT was compared with
the poststimulation 8-item RT. In the sham group, 相同
comparisons failed to show a difference for pre- or post-
measures of RT, SCR, or Ne (人物 2 和 3).

讨论

The present study compared the relative effects of active
and sham 10 Hz rTMS directed to the right and left DLPFC
on psychomotor processing speed and accuracy, indices
of Ne, in healthy participants performing the Sternberg
任务, a short-term verbal recognition memory paradigm
that has been shown to model the association between
WM load and task behavior (Sternberg, 1966). 以前的
studies of the role of DLPFC in WM have been designed
to identify the temporal and spatial dynamics of the com-
putational role of DLPFC in a WM network. 例如,
Osaka et al. (2007) investigated the role of executive func-
tion in human left DLPFC in eight normal participants
using low-frequency TMS after fMRI activation confirmed
a role for left DLPFC. TMS was applied to left DLPFC im-
mediately after subjects finished reading sentences from
the reading span test, a measure of verbal WM. They found

Preston et al.

451

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

t

.

/

F

.

.


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

桌子 1. Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural Efficiency of
Working Memory

Descriptive Statistics

Active

Sham

意思是

标清

意思是

标清

Pre-10 Hz rTMS SCRa

Post-10 Hz rTMS SCRa

Pre-10 Hz rTMS RTb

Post-10 Hz rTMS RTb

Pre-10 Hz rTMS NEc

Post-10 Hz rTMS NEc

意思是 % Change NEc

0.920

0.870

1.117

0.874

0.832

0.995

1.200

0.07

0.970

0.03

0.06

0.860

0.05

0.08

1.010

0.10

0.05

1.014

0.11

0.17

0.969

0.21

0.13

0.866

0.20

0.30

0.900

0.10

Repeated Measures ANOVA

F (df = 1, 30)

说.

Pre-10 Hz rTMS SCRa

Post-10 Hz rTMS SCRa

Pre-10 Hz rTMS RTb

Post-10 Hz rTMS RTb

Pre-10 Hz rTMS NEc

Post-10 Hz rTMS NEc

0.019

0.012

3.170

7.380

1.628

5.344

意思是 % Change NEc

14.253

.892

.915

.086

.011

.212

.028

.001

aAccuracy.
bReaction time.
cNeural efficiency.

a significant deterioration of performance in this WM task,
因此, supporting a crucial role for DLPFC in WM. 科赫
等人. (2005) used an rTMS approach to investigate the
temporal and spatial dynamics of the parieto-frontal net-
work in normal subjects performing a spatial WM task.
They aimed to compare neural activity in the relevant
areas during the delay and decision phases of the task.
Trains of rTMS at 25 Hz were directed to PPC and right
DLPFC during the two phases. Premotor cortex was used
as a spatial control. They observed that TMS interfered
with performance during the delay phase in the posterior
parietal and DLPFC sites. There was no effect for the con-
trol site. When rTMS trains were applied during the re-
trieval phase, interference was observed only in DLPFC.
The authors concluded that their study identified an exam-
ple of parallel processing in the parieto-frontal network of
spatial WM during the delay phase. The results of Koch
等人. represent additional evidence of the critical role of
DLPFC during both the delay and the retrieval phases of
a WM task. Mottaghy, Gangitano, 克劳斯, and Pascual-
莱昂内 (2003) observed significant interference with WM

RT after 180 msec of left PFC stimulation. Results in a simi-
lar vein were observed by other researchers (Postle et al.,
2006; Mull & Seyal, 2001; Mottaghy et al., 2000).

The present study was designed, 部分地, to identify a
putative role for DLPFC in the Ne of a WM task. Accuracy
(SCR) declined to a similar extent in both groups, likely
due to the effects of memory load, hence supporting the
conclusion of previous studies that also found no effect
of TMS on WM task accuracy (Luber et al., 2007, 2008;
德斯蒙德, 陈, & Shieh, 2005). 反过来, such findings
suggest that the effect of 10 Hz rTMS on RT when di-
rected to DLPFC 10 sec prior to task onset mediates the
enhancement of Ne. 此外, these data suggest that
the retrieval phase of this WM task may be the temporal
boundary of the observed effects.

Results of the present study are in line with Rypma
等人. (2006), who set out to identify the neural bases of
interindividual differences in cognitive performance. 他们
had participants perform a simple speed of processing task
during fMRI scanning. In certain prefrontal cortical regions
(全氟碳化物), faster performers used fewer neural resources than
slower performers, whereas in other PFC and parietal
地区, they used more. These results were interpreted
to suggest that a critical determinant of interindividual
differences in cognitive performance was the efficiency of
interactions between brain regions. In order to perform
出色地, slower individuals may utilize more prefrontal re-
sources than faster individuals (Rypma et al., 2006).

Taking into account the lack of difference between
groups in baseline RT (桌子 1), together with a robust
RT enhancement with active stimulation, and consider-
ing that participants in both groups had equal practice ex-
perience over the course of experiment, 一个可能
plausibly argue that our data support the conclusion that
the poststimulation difference in task performance be-
tween groups is best explained as a difference between ac-
tive and sham stimulation (桌子 3). 即使是这样, we recognize
valid challenges to these conclusions. Our results were

桌子 2. Pairwise Comparisons: Effects of 10 Hz rTMS Pre- 和
Post-10 Hz rTMS

t

p ( Two-Tailed)

Active

Pre vs. Post SCR

Pre vs. Post RT

Pre vs. Post Efficiency

Sham

Pre vs. Post SCR

Pre vs. Post RT

Pre vs. Post Efficiency

2.362

4.417

−4.993

3.113

−1.264

0.986

.0340

.0010

.0002

.008

.228

.342

452

认知神经科学杂志

体积 22, 数字 3

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

/

t

.

.

F


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

with Klimesch et al. (2003), who showed that mean peak
alpha spectral frequency (IAF) is related to memory per-
formance. Klimesch et al. showed that compared with at-
tentional demands, memory performance exerted the
strongest effect on IAF. The difference in IAF between
good and bad memory performers peaked when subjects
retrieved information from memory. During retrieval, 这
IAF of good performers was 1.25 Hz higher than that of
bad performers.

Brignani, Manganotti, Rossini, and Miniussi (2008) 成立
that low-frequency TMS over primary motor cortex in-
duced a synchronization of the background oscillatory
activity in the stimulated region, hence suggesting that
TMS effects may involve short-term modification of the
neural circuitry sustaining motor behavior (Brignani et al.,
2008; Leiberg, Lutzenberger, & Kaiser, 2006; Klimesch,
1997). Emergent evidence suggests that a diminished
capacity to synchronize distributed neural assemblies me-
diating WM may be a critical and enduring underlying
mechanism of impaired WM in a broad array of seemingly
disparate neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophre-
尼亚 (Ford, Krystal, & Mathalon, 2007; Andreasen et al.,
1999; 弗里斯顿, 1999), depression (林肯卡尔-汉森
等人。, 2005), Alzheimerʼs disease (Yener, Güntekin, Oniz,
& Basar, 2007; Osipova, Ahveninen, 詹森, Ylikoski, &
Pekkonen, 2005), and autism (Uhlhaas & 歌手, 2007). 的-
spite such recognition, there is no treatment for impaired
memory in any such disorder. TMS has been shown to
mediate events at the neural synapse (Fuggetta, 孔雀,
Fiaschi, & Manganotti, 2008). In light of previous work, 这
suggests that such effects may be related to 10 Hz rTMS in-
duced phase-state perturbation (Anderson et al., 2007); 如果
所以, it may also suggest a role for TMS as a clinical tool for

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

F

.

/

.

t


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

数字 3. Lack of effect of 10 Hz rTMS on accuracy within or between
subjects. The difference in accuracy within and between groups pre-
and poststimulation was not significant.

Preston et al.

453

数字 2. Comparative effects of active and sham 10 Hz rTMS on
working memory reaction time. Left-side sequence: Pre-10 Hz rTMS
RT*; Post-10 Hz rTMS RT; RT difference. Right-side sequence: RT
difference; Pre-10 Hz rTMS RT*; Post-10 Hz rTMS RT. 比较的
with sham, the group treated with active 10 Hz rTMS applied to DLPFC
10 sec before onset experienced a 219-msec enhancement of RT.

specific to SCR, RT, and Ne; even so it is possible that the
effects were due to a priming effect of rTMS on psychomo-
tor functions that are also mediated through prefrontal net-
作品 (Rounis, Yarrow, & Rothwell, 2007; Vogt et al., 2007).
Although we would suggest that it is unlikely the observed
effects are strictly due to learning, inasmuch as both the
sham and active rTMS groups had equal opportunity to
实践, we recognize there may have been an interaction
之间 10 Hz rTMS and the learning of a response strat-
egy that results in faster but not more accurate responses.
An additional challenge to our conclusions is related to the
capacity limits of short-term memory (Marois & Ivanoff,
2005; 考恩, 2001). To overcome such limitations, 学习
subjects may devise strategies, such as chunking to suc-
cessfully perform at supra-capacity limits, which can affect
neural activity in a complex way, leading to potential com-
plications in the interpretation of results. In view of the
high-capacity nature of the task we used in this study, 这是
possible that chunking or other mnemonic strategies may
have been used to manage supra-capacity aspects of task
表现.

The underlying neurobiological mechanisms subserving
the effects of TMS on WM Ne are unclear. In a previous ex-
ploratory study, the present authors found that 10 Hz rTMS
applied to DLPFC in healthy humans resulted in a signifi-
cantly enhanced measure of individual mean peak alpha
频率, and enhanced within band 10–12 Hz phase syn-
chrony (Anderson et al., 2007). These results are in line

桌子 3. Pairwise Comparisons of Reaction Time × Memory
Load for Each Group

团体

RT (我)

RT (J)

Difference (I−J)

意思是

Active

5-item 6-item

8-item

7-item 6-item

8-item

Sham

5-item 6-item

8-item

7-item 6-item

8-item

0.278

0.252

0.167

0.141

−0.046

−0.101

0.098

0.052

SEM

0.07

0.07

0.03

0.03

说.

.0060

.0170

.0001

.0005

0.03

1.0000

0.04

0.04

.1930

.1450

0.05

1.0000

In the actively stimulated group, the 5-item prestimulation RT is slower
比 6- and 8-item RT. The same relationship is true for comparison
的 7- with the 8-item RT; in the sham group, 这 6- and 8-item RTs
are slower than the 5-item RT, as would be expected.

neural circuit repair or enhancement (Plewnia et al., 2008;
Sauseng & Klimesch, 2008). In a study with 24 subjects,
Hamidi et al. (2009) used high-frequency rTMS to evaluate
the role of DLPFC in memory-guided response to two dif-
ferent types of spatial WM tasks: one requiring a recognition
decision about a probe stimulus (operationalized with a
yes/no button press), and another requiring direct recall
of the memory stimulus by moving a cursor to the remem-
bered location. In half the trials, randomly distributed, rTMS
was applied to DLPFC, and in a separate session, to the SPL,
a brain area implicated in spatial WM storage. A 10-Hz (3 秒,
110% of motor threshold) train of rTMS was delivered at the
onset of the response period. They found that only rTMS
applied to DLPFC significantly affected performance. 什么时候
10 Hz rTMS was directed to right DLPFC, accuracy declined
on delayed-recall trials. 什么时候 10 Hz rTMS was directed to
left DLPFC, accuracy on the delayed-recognition trials was en-
hanced. These findings support DLPFCʼs role in memory-
guided response and suggest that the nature of this role
varies depending on the processes required for making a
response. Koch et al. (2005) used an rTMS approach to in-
vestigate the temporal dynamics and reciprocal interactions
of different regions of the parieto-frontal network in normal
subjects performing a spatial WM task. They aimed to com-
pare neural activity in these regions in the delay and deci-
sion phases of the task. Trains of rTMS at 25 Hz were
delivered over PPC, premotor cortex (SFG), and DLPFC
of the right hemisphere alternatively during the two phases.
They observed TMS interference during the delay phase for
PPC and DLPFC sites of stimulation, with no effect on the
control site. When rTMS trains were applied during the re-
trieval phase, task interference was limited to DLPFC. 这
authors concluded their study supported the existence of
a parallel processing network that was active during the
delay phase of the task. They concluded that in DLPFC,

two task-specific networks coexisted: a local neural network
subserving the decisional processes, and a second neural
population functionally connected to PPC that was activated
when spatial-specific information maintained in memory
was made available for use.

Luber et al. (2007) designed a delayed match-to-sample
task in which rTMS at 1, 5, 或者 20 Hz was applied to either
left DLPFC or midline parietal cortex during the delay
phase of the task. 仅有的 5 Hz stimulation to the parietal site
resulted in a significant decrease in RT with no effect on
SCR. In a second experiment, 5 Hz rTMS was directed to
the parietal site during either the maintenance or retrieval
phase of the recognition probe. RT enhancement occurred
in the retention phase but not the probe phase. They con-
cluded their results suggest that 5 Hz rTMS may improve
WM performance when stimulation of a specific frequency
was timed to a specific phase of task performance.

Earlier work to show that TMS may enhance prefrontal
oscillatory synchrony in spectral bands subserving WM in-
formation processing (Anderson et al., 2007), as noted
多于, lends support to the idea of developing a neural
circuit-based model for the treatment of memory impair-
ment using noninvasive brain stimulation (NIBS) 方法
such as TMS. Presently, evidence to support an NIBS-based
approach is supported by work to show improved clinical
status in depression (OʼReardon et al., 2007), hallucina-
tions in schizophrenia (Aleman, 索默, & Kahn, 2007;
Hoffman et al., 2005), and posttraumatic stress disorder
(Osuch et al., 2009; Grisaru, Amir, 科恩, & 卡普兰, 1998).

致谢
This study was supported by the NARSAD Young Investigator
奖 (to G. P。), the L. C. Tanner Family Foundation (to G. P。),
and Intramural NINDS, DARPA (to E. W.). National Science Foun-
日期 (IIS-0513692 to C. S。, CCF-0401498 to C. S。, EIA-0323604 到
C. S。, CNS-0514485 to C. S。, IIS-0534628 to C. S。, CNS-0528201 到
C. S。, OISE-0405402 to C. S。); 能源部 (to C. S。);
IBM Faculty Award (to C. S。).

Reprint requests should be sent to Dr. Gilbert Preston, Depart-
ment of Psychiatry, University of Utah School of Medicine, 30
北 1900 East, Salt Lake City, 犹他州 84132-2502, or via e-mail:
gilbert.preston@hsc.utah.edu or gilbert.preston@med.va.gov.

参考

Abler, B., Walter, H。, Wunderlich, A。, Grothe, J。, Schonfeldt-
Lecuona, C。, 斯皮策, M。, 等人. (2005). Side effects of
transcranial magnetic stimulation biased task performance
in a cognitive neuroscience study. Brain Topography, 17,
193–196.

Aleman, A。, 索默, 我. E., & Kahn, 右. S. (2007). Efficacy of
slow repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the
treatment of resistant auditory hallucinations in
schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 68, 416–421.

Altamura, M。, Elvevag, B., Blasi, G。, Bertolino, A。, Callicott, J. H。,
温伯格, D. R。, 等人. (2007). Dissociating the effects of
Sternberg working memory demands in prefrontal cortex.
Psychiatry Research, 154, 103–114.

454

认知神经科学杂志

体积 22, 数字 3

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

F

t

/

.

.

.


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

安德森, 乙. W., 普雷斯顿, G. A。, & 席尔瓦, C. 时间. (2007).

Towards development of a circuit based treatment for
impaired memory: A multidisciplinary approach. 会议记录
of the 3rd International IEEE EMBS Conference on
Neural Engineering (PP. 302–305).

Andreasen, 氮. C。, Nopoulos, P。, OʼLeary, D. S。, 磨坊主, D. D .,
Wassink, T。, & Flaum, 中号. (1999). Defining the phenotype
精神分裂症的: Cognitive dysmetria and its neural
mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 908–920.

Boroojerdi, B., Phipps, M。, Kopylev, L。, Wharton, C. M。, 科恩,
L. G。, & Grafman, J. (2001). Enhancing analogic reasoning
with rTMS over the left prefrontal cortex. Neurology, 56,
526–528.

Brandt, S. A。, Ploner, C. J。, 迈耶, 乙. U。, Leistner, S。, &
狂野的召唤者, A. (1998). Effects of repetitive transcranial
magnetic stimulation over dorsolateral prefrontal and
posterior parietal cortex on memory-guided saccades.
Experimental Brain Research, 118, 197–204.

Brignani, D ., Manganotti, P。, Rossini, 磷. M。, & Miniussi, C.
(2008). Modulation of cortical oscillatory activity during
transcranial magnetic stimulation. 人脑图谱,
29, 603–612.

考恩, 氮. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term

记忆: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.
Behavioural Brain Science, 24, 87–114; 讨论
114–185.

德斯蒙德, J. E., 陈, S. H。, & Shieh, 磷. 乙. (2005). Cerebellar
transcranial magnetic stimulation impairs verbal working
记忆. Annals of Neurology, 58, 553–560.

DʼEsposito, M。, Postle, 乙. R。, & Rypma, 乙. (2000). Prefrontal

cortical contributions to working memory: 证据
from event-related fMRI studies. Experimental Brain
研究, 133, 3–11.

Duncan, J。, & 欧文, A. 中号. (2000). Common regions of
the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive
需要. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 475–483.

Ford, J. M。, Krystal, J. H。, & Mathalon, D. H. (2007). Neural

synchrony in schizophrenia: From networks to new
treatments. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 848–852.

弗里斯顿, K. J. (1999). Schizophrenia and the disconnection

假设. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplementum,
395, 68–79.

Fuggetta, G。, 孔雀, 乙. F。, Fiaschi, A。, & Manganotti, 磷. (2008).
Acute modulation of cortical oscillatory activities during
short trains of high-frequency repetitive transcranial
magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex: A
combined EEG and TMS study. 人脑图谱,
29, 1–13.

Goel, 五、, & Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal lobes implicated
in “planning” functions? Interpreting data from the Tower
of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia, 33, 623–642.

古尔德, 右. L。, 棕色的, 右. G。, 欧文, A. M。, ffytche, D. H。, &
霍华德, 右. J. (2003). fMRI BOLD response to increasing
task difficulty during successful paired associates learning.
Neuroimage, 20, 1006–1019.

Grisaru, N。, Amir, M。, 科恩, H。, & 卡普兰, Z. (1998). Effect
of transcranial magnetic stimulation in posttraumatic stress
紊乱: A preliminary study. Biological Psychiatry,
44, 52–55.

Hamidi, M。, 托诺尼, G。, & Postle, 乙. 右. (2009). Evaluating the
role of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided
response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.
Neuropsychologia, 47, 295–302.

Hoffman, 右. E., Gueorguieva, R。, Hawkins, K. A。, Varanko, M。,
Boutros, 氮. N。, 吴, 是. T。, 等人. (2005). Temporoparietal
transcranial magnetic stimulation for auditory hallucinations:
Safety, efficacy and moderators in a fifty patient sample.
Biological Psychiatry, 58, 97–104.

Klimesch, 瓦. (1997). EEG-alpha rhythms and memory processes.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 26, 319–340.
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P。, & 格尔洛夫, C. (2003). Enhancing

cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation at human individual alpha frequency.
欧洲神经科学杂志, 17, 1129–1133.

科赫, G。, Oliveri, M。, Torriero, S。, Carlesimo, G. A。, Turriziani,
P。, & Caltagirone, C. (2005). rTMS evidence of different
delay and decision processes in a fronto-parietal neuronal
network activated during spatial working memory.
Neuroimage, 24, 34–39.

Kohler, S。, Paus, T。, 巴克纳, 右. L。, & Milner, 乙. (2004). Effects
of left inferior prefrontal stimulation on episodic memory
形成: A two-stage fMRI–rTMS study. 杂志
Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 178–188.

Leiberg, S。, Lutzenberger, W., & Kaiser, J. (2006). Effects of

memory load on cortical oscillatory activity during auditory
pattern working memory. 脑研究, 1120, 131–140.
林肯卡尔-汉森, K., 数量, S。, Rytsälä, H。, Suominen, K.,

Isometsä, E., & Kähkönen, S. (2005). Breakdown of
long-range temporal correlations in theta oscillations in
patients with major depressive disorder. 杂志
神经科学, 25, 10131–10137.

Luber, B., Kinnunen, L. H。, Rakitin, 乙. C。, Ellsasser, R。, Stern, Y。,

& Lisanby, S. H. (2007). Facilitation of performance in
a working memory task with rTMS stimulation of the
precuneus: Frequency- and time-dependent effects.
脑研究, 1128, 120–129.

Luber, B., 斯坦福大学, A. D ., Bulow, P。, 阮, T。, Rakitin, 乙. C。,
Habeck, C。, 等人. (2008). Remediation of sleep-deprivation-
induced working memory impairment with fMRI-guided
transcranial magnetic stimulation. 大脑皮层,
18, 2077–2085.

Marois, R。, & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information
processing in the brain. 认知科学的趋势, 9,
296–305.

Mottaghy, F. M。, Gangitano, M。, 克劳斯, 乙. J。, & Pascual-Leone, A.
(2003). Chronometry of parietal and prefrontal activations
in verbal working memory revealed by transcranial magnetic
stimulation. Neuroimage, 18, 565–575.

Mottaghy, F. M。, 克劳斯, 乙. J。, Kemna, L. J。, Töpper, R。,

Tellmann, L。, Beu, M。, 等人. (2000). Modulation of the
neuronal circuitry subserving working memory in healthy
human subjects by repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation. Neuroscience Letters, 280, 167–170.

Mottaghy, F. M。, Sparing, R。, & Topper, 右. (2006). Enhancing
picture naming with transcranial magnetic stimulation.
Behavioural Neurology, 17, 177–186.

Mull, 乙. R。, & Seyal, 中号. (2001). Transcranial magnetic
stimulation of left prefrontal cortex impairs working
记忆. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 1672–1675.

OʼReardon, J. P。, Solvason, H. B., Janicak, 磷. G。, 桑普森, S。,
Isenberg, K. E., Nahas, Z。, 等人. (2007). Efficacy and safety
of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the acute treatment
of major depression: A multisite randomized controlled
审判. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 1208–1216.

大阪, N。, Otsuka, Y。, Hirose, N。, Ikeda, T。, Mima, T。, 福山,
H。, 等人. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
applied to left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex disrupts verbal
working memory performance in humans. 神经科学
信件, 418, 232–235.

Osipova, D ., Ahveninen, J。, 詹森, 奥。, Ylikoski, A。, & Pekkonen,
乙. (2005). Altered generation of spontaneous oscillations
in Alzheimerʼs disease. Neuroimage, 27, 835–841.

Osuch, 乙. A。, 本森, 乙. E., Luckenbaugh, D. A。, Geraci, M。,

邮政, 右. M。, & McCann, U. (2008). Repetitive TMS combined
with exposure therapy for PTSD: A preliminary study.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 54–59.

Preston et al.

455

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

t

.

.

F

.

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

Plewnia, C。, Rilk, A. J。, Soekadar, S. R。, 阿费勒, C。, Huber, H. S。,
Sauseng, P。, 等人. (2008). Enhancement of long-range EEG
coherence by synchronous bifocal transcranial magnetic
stimulation. 欧洲神经科学杂志, 27,
1577–1583.

Postle, 乙. R。, Ferrarelli, F。, Hamidi, M。, Feredoes, E., Massimini,

M。, 彼得森, M。, 等人. (2006). Repetitive transcranial
magnetic stimulation dissociates working memory
manipulation from retention functions in the prefrontal,
but not posterior parietal, cortex. 认知杂志
神经科学, 18, 1712–1722.

Rounis, E., Yarrow, K., & Rothwell, J. C. (2007). Effects of
rTMS conditioning over the fronto-parietal network on
motor versus visual attention. 认知杂志
神经科学, 19, 513–524.

Rypma, B., Berger, J. S。, & DʼEsposito, 中号. (2002). The influence
of working-memory demand and subject performance on
prefrontal cortical activity. 认知杂志
神经科学, 14, 721–731.

Rypma, B., Berger, J. S。, Genova, H. M。, Rebbechi, D ., &

DʼEsposito, 中号. (2005). Dissociating age-related changes
in cognitive strategy and neural efficiency using
event-related fMRI. Cortex, 41, 582–594.

Rypma, B., Berger, J. S。, Prabhakaran, 五、, Bly, 乙. M。, Kimberg,

D. Y。, Biswal, 乙. B., 等人. (2006). Neural correlates of
cognitive efficiency. Neuroimage, 33, 969–979.

Rypma, B., Prabhakaran, 五、, 德斯蒙德, J. E., & Gabrieli, J. D.
(2001). Age differences in prefrontal cortical activity in
工作记忆. Psychology and Aging, 16, 371–384.

Sauseng, P。, & Klimesch, 瓦. (2008). What does phase
information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about
认知过程? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
评论, 32, 1001–1013.

Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human

记忆. 科学, 153, 652–654.

Uhlhaas, 磷. J。, & 歌手, 瓦. (2007). What do disturbances
in neural synchrony tell us about autism? Biological
Psychiatry, 62, 190–191.

Vogt, S。, Buccino, G。, 沃尔施拉格, A. M。, Canessa, N。, Shah,

氮. J。, Zilles, K., 等人. (2007). Prefrontal involvement in
imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice
and expertise. Neuroimage, 37, 1371–1383.

Wassermann, 乙. 中号. (1998). Risk and safety of repetitive

transcranial magnetic stimulation: Report and suggested
guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety
of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5–7,
1996. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology,
108, 1–16.

Yener, G. G。, Güntekin, B., Oniz, A。, & Basar, 乙. (2007). 增加
frontal phase-locking of event-related theta oscillations in
Alzheimer patients treated with cholinesterase inhibitors.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64, 46–52.

D

w
n


A
d
e
d

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/


t
.

:
/
/

F
r


D
H

t
w
t
n
p

:
A
/
d
/
e

d

F
t
r

p

r
C
H
.
s
p


d
v

e
r
e
r
C
C
t
.
H

A

r
e
.
d

C



C
/
n
j
A

r
C
t

n
C
/
e
A

p
r
d
t

2
C
2

3
e

4
p
4
d
7
F
1
/
9
2
3
2
8
/
4
3
6
/
1
4

4
C
7
n
/
1
2
0
7
0
6
9
9
2
2
8
1
7
2
0
/
9
j

p
C
d
n
.

2
y
0
G
0

9
e
.
s
t
2

1
n
2
0
0
9
8
.
S
p
e
d
p
F
e


y

e
G
r

2
e
0
2
s
3
t

/
j

.

.

F

.

t

/


n

1
8

中号
A
y

2
0
2
1

456

认知神经科学杂志

体积 22, 数字 3Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural image
Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural image
Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural image
Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural image
Effects of 10 Hz rTMS on the Neural image

下载pdf