Opening Questions in Visual Working Memory
Anna C. Nobre
Abstracto
■ In this reflective piece on visual working memory, I depart
from the laboriously honed skills of writing a review. Instead of
integrating approaches, synthesizing evidence, and building a
cohesive perspective, I scratch my head and share niggles and
puzzlements. I expose where my scholarship and understanding
are stumped by findings and standard views in the literature. ■
The act of self-exposure on paper is unnerving. Sin embargo,
confronting ignorance is a necessary and important step
for scientific advancement. In private or, even better, en
the good company of a close colleague, surfacing and shar-
ing bafflements can become the most rewarding source of
creative energy. Para mí, many new experimental designs,
methods, and conceptualizations were born from open-
ended conversations with kindred science friends, com-
paring and discussing points of contention and confusion.
I have been lucky to enjoy the friendship of some brilliant,
curious, generous, and unpretentious collaborators. Marca
Stokes stands out, playing a vital role in shaping and
unshaping my ideas and methods. I offer this soliloquy
as a snapshot of what I might bring to such conversations.
I do so in the spirit of spreading the courage and fun of fac-
ing and respecting ignorance before building knowledge.
Science shows humankind at its best. By building on evi-
dence with curiosity, ingenuity, logic, and years of prepa-
ration, scientists expose the nature of matter, vida, y
mind with increasing sophistication and dazzling discover-
es. Todavía, as a human endeavor, science cannot escape our
cognitive limitations or the social and political dimensions
of our behavior. De este modo, experimental paradigms1 develop
not only for the quality of the methods and the robustness
of the results that ground models and theories. They also
reflect the technologies of the times, the influence of
powerful and charismatic individuals, clannish allegiances
driving competition and cooperation, the catchiness of
simplistic and categorical ideas, the lure of the novel,
and an occasional disregard for earlier foundational dis-
coveries. Clear dichotomies and intuitive models that
appeal to folk psychology often have an edge over better
nuanced and integrative views.
Science progresses nonetheless, although not without
occasionally derailing, reinventing, or getting stuck in local
minima. Promoting progress takes seeing through the
Universidad de Oxford
standard accepted narratives and models in one’s field
and looking anew with scholarship and a beginner’s
mindset.
DEFINING VISUAL WORKING MEMORY
Working memory is a core psychological construct essen-
tial for guiding flexible and adaptive human behavior. El
term working memory was probably first introduced by
Molinero, Galanter, and Pribram (1960) to refer to a quick-
access form of memory for executing plans. In their pro-
posal, uncompleted parts of plans in working memory
comprise intentions. Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) también
used the term in their seminal theoretical treatment of
memory and its control processes. In their proposal, el
short-term memory store makes up the person’s working
memory, receiving selected inputs from a brief sensory
register and the long-term memory store. Flexible and
volitional control processes operate in the short-term
almacenar, helping to support storage, buscar, and retrieval.
Both accounts echo the information-processing paradigm
inspired by advances in communication and computer
technologies during the inception of modern cognitive
psicología (Neisser, 1967). Baddeley and Hitch (1974)
later used the term to describe a specific, multicomponent
model of short-term maintenance and manipulation of
visual and verbal material. This highly popular model
and its various alterations (Baddeley, Hitch, & allen,
2019; Baddeley, 2012) have heavily shaped research, especialmente-
cially in the verbal domain.
Actualmente, there is some confusion and disagreement
concerning how the term “working memory” should be
usado. Some scholars reserve it for the multicomponent
modelo (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974); others use it more gen-
erally but contend it implies necessarily more than just
temporary storage, requiring the manipulation of contents
and/or conscious access. This piece adopts a more basic,
bare-bones definition: the temporary storage of visual
contents to guide behavior. The simple definition
© 2022 Instituto de Tecnología de Massachusetts. Published under a
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Internacional (CC POR 4.0) licencia.
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva 35:1, páginas. 49–59
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01920
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
preserves the essential role in guiding future behavior
from the original definition (Miller et al., 1960) and the sep-
aration between mnemonic content and control processes
operating upon them (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968).
UPDATING THE STANDARD PARADIGM
Until recently, most research on visual working memory
examined its representational aspects—the nature of its
contents and the related psychological and neural mecha-
nisms supporting storage. The standard view was that
visual working memory holds contents in a robust format,
and these are accessed through an inflexible process of
exhaustive search (Sternberg, 1966; Sperling, 1960).
Capacity is severely limited, and dichotomies arose to
explain the limitations. Polar positions propose that
resources are distributed evenly across the contents to
be memorized (Mamá, Husain, & Laureles, 2014; Laureles & Husain,
2008) versus that a fixed number of slots are available for
contents to occupy (zhang & Luck, 2008). Another related
dichotomy concerns whether there is only one item in the
focus of visual working memory (Oberauer, 2002) or the
full set of items within the capacity limitations (Cowan,
2010). The primary mechanism proposed for maintaining
contents in visual working memory is persistent “delay”
activity in neuronal populations signaling attributes of
memorized items in prefrontal and sensory areas (Kojima
& Goldman-Rakic, 1982; Fuster & Alexander, 1971; Kubota
& Niki, 1971). The delay activity during working memory is
considered a major top–down source for selective atten-
ción, guiding sensory processing of task-relevant items
(Desimone & Duncan, 1995). The focusing of perception
by working memory thus feeds a virtuous cycle, al final
helping encode relevant memory content, which in turn
guides sensory processing (Nobre & alimenta, 2011).
Current research is breaking from the standard visual
working-memory paradigm and upgrading our under-
standing in various ways.
Work in my group has promoted the realization that
visual working memory is much more flexible than previ-
ously envisaged, with selective attention continuing to
operate within visual working memory to prioritize main-
tenance, selección, and access to mnemonic contents (camioneta
Ede & Nobre, 2023; Nobre & alimenta, 2019). Initial studies
revealed that retroactive attention cues (retrocues) durante
the maintenance period indicating the memory content
that is relevant for the upcoming task confer significant
performance benefits (Griffin & Nobre, 2003; Landman,
Spekreijse, & Lamme, 2003). Subsequent studies added
observations of selective attention modulating working
memory based on changing internal states in the absence
of cueing stimuli (van Ede, Niklaus, & Nobre, 2017), sen-
sory capture by external stimuli sharing features with
working-memory content (van Ede, Board, & Nobre,
2020), and intended actions prioritizing congruent items
(Heuer, Ohl, & Rolfs, 2020). Selective attention can mod-
ulate feature-based (Niklaus, Nobre, & van Ede, 2017; S.M,
Hu, Ristaniemi, Gendron, & Liu, 2016) and object-based
(lin, kong, & Fougnie, 2021; Peters, Kaiser, Rahm, &
Bledowski, 2015) information in visual working memory
(Hajonides, van Ede, alimenta, & Nobre, 2020), with other
possible substrates still to be tested. Curiosamente, selec-
tive attention in visual working memory is also revers-
ible, such that priorities can shift flexibly among contents
without obligatory trade-offs in their quality or accessibility
(Zokaei, Board, Manohar, & Nobre, 2019; miers,
Chekroud, alimenta, & Nobre, 2018; van Ede et al., 2017;
Rerko & Oberauer, 2013; Lewis-Peacock, Drysdale,
Oberauer, & Postle, 2012).
In addition to dispelling the notion of an inflexible store,
the growing literature on selective attention in visual work-
ing memory also challenges other standard views and
opens new questions. The ability to juggle selective atten-
tion among contents reversibly argues against strong pro-
posals of fixed resource allocation within capacity limits
(miers, alimenta, & Nobre, 2017). The ability to modulate
features and object-level representations questions the
nature of unitary entities for slots. Reconsidering such
concepts as “resources” and “slots” also highlights their
underspecified nature, which compromises their utility
as theoretical tools. The influence of intended actions
on setting internal attention reminds us of the ecological
purpose of visual working memory. Whereas emphasis
has traditionally been placed on representational proper-
corbatas, the most important aspect of visual working memory
is its function of preparing for future behavior (van Ede,
2020; Myers et al., 2017; see also Fuster & Alexander,
1971). Respectivamente, studies re-introducing action links to
visual stimuli in working-memory tasks have underlined
the major role of action readiness (Boettcher, Gresch,
Nobre, & van Ede, 2021; van Ede, Chekroud, alimenta, &
Nobre, 2019; Schneider, Barth, & Wascher, 2017).
The paradigm is thereby shifting from visual working
memory as an inflexible representational state that is
severely limited in capacity to a highly flexible pragmatic
functional state that adaptively prepares the individual
for potential futures and guides action (van Ede & Nobre,
2023). Sin embargo, this updating of views deepens old ques-
tions and opens new ones. Much effort lies ahead before a
full understanding emerges of how signals derived from
experience persist to constitute structured content and
prepare possible actions; how control processes, incluir-
ing but not limited to selective attention, interact with
such traces; and how they relate to long-term traces, ambos
pre-existing long-term memories that scaffold perception
and those yet to form based on the recent sensory input.
Addressing these puzzles will ultimately require looking
under the hood at the changing patterns, dinámica, y
transformations of activity in neurons and networks.
NEURAL TRACES
The dissociation between working memory and long-term
memory started as a theoretical one. James (1890), para
50
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva
Volumen 35, Número 1
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
ejemplo, contrasted primary memory (working memory),
as never cut off in consciousness from the present
moment, with secondary memory (long-term memory),
as the knowledge of a former state already dropped from
consciousness. The case of patient HM (Scoville & Milner,
1957) provided a striking neuropsychological manifesta-
tion of the distinction. Whereas HM had profound epi-
sodic amnesia, he was nevertheless able to maintain
information in mind over short periods if not interrupted,
using elaborate strategies if required (Milner, Corkin, &
Teuber, 1968).
Todavía, the nature of the relation between working mem-
ory and long-term memory is still unresolved. Some of
the initial neuropsychological evidence that grounded the-
ories proposing a strong separation between their neural
systems may have been overinterpreted (p.ej., Warrington
& Shallice, 1972). Por ejemplo, these dissociations were
often based on different types of content and tasks with
varied performance demands. More recently, alguno
scholars emphasize the association between neural sys-
tems supporting working memory and long-term memory
for equivalent types of material (p.ej., Zokaei, Nour, et al.,
2019; Pertzov et al., 2013; graham, Barense, & Sotavento, 2010;
Olson, moore, Rígido, & Chatterjee, 2006). Sin embargo, even if
the memory systems are co-extensive, the neural pro-
cesses supporting information maintenance over short
and long spans are likely to differ, given the time required
for cementing the structural changes involved in enduring
plasticity.
Curiosamente, early theoretical models are often misin-
terpreted as proposing a strong division between the
memory systems, whereas they did “not require that the
two stores necessarily be in different parts of the brain
or involve different physiological structures” (Atkinson &
Shiffrin, 1971). My own working hypothesis is that overlap-
ping brain areas are involved in storing contents over short
and long timespans and that multiple functions operate
upon these traces to facilitate performance based on work-
ing memory or long-term memory (p.ej., selecting, no-
grating, individuating, relating, or organizing contents).
Sin embargo, I have not completely ruled out the possibility
that some brain structure(s) may be specifically recruited
for an adaptive temporary transient store for task-relevant
contenido (see Xu, 2017; Duncan, 2001).
A clear theoretical proposal for distinct processes to
support traces over the short and long term in the brain
was introduced by Hebb (1949). He suggested that lin-
gering reverberatory activity within cell assemblies was
sufficient for short-term storage, whereas enduring
strengthening of functional connections among neurons
with correlated activity (through growth process or met-
abolic change) supported long-term storage.
Kubota & Niki, 1971) and then in many other brain regions
(see Christophel, Klink, Spitzer, Roelfsema, & Haynes,
2017) provided a simple and intuitive solution for
working-memory maintenance. The idea stuck and has
become the standard textbook explanation for working-
memory storage.
There are many appealing aspects to the explanation.
Persistent neuronal firing yields a simple and effective attrac-
tor state for models of working-memory maintenance
(Wang, 2001). Persistent activity can also be measured with
noninvasive human imaging (Curtis & D'Esposito, 2003) y
neurophysiological (Vogel & Machizawa, 2004) methods.
The strength of delay activity correlates with working-
memory performance variables (p.ej., Adán, Robison, &
Vogel, 2018; Rypma, Berger, & D'Esposito, 2002).
Sin embargo, there are also bothersome niggles. I first
remember focusing on these with Mark Stokes when we
confronted how a stimulus presented during the
working-memory delay impaired the contralateral-delay
activity marker of sustained activity in ERPs in an ongoing
experimento. The observation cast our mind to how single-
unit delay activity in sensory areas is interrupted by inter-
vening stimuli (Molinero, Erickson, & Desimone, 1996) y
how sustained firing in the pFC is interrupted in dual-task
contextos (p.ej., Watanabe & Funahashi, 2014). Examining
the time courses of neuronal activity in various papers also
revealed a more dynamic picture (p.ej., Hussar & Pasternak,
2010; Fuster & Alexander, 1971). Most of the delayed-
response working-memory tasks also conflated mainte-
nance of the memoranda with anticipation of the probe,
suggesting delay activity could reflect anticipatory attention
en cambio (see Lewis-Peacock & Postle, 2012; Ikkai & Curtis,
2011; Nobre & alimenta, 2011; Lepsien & Nobre, 2007, para
similar arguments).
If persistent delay activity is not the full answer, what else
could maintain relevant neural traces in readiness for
guiding behavior? One possibility is to return to Hebb’s
more dynamic notion of reverberation in a cell assembly.
Dynamic patterns of activity reverberating across cell assem-
blies within and across brain areas could lead to more com-
plex but still efficient attractor states (Wang, 2021). Changes
in excitability in neuronal populations resulting from mem-
brane kinetics and adaptation could also contribute (see Fitz
et al., 2020). Reviewing the evidence and models at the time,
alimenta (2015) suggested that activity-dependent short-term
synaptic plasticity operating upon a spatiotemporally
dynamic neural system could provide an effective mecha-
nism for preserving patterns of neuronal signals in an
excitable functional state across working-memory delays.
Similar models had been proposed for encoding temporal
associations between events (Buonomano & Maass, 2009;
Karmarkar & Buonomano, 2007).
Persistent Delay Activity
Latent Functional States
The discovery of persistent delay-activity neurons in the
pFC (Goldman-Rakic, 1984; Fuster & Alexander, 1971;
The proposal that short-term synaptic weights silently pre-
serve functional states in neuronal assemblies that serve
Nobre
51
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
working memory was simple and elegant, but it also gen-
erated controversy and dilemmas. Controversies consid-
ered the undeniable existence of delay activity and what
specific role it served, as well as the plausibility and evi-
dence for the necessary short-term plasticity. Questions
also resurfaced about whether the mere existence of latent
functional states counts as working memory and about the
appropriate definitions for short-term versus working
memory.
The possibility of information coding in latent func-
tional states additionally provoked the interesting ques-
tion of how scientific knowledge is distorted by what we
can and cannot easily measure. Stokes’ group borrowed
tricks from sonar technology to “ping” latent states with
a high-contrast neutral visual stimulus (Wolff, Ding, miers,
& alimenta, 2015). The random stimulation interacted with
the pattern of stronger synaptic weights associated with
the recently encoded item, reactivating the latent state,
even if the item was not currently in the focus of attention.
Other studies showed that the availability of latent states
for pinging with visual or transcranial stimulation
depended on the continued task relevance of the memo-
randum ( Wolff, Jochim, Akyürek, & alimenta, 2017; Rose
et al., 2016). Además, cross-temporal decoding analyses
revealed working-memory states to be dynamic ( Wolff
et al., 2015, 2017).
The attractive silent-working-memory proposal and the
intriguing pinging results raise many interesting questions.
Experimental evidence for silent, latent representations
derives mainly from pinging studies. Sin embargo, it is impor-
tant to remember that the stimulation used for pinging can
equally well interact with active representations involving
neuronal spiking. Por lo tanto, decoding after pinging is not
direct evidence for a representation being silent.
Silent working-memory representations depend on
synaptic plasticity reinforcing a state within a dynamic sys-
tema. But what are the implications of finding dynamic pat-
terns of activity for arbitrating between silent versus active
neuronal states? The presentation of a visual stimulus nec-
essarily triggers an initial dynamic cascade of sensory pro-
cessing early in encoding. Después, spiking activity could
continue to support working memory either by settling
into a steady state or by continuing to unfold in a more
dynamic way within circuits and networks. Por lo tanto,
finding dynamic patterns of activity is not enough for
concluding that activity-silent mechanisms are sufficient
for supporting working memory.
Además, if plasticity is sufficient, what point(s) en
time should be reinforced? Would a sequential string of
states be reinforced and temporally linked? If not, son
there salient or privileged time points? Comprensión
the role of plasticity in temporally evolving brain signals
within extended experience in changing environments
can be daunting. Methods for pinpointing the timing of
cellular events that contribute to relevant states and
computations should prove particularly informative (ver
Day-Cooney, Cone, & Maunsell, 2022).
One consequence of encoding information through
short-term plasticity in a dynamical system is that the asso-
ciations are vulnerable to disruption from intervening
events, which themselves may stamp in new associations.
Cómo, por lo tanto, do such models circumvent or overcome
interference to deliver adequate working-memory perfor-
mance in dual-task or multi-stimuli contexts? Would the
strong neutral pinging stimulus itself not be expected to
disrupt the functional state of associations? En otras palabras,
is there a measurable observer effect of pinging a silent
estado, eso es, a disturbance of the representation by the
act of observation? Recovering activity following interven-
ing stimulation was one of the motivations for developing
an alternative model to persistent delay activity, but it is
not clear how plasticity naturally overcomes the limitation.
The differential decodability between potentially rele-
vant versus irrelevant content in working memory is also
misterioso. Pinging studies have shown that it is possible to
reactivate the contents of currently unattended items in
working memory ( Wolff et al., 2017; Rose et al., 2016).
Sin embargo, pinging is only effective when the unattended
item may still become relevant for subsequent task perfor-
mance. Pinging leads to significant decoding of unat-
tended items after the first of two retrocues, cuando el
unattended item may still become relevant for a subse-
quent phase of the trial. Sin embargo, pinging is ineffective
after the final retrocue, which renders the unattended
item fully irrelevant (fulvio & Postle, 2020; Wolff et al.,
2017; Rose et al., 2016). It is not obvious how changing
item relevance based on task goals can selectively disrupt
some but not other latent functional states. Is an active
process suggested to flush out the functional connectivity
state associated with the irrelevant items? How would an
active control process interact with the patterns of synap-
tic weights related to different contents within a dynamic
sistema?
Answers to some of these questions may rest in the
transformations of neural states, but what exactly is meant
by this and how transformations occur are underspecified.
Por ejemplo, focusing on an item in working memory has
been proposed to engage an output-gating process, plac-
ing the attended content in a state of action readiness
(Buen Karbe, miers, & alimenta, 2021; Panichello & Buschman,
2021; van Ede & Nobre, 2021; Myers et al., 2017; Chatham
& malo, 2015). Además, items with similar sensory
content have been proposed to occupy different states
depending on their associated task demands (Nobre &
alimenta, 2019). Multivariate and state-space analyses facili-
tate testing such proposals, but the neurophysiological
underpinnings remain elusive. Does information preserva-
tion in different states rely mainly on changing the involve-
ment of brain areas or the pattern of synaptic changes
within regions? (Cómo) Do rhythmic events at different fre-
quencies implicated in working memory play into the pro-
impuesto (p.ej., Molinero, Lundqvist, & Bastos, 2018)? Addressing
these questions will undoubtedly benefit from improving
methods in animal models to measure rapid shifts in brain
52
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva
Volumen 35, Número 1
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
states and functional connectivity (p.ej., Perrenoud et al.,
2022; Benisty et al., 2021; Marshall et al., 2021).
Short-term changes in synaptic plasticity are likely to
exist and contribute to the storage and accessibility of sig-
nals deriving from recent experience. Sin embargo, they may
not provide a full account of working-memory mainte-
maricón. Perhaps the most valuable contribution of the pro-
posal is to rouse researchers out of a complacent state of
not questioning the standard paradigm. This new alertness
to other possibilities must now endure, so that we explore
all options with an open mind and additionally consider
how different processes (p.ej., tonic sustained activity,
plasticity, adaptación, dynamical reverberation) may come
together and interact to support working-memory encod-
En g, maintenance, prioritization, and selection.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Refining Methods
A fuller understanding of working memory will benefit
from the relentless breakthroughs in measurement and
analysis methods in human neuroscience plus from the
ability to relate the research on humans to that in model
sistemas.
Advances in human neuroscience methods have trans-
formed the research agenda (see Nobre & van Ede,
2020). The sensitivity and spatiotemporal acuity of brain
imaging and neurophysiological methods have increased
continuously. Measures have shifted from group averages
to single trials. Analyses have progressed from univariate
variables related to the amount and timing of information
processing to multivariate variables that also tap into infor-
mation content and quality. Neurophysiological studies can
examine activity across the frequency spectrum, with data-
driven methods to separate frequencies that preserve the
morphology and phase of physiological signals (quinn,
Lopes-dos-Santos, Dupret, Nobre, & lana rica, 2021).
Large-scale functional networks can be studied in terms of
their activity strength, conectividad, and dynamics (quinn
et al., 2019; Brookes et al., 2011). Statistics have evolved
to include Bayesian approaches. Methods borrowed from
other disciplines help analyze and compare structures,
dinámica, and trajectories of functional networks. Compu-
tational models have increased their sophistication and
physiological validity (p.ej., Langdon & ángel, 2022).
Human neuroscience researchers should continue to
exercise creativity and ingenuity to push the boundaries
of what we can investigate. Developing the ping method
is a great example ( Wolff et al., 2015). Other possibilities
should be pursued. Por ejemplo, could invisible visual tag-
ging (Zhigalov, Herring, Herpers, Bergmann, & Jensen,
2019) include stochastic stimulation to yield information
about the timing of noninvasive neural markers causally
influencing performance (see Day-Cooney et al., 2022)?
En tono rimbombante, we should enhance and upgrade methods
for capturing behavioral signals. While skill and cleverness
have gone into measuring and analyzing brain activity, el
rich repertoire of behavioral activity has been underappre-
ciated (Nobre & van Ede, 2020). Por ejemplo, continuous
recordings of pupil diameter and microsaccades have
started yielding new insights into the dynamics of atten-
tion within working memory (van Ede et al., 2020; camioneta
Ede, Chekroud, & Nobre, 2019; Zokaei, Nour, et al.,
2019). Continuous measures of motor excitability and spa-
tiotemporal aspects of response trajectories are similarly
promising (Echeverria-Altuna et al., 2022; Dotan,
Pinheiro-Chagas, Al Roumi, & Deheane, 2019; Novembre
et al., 2019). Rather than discard external behavioral
markers correlated with psychological functions (Quax,
Dijkstra, van Staveren, Bosch, & van Gerven, 2019; Mostert
et al., 2018), we should embrace their information value to
unlock new questions.
Sin embargo, characterizing specific dynamical circuits in
the human brain may remain out of reach for current or
forthcoming noninvasive methods, either for defining
computations within microcircuits or in networks across
brain areas. The problem is challenging for tracking neu-
ronal firing or extracellular field potentials, and the chal-
lenge is magnified manifold for revealing latent functional
states in circuits or networks. Methods development in
animal models increasingly allows the individuation and
manipulation of specific circuits and networks. Para examen-
por ejemplo, mini-scope calcium imaging in rodents can reveal acti-
vation and changes in neuronal ensembles resulting from
experimental manipulations (Mau et al., 2022). Optoge-
netic methods enable the targeting of neuronal subpop-
ulaciones, and methods for genetically sequencing and
classifying neurons are developing at a staggering pace
(p.ej., BICCN, 2021; Kriene et al., 2020). Exciting innova-
tive optogenetic protocols using random (white-noise)
trains of stimulation are opening the doors for investigat-
ing the timings of critical neuronal contributions to per-
formance in tasks (Day-Cooney et al., 2022).
Integrating Research Camps
Researchers studying working memory in humans and ani-
mal models will need to work together for real progress.
Task developments and whole-brain measurements in
humans can expose principles of psychological and neural
organization to frame in-depth investigations at the net-
trabajar, circuito, and cellular levels. Sucesivamente, mechanistic dis-
coveries can prompt the search for related markers in
noninvasive measurements and test for their functional
contributions. We are far from a coordinated collaboration
in working-memory research. Alignment of work in
humans and nonhuman primates is better, although the
conceptual and task advances from human research have
been slow to percolate into non-human-primate research,
Por ejemplo, on the flexible attention functions operating
in working memory (but see Panichello & Buschman,
2021; van Ede & Nobre, 2021). In rodents, the very term
working memory carries different connotations and tasks
Nobre
53
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
employed typically diverge substantially from those in
primates.
Virtual reality provides a promising opportunity for
cross-linking research across species while also upgrad-
ing the ecological validity of experimental tasks. El
methodology allows for capturing varied continuous
behavioral and brain measures with strong experimental
control in naturalistic immersive settings (Draschkow,
2022). Virtual reality is already widely used in rodent
studies to great effect. Working-memory studies are just
starting in humans and already changing our understand-
ing of natural working memory (Draschkow, Kallmayer,
& Nobre, 2021). A propitious way forward would see
the development of closely matched experimental tasks
across species, although recognizing that no task can be
fully equated given their different evolutionary adapta-
tions and the training methods required. Analytical tools
that can describe functional states, relaciones, y
dynamics in data at various scales may help interrelate
findings.
Work within species, including humans, also needs to
proceed in a better-integrated way. Focusing on certain
brain markers is a natural tendency, but this seeds differ-
ent strands of research that can be difficult to reconcile.
In human visual working memory, Por ejemplo, diferente
sets of studies concentrate on developmental trajecto-
ries; neuropsychological dissociations; and various types
of neural measures: sustained activity, theta oscillations
and cross-frequency coupling, alpha oscillations, decod-
ing of contents, and dynamics in state-space. Are the var-
ious lines of findings compatible? Are there systematic
differences in the types of tasks associated with different
measurement types? Can we build cohesive models that
harmonize the various strands to guide future research?
It is time to remove the blinders and pull groups
together to discuss points of tension and how to resolve
a ellos.
Rifts between research subfields are even deeper and
can be similarly counterproductive. Fault lines follow folk
psychological concepts—attention, memory, decisión-
haciendo, acción, idioma, planificación, Etcétera. En realidad,
these concepts are highly interrelated. One day, we may
reach a very different description of the natural kinds
supporting cognition (Iglesia, 1981). For now,
researchers stand to gain from considering concepts and
experimental approaches across the textbook psychologi-
cal domains. Working memory is a special point in case. Como
a bridge between sensory experience and adaptive behav-
ior, working memory should be considered from an evo-
lutionary and ecological perspective (Un cuerpo, 2019) and not
just for its phenomenological qualities. Working memory
is an essential building block for orienting attention,
Toma de decisiones, long-term memory formation, acción
control, idioma, planificación, Etcétera. Tal como, empha-
sis is increasingly falling on the functional and pragmatic
aspects of working memory (van Ede & Nobre, 2023;
Nobre & alimenta, 2019).
When integrating findings and perspectives across spe-
cíes, strands from different experimental methods, y
psychological domains, it is also important to preserve rel-
evant distinctions. Early neuropsychological studies
revealed many important dissociations between psycho-
logical functions. Some of the dissociations may have been
misleading, such as dividing short-term and long-term
memory based on performance in tasks with disparate
materials and demands. Some of the dissociations, cómo-
alguna vez, may still stand, such as HM’s ability to maintain online
behavior that relies on working memory without the ability
to commit those contents to long-term memories. By their
naturaleza, neuropsychological milestones become associated
with specific brain areas. In the case of memory, the hippo-
campus and medial temporal area became the territory
dedicated to episodic long-term memory, characterized
by contextual and relational associations. Cada vez más,
studies indicate the involvement of these areas in working
memory and perception, depending on the type of content
required for task performance (p.ej., Graham et al., 2010).
De este modo, alternative models of hippocampal function are
developing (p.ej., Barry & Maguire, 2019; Maguire & Mullally,
2013). These advances, sin embargo, do not negate the impor-
tant distinction between perception, working memory,
and long-term memory. The challenge ahead is to refine
the understanding of how brain areas contribute to differ-
ent extents and in different ways to larger-scale networks
supporting psychological functions and to uncover the
critical structural, actividad, plasticity, conectividad, y
computational parameters for performing natural tasks
based on different types of content. Developing tasks that
equate stimulus materials and task demands for assaying
performance based on perception, working memory,
and long-term memory should prove particularly fruitful
(p.ej., Richter, Cooper, Laureles, & simons, 2016).
Another fundamental distinction to preserve, at least in
human studies, is the obvious separation between the
mental (subjetivo) and neural (implementational) niveles
of description. Most of us, as reductionists, believe in a
mapping between the neural and the mental. Sin embargo,
centuries of musing, theorizing, and experimenting rule
out simple one-to-one isomorphic mappings. In the case
de memoria de trabajo, the subjective experience of appre-
hending the rich gamut of sensory experience contrasts
sharply with the limited capacity to report memoranda
objectively. Most working-memory studies focus primarily
on objective behavioral measures, leaving subjective phe-
nomenology untapped. We study humans as what philos-
ophers describe as zombies (Chalmers, 1996; Kirk, 1974).
Todavía, conflating or flip-flopping between these levels is a
frequent bad habit when generating hypotheses and inter-
preting findings. Holding something fixed in mind does
not imply a sustained fixed neural mechanism. En cambio,
many consciously inaccessible short-term traces of experi-
ence impact behavior. Stitching across this ultimate divide
requires a careful and critical appreciation of their concep-
tual distinctions.
54
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva
Volumen 35, Número 1
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
Conclusions
Recent research findings and theoretical proposals have
disrupted the relatively calm field of working memory,
highlighting just how enigmatic this fundamental building
block of adaptive cognition remains. We are not “there
todavía,” so we should maintain the level of inquisitive alert-
ness toward the new standard views that are forming.
Fuller understanding may require conciliating and inte-
grating multiple perspectives that still seem antagonistic.
Breaking some scientific bad habits could help set the field
on a more productive path.
Agreeing on a core nomenclature would be transforma-
tivo. Por ejemplo, is it enough to consider the traces of
recent experience for guiding behavior as working mem-
ory or is active manipulation required? What functions
would count? Could terminology usefully differentiate
the neural and subjective states? Short of a shared taxon-
omy, defining terms would be a start. Keywords carry dif-
ferent connotations in the working-memory field and vary
even more between neighboring fields. Papers rarely spell
out definitions and premises, causing readers to project
their own intellectual biases, which may distort the
intended messages.
Stepping outside the comfort zone will be essential. Dia-
logue between divergent and even opposing camps
should help distil what notions are mutually incompatible
or reconcilable, develop falsifiable models, and inspire piv-
otal experimental hypotheses. Scientific niches have
formed across many domains, such as species of study,
methods, methodological markers, level of analysis, y
experimental tasks. Small communities thus indepen-
dently develop repertoires of findings, ideas, and termi-
nology in parallel, with few attempts to build bridges.
Workshops and symposia in larger conferences often con-
vene like-minded scientists. En cambio, intensive meetings
and sessions exploring points of disconnection and ten-
sion within a safe environment might prove much more
instructive and possibly even more enjoyable.
Finalmente, as scientists, we are trained to “pontificate.” Our
presentations and writings carefully synthesize what we
have learned and understood, preferably in a cohesive
and engaging narrative. Sin embargo, we also need to find a
voice for our complementary and equally important igno-
rance. Ignorance, después de todo, is the true engine of science
(Firestein, 2012). Openly admitting ignorance is not a nat-
ural or comfortable activity. Those lingering confusions,
nagging doubts, methodological preoccupations, y
unyielding questions are often saved for solitary contem-
plation. Sometimes though, you find a kindred spirit in a
close colleague with whom you can share and learn from
ignorance. Franco, amplio, probing, meandering conversa-
tions then turn into creative fuel and intellectual advance-
mento. I am lucky to count on some close mind-opening
colegas, including Mark Stokes. My hunch is that find-
ing a way to open similar exchanges more widely would
turbocharge our science and improve us as scientists.
Mientras tanto, I remain open-minded and will be curious
to look back in another decade to check how our under-
standing of working memory has progressed. Who knows?
By then, we may have done away with working memory as
a separate psychological domain altogether, and rather
consider it as an intrinsic memory function embedded
within other psychological domains that evolved to sup-
port adaptive behavior. Pero, whether an independent
domain or a ubiquitous property of neural systems, trabajar-
ing memory will always be that remarkable bridge between
the past and the future essential for so much intelligent
and flexible human behavior.
Expresiones de gratitud
The ideas in this reflection benefitted from many conversations
and exchanges with current and past members and collabora-
tors of the Brain & Cognition Lab, especially Freek van Ede,
Nahid Zokaei, Sage Boettcher, and Dejan Draschkow in recent
años. Our working-memory research is supported by a Well-
come Trust Senior Investigator Award (104571/Z/14/Z), a James
S. McDonnell Foundation Understanding Human Cognition
Collaborative Award (220020448), and the NIHR Oxford Health
Biomedical Research Centre. The Wellcome Centre for Inte-
grative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the
Wellcome Trust (203139/ Z/16/ Z). For the purpose of open
access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright license
to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this
envío.
My thanks go to Brad Postle for suggesting this contribution and
to Brad Postle, Freek van Ede, Nahid Zokaei, Dejan Draschkow,
Dongyu Gong, and Irene Echeverria-Altuna for helpful comments
on a previous draft of the manuscript.
Reprint requests should be sent to Anna C. Nobre, Departamento
of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford and Oxford
Centre for Human Brain Activity, Wellcome Centre for Integra-
tive Neuroimaging, Department of Psychiatry, Universidad de
Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford, Reino Unido, OX1 3UD, o por correo electrónico:
kia.nobre@ohba.ox.ac.uk.
Información de financiación
James S. McDonnell Foundation (https://dx.doi.org/10
.13039/100000913), conceder número: 220020448. Wellcome
Confianza (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100010269), grant
number: 104571/Z/14/Z. Wellcome Trust (https://dx.doi
.org/10.13039/100010269), conceder número: 203139/Z/16/Z.
Diversidad en las prácticas de citas
Análisis retrospectivo de las citas en cada artículo publicado.-
publicado en esta revista de 2010 a 2021 revela una persistente
patrón de desequilibrio de género: Aunque las proporciones de
equipos de autoría (categorizado por iden de género estimado-
tification of first author/ last author) publishing in the
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva ( JoCN ) during this
period were M(un)/m = .407, W.(Omán)/m = .32, M/M =
.115, and W/ W = .159, the comparable proportions for
the articles that these authorship teams cited were
M/M = .549, W/M = .257, M/M = .109, and W/ W = .085
Nobre
55
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
(Postle and Fulvio, JoCN, 34:1, páginas. 1–3). Como consecuencia,
JoCN encourages all authors to consider gender balance
explicitly when selecting which articles to cite and gives
them the opportunity to report their article’s gender
citation balance. The author of this article reports its pro-
portions of citations by gender category to be as follows:
M/M = .539; W/M = .211; M/M = .211; P/P = .039.
Nota
1. By paradigm, I mean “a conceptual or methodological
model underlying the theories and practices of a science or dis-
cipline at a particular time; (hence) a generally accepted world
view” (Oxford English Dictionary). This word is often used,
incorrectly in my view, to refer to an instance of an experimen-
tal design or setup.
REFERENCIAS
Brookes, METRO. J., lana rica, METRO., Luckhoo, h., Precio, D., Hale, j. r.,
Stephenson, METRO. C., et al. (2011). Investigating the
electrophysiological basis of resting state networks using
magnetoencephalography. Actas del Nacional
Academia de Ciencias, EE.UU., 108, 16783–16788. https://doi
.org/10.1073/pnas.1112685108, PubMed: 21930901
Buonomano, D. v., & Maass, W.. (2009). State-dependent
computations: Spatiotemporal processing in cortical
redes. Naturaleza Reseñas Neurociencia, 10, 113–125.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2558, PubMed: 19145235
Chalmers, D. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a
fundamental theory. prensa de la Universidad de Oxford.
Chatham, C. h., & malo, D. (2015). Multiple gates on working
memory. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 23–31.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.08.001, PubMed:
26719851
Christophel, t. B., Klink, PAG. C., Spitzer, B., Roelfsema, PAG. r., &
Haynes, J.-D. (2017). The distributed nature of working
memory. Tendencias en Ciencias Cognitivas, 21, 111–124. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.007, PubMed: 28063661
Iglesia, PAG. METRO. (1981). Eliminative materialism and
propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy, 78, 67–90.
https://doi.org/10.5840/jphil198178268
Adán, k. C. S., Robison, METRO. K., & Vogel, mi. k. (2018).
Un cuerpo, PAG. (2019). Resintetizando el comportamiento a través de la filogenética.
Contralateral delay activity tracks fluctuations in working
memory performance. Revista de neurociencia cognitiva,
30, 1229–1240. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01233,
PubMed: 29308988
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. METRO. (1968). Human memory: A
proposed system and its control processes. In K. W.. Spence
& j. t. Spence (Editores.), Psychology of learning and
motivación: Advances in research and theory (volumen. 2,
páginas. 89–195). Nueva York: Académico. https://doi.org/10.1016
/S0079-7421(08)60422-3
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. METRO. (1971). The control of
short-term memory. Scientific American, 225, 82–90. https://
doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0871-82, PubMed: 5089457
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, modelos, y
controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422,
PubMed: 21961947
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, GRAMO. (1974). Working memory. En
Psychology of learning and motivation (volumen. 8, páginas. 47–89).
Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, GRAMO. J., & allen, R. j. (2019). De
short-term store to multicomponent working memory: El
role of the modal model. Memoria & Cognición, 47, 575–588.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0878-5, PubMed: 30478520
Barry, D. NORTE., & Maguire, mi. A. (2019). Remote memory and the
hippocampus: A constructive critique. Tendencias en Cognitivo
Ciencias, 23, 128–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.11
.005, PubMed: 30528612
Laureles, PAG. METRO., & Husain, METRO. (2008). Turnos dinámicos de duración limitada.
Recursos de la memoria de trabajo en la visión humana.. Ciencia, 321,
851–854. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1158023, PubMed:
18687968
Benisty, h., Moberly, A. h., Lohani, S., Barson, D., Coifman, R. r.,
Mishne, GRAMO., et al. (2021). Rapid fluctuations in functional
connectivity of cortical networks encode spontaneous
comportamiento. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.15.456390
BICCN (BRAIN Initiative Cell Census Network). (2021). A
multimodal cell census and atlas of the mammalian primary
motor cortex. Naturaleza, 598, 86–102. https://doi.org/10.1038
/s41586-021-03950-0, PubMed: 34616075
Boettcher, S. mi. PAG., Gresch, D., Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F.
(2021). Output planning at the input stage in visual working
memory. Science Advances, 7, eabe8212. https://doi.org/10
.1126/sciadv.abe8212, PubMed: 33762341
refinamiento. Atención, Percepción, & Psicofísica, 81,
2265–2287. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01760-1,
PubMed: 31161495
Cowan, norte. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working
memory capacity limited, y por qué? Direcciones actuales en
ciencia psicológica, 19, 51–57. https://doi.org/10.1177
/0963721409359277, PubMed: 20445769
Curtis, C. MI., & D'Esposito, METRO. (2003). Persistent activity in the
prefrontal cortex during working memory. Tendencias en
Cognitive Sciences, 7, 415–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364
-6613(03)00197-9, PubMed: 12963473
Day-Cooney, J., Cone, j. J., & Maunsell, j. h. R. (2022).
Perceptual weighting of V1 spikes revealed by optogenetic
white noise stimulation. Revista de neurociencia, 42,
3122–3132. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1736-21
.2022, PubMed: 35232760
Desimone, r., & Duncan, j. (1995). Neural mechanisms of
selective visual attention. Revisión anual de neurociencia,
18, 193–222. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195
.001205, PubMed: 7605061
Dotan, D., Pinheiro-Chagas, PAG., Al Roumi, F., & Dehaene, S.
(2019). Track it to crack it: Dissecting processing stages with
finger tracking. Tendencias en Ciencias Cognitivas, 23, 1058–1070.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.10.002, PubMed: 31679752
Draschkow, D. (2022). Remote virtual reality as a tool for
increasing external validity. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1,
433–434. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00082-8
Draschkow, D., Kallmayer, METRO., & Nobre, A. C. (2021). Cuando
natural behavior engages working memory. Biología actual,
31, 869–874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.11.013,
PubMed: 33278355
Duncan, j. (2001). An adaptive coding model of neural function
in prefrontal cortex. Naturaleza Reseñas Neurociencia, 2,
820–829. https://doi.org/10.1038/35097575, PubMed:
11715058
Echeverria-Altuna, I., quinn, A. J., Zokaei, NORTE., lana rica, METRO. w.,
Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F. (2022). Transient beta activity and
cortico-muscular connectivity during sustained motor
behaviour. Progress in Neurobiology, 214, 102281. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102281, PubMed: 35550908
Firestein, S. (2012). Ignorance: How it drives science. Oxford
Prensa universitaria.
Fitz, h., Uhlmann, METRO., van den Broek, D., Duarte, r., Hagoort,
PAG., & Petersson, k. METRO. (2020). Neuronal spike-rate adaptation
56
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva
Volumen 35, Número 1
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
supports working memory in language processing.
procedimientos de la Academia Nacional de Ciencias, EE.UU.,
117, 20881–20889. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2000222117,
PubMed: 32788365
fulvio, j. METRO., & Postle, B. R. (2020). Cognitive control, not time,
determines the status of items in working memory. Diario
of Cognition, 3, 8. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.98, PubMed:
32292872
Fuster, j. METRO. (1973). Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during
delayed-response performance: Neuronal correlates of
transient memory. Revista de neurofisiología, 36, 61–78.
https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1973.36.1.61, PubMed: 4196203
Fuster, j. METRO., & Alexander, GRAMO. mi. (1971). Neuron activity related
to short-term memory. Ciencia, 173, 652–654. https://doi.org
/10.1126/science.173.3997.652, PubMed: 4998337
Goel, A., & Buonomano, D. V. (2014). Timing as an intrinsic
property of neural networks: Evidence from in vivo and in
vitro experiments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society of London, Serie B: Ciencias Biologicas, 369,
20120460. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0460, PubMed:
24446494
Goldman-Rakic, PAG. S. (1984). The frontal lobes: Uncharted
provinces of the brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 7, 425–429.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(84)80147-2
graham, k. S., Barense, METRO. D., & Sotavento, A. C. h. (2010). Going
beyond LTM in the MTL: A synthesis of neuropsychological
and neuroimaging findings on the role of the medial
temporal lobe in memory and perception.
Neuropsicología, 48, 831–853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuropsychologia.2010.01.001, PubMed: 20074580
Griffin, I. C., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). Orienting attention to
locations in internal representations. Revista de Cognitivo
Neurociencia, 15, 1176–1194. https://doi.org/10.1162
/089892903322598139, PubMed: 14709235
Hajonides, j. MI., van Ede, F., alimenta, METRO. GRAMO., & Nobre, A. C.
(2020). Comparing the prioritization of items and
feature-dimensions in visual working memory. Diario de
Vision, 20, 25. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.8.25, PubMed:
32841318
Hebb, D. oh. (1949). The organization of behavior: A
neuropsychological theory. wiley.
the primate interneuron repertoire. Naturaleza, 586, 262–269.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2781-z, PubMed: 32999462
Kubota, K., & Niki, h. (1971). Prefrontal cortical unit activity and
delayed alternation performance in monkeys. Diario de
Neurofisiología, 34, 337–347. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn
.1971.34.3.337, PubMed: 4997822
Landman, r., Spekreijse, h., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Large
capacity storage of integrated objects before change
blindness. Vision Research, 43, 149–164. https://doi.org/10
.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9, PubMed: 12536137
Langdon, C., & ángel, t. A. (2022). Latent circuit inference from
heterogeneous neural responses during cognitive tasks.
bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.01.23.477431
Lepsien, J., & Nobre, A. C. (2007). Attentional modulation of
object representations in working memory. Corteza cerebral,
17, 2072–2083. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl116,
PubMed: 17099066
Lewis-Peacock, j. A., Drysdale, A. T., Oberauer, K., & Postle,
B. R. (2012). Neural evidence for a distinction between
short-term memory and the focus of attention. Diario de
Neurociencia Cognitiva, 24, 61–79. https://doi.org/10.1162
/jocn_a_00140, PubMed: 21955164
Lewis-Peacock, j. A., & Postle, B. R. (2012). Decoding the
internal focus of attention. Neuropsicología, 50, 470–478.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.006,
PubMed: 22108440
lin, y., kong, GRAMO., & Fougnie, D. (2021). Object-based selection
in visual working memory. Boletín psiconómico & Revisar,
28, 1961–1971. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-01971-4,
PubMed: 34258730
Mamá, W.. J., Husain, METRO., & Laureles, PAG. METRO. (2014). Conceptos cambiantes
de memoria de trabajo. Neurociencia de la naturaleza, 17, 347–356.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3655, PubMed: 24569831
Maguire, mi. A., & Mullally, S. l. (2013). The hippocampus: A
manifesto for change. Revista de Psicología Experimental:
General, 142, 1180–1189. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033650,
PubMed: 23855494
marshall, norte. J., Glaser, j. I., Trautmann, mi. METRO., Amematsro, mi. A.,
Perkins, S. METRO., Shadlen, METRO. NORTE., et al. (2021). Flexible neural
control of motor units. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021
.05.05.442653
Heuer, A., Ohl, S., & Rolfs, METRO. (2020). Memory for action: A
Mau, w., Morales-Rodriguez, D., Dong, Z., Pennington, z. T.,
functional view of selection in visual working memory. Visual
Cognición, 28, 388–400. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285
.2020.1764156
Hussar, C., & Pasternak, t. (2010). Trial-to-trial variability of the
prefrontal neurons reveals the nature of their engagement in
a motion discrimination task. Actas del Nacional
Academia de Ciencias, EE.UU., 107, 21842–21847. https://doi
.org/10.1073/pnas.1009956107, PubMed: 21098286
Ikkai, A., & Curtis, C. mi. (2011). Common neural mechanisms
supporting spatial working memory, attention and motor
intention. Neuropsicología, 49, 1428–1434. https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.020, PubMed: 21182852
James, W.. (1890). The principles of psychology. Nueva York:
Henry Holt and Company. https://doi.org/10.1037/10538-000
Karmarkar, Ud.. r., & Buonomano, D. V. (2007). Timing in the
absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states.
Neurona, 53, 427–438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007
.01.006, PubMed: 17270738
Kirk, R. (1974). Sentience and behaviour. Mente, 83, 43–60.
https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/LXXXIII.329.43
Kojima, S., & Goldman-Rakic, PAG. S. (1982). Delay-related activity
of prefrontal neurons in rhesus monkeys performing delayed
respuesta. Brain Research, 248, 43–50. https://doi.org/10
.1016/0006-8993(82)91145-3, PubMed: 7127141
Krienen, F. METRO., hombre de oro, METRO., zhang, P., Del Rosario, R. C. h.,
Florio, METRO., Machold, r., et al. (2020). Innovations present in
Francisco, T., Baxter, METRO. GRAMO., et al. (2022). Ensemble
remodeling supports memory-updating. bioRxiv. https://doi
.org/10.1101/2022.06.02.494530
Molinero, mi. K., Erickson, C. A., & Desimone, R. (1996). Neural
mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the
macaque. Revista de neurociencia, 16, 5154–5167. https://doi
.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-16-05154.1996, PubMed: 8756444
Molinero, GRAMO. A., Galanter, MI., & Pribram, k. h. (1960). Plans and
the structure of behavior. Nueva York: Henry Holt & Co.
https://doi.org/10.1037/10039-000
Molinero, mi. K., Lundqvist, METRO., & Bastos, A. METRO. (2018). Working
memory 2.0. Neurona, 100, 463–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuron.2018.09.023, PubMed: 30359609
Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H.-L. (1968). Further analysis of
the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study
of H.M. Neuropsicología, 6, 215–234. https://doi.org/10
.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3
Mostert, PAG., Albers, A. METRO., Brinkman, l., Todorova, l., Kok, PAG., &
el largo, F. PAG. (2018). Eye movement-related confounds in
neural decoding of visual working memory representations.
eNeuro, 5, ENEURO.0401-17.2018. https://doi.org/10.1523
/ENEURO.0401-17.2018, PubMed: 30310862
Buen Karbe, PAG. S., miers, norte. MI., & alimenta, METRO. GRAMO. (2021). A
hierarchy of functional states in working memory. Diario de
Neurociencia, 41, 4461–4475. https://doi.org/10.1523
/JNEUROSCI.3104-20.2021, PubMed: 33888611
Nobre
57
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
miers, norte. MI., Chekroud, S. r., alimenta, METRO. GRAMO., & Nobre, A. C.
(2018). Benefits of flexible prioritization in working memory
can arise without costs. Revista de Psicología Experimental:
Percepción y desempeño humanos, 44, 398–411. https://
doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000449, PubMed: 28816476
miers, norte. MI., alimenta, METRO. GRAMO., & Nobre, A. C. (2017). Prioritizing
information during working memory: Beyond sustained
internal attention. Tendencias en Ciencias Cognitivas, 21, 449–461.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.010, PubMed: 28454719
Neisser, Ud.. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Nueva York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts.
Niklaus, METRO., Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F. (2017). Feature-based
attentional weighting and spreading in visual working
memory. Informes Científicos, 7, 42384. https://doi.org/10.1038
/srep42384, PubMed: 28233830
Nobre, A. C., & alimenta, METRO. GRAMO. (2011). Attention and short-term
memory: Crossroads. Neuropsicología, 49, 1391–1392.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.014,
PubMed: 21571124
Nobre, A. C., & alimenta, METRO. GRAMO. (2019). Premembering
experiencia: A hierarchy of time-scales for proactive attention.
Neurona, 104, 132–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019
.08.030, PubMed: 31600510
Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F. (2020). Under the mind’s hood:
What we have learned by watching the brain at work. Diario
of Neuroscience, 40, 89–100. https://doi.org/10.1523
/JNEUROSCI.0742-19.2019, PubMed: 31630115
Novembre, GRAMO., Pawar, V. METRO., Kilintari, METRO., Bufacchi, R. J., guo,
y., Rothwell, j. C., et al. (2019). The effect of salient stimuli on
neural oscillations, isometric force, and their coupling.
Neuroimagen, 198, 221–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2019.05.032, PubMed: 31085301
Oberauer, k. (2002). Access to information in working memory:
Exploring the focus of attention. Revista de experimentación
Psicología: Aprendiendo, Memoria, and Cognition, 28, 411–421.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411, PubMed:
12018494
Olson, I. r., moore, k. S., Rígido, METRO., & Chatterjee, A. (2006).
Visual working memory is impaired when the medial
temporal lobe is damaged. Revista de Cognitivo
Neurociencia, 18, 1087–1097. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn
.2006.18.7.1087, PubMed: 16839283
Panichello, METRO. F., & Buschman, t. j. (2021). Shared mechanisms
underlie the control of working memory and attention.
Naturaleza, 592, 601–605. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021
-03390-w, PubMed: 33790467
Perrenoud, P., Fonseca, A. h. d. o., Airhart, A., Bonanno, J.,
Mao, r., & Cardin, j. A. (2022). Flexible perceptual encoding
by discrete gamma events. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101
/2022.05.13.491832
Pertzov, y., Molinero, t. D., Gorgoraptis, NORTE., Caine, D., Schott, j. METRO.,
mayordomo, C., et al. (2013). Binding deficits in memory following
medial temporal lobe damage in patients with voltage-gated
potassium channel complex antibody-associated limbic
encephalitis. Cerebro, 136, 2474–2485. https://doi.org/10.1093
/brain/awt129, PubMed: 23757763
Peters, B., Kaiser, J., Rahm, B., & Bledowski, C. (2015). Activity
in human visual and parietal cortex reveals object-based
attention in working memory. Revista de neurociencia, 35,
3360–3369. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3795-14
.2015, PubMed: 25716836
Quax, S. C., Dijkstra, NORTE., van Staveren, METRO. J., Bosch, S. MI., & camioneta
Gerven, METRO. A. j. (2019). Eye movements explain decodability
during perception and cued attention in MEG. Neuroimagen,
195, 444–453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03
.069, PubMed: 30951848
quinn, A. J., Lopes-dos-Santos, v., Dupret, D., Nobre, A. C.,
& lana rica, METRO. W.. (2021). EMD: Empirical mode
decomposition and Hilbert-Huang spectral analyses in
Python. Journal of Open Source Software, 6, 2977. https://
doi.org/10.21105/joss.02977, PubMed: 33855259
quinn, A. J., van Ede, F., Brookes, METRO. J., Heideman, S. GRAMO.,
Nowak, METRO., Seedat, z. A., et al. (2019). Unpacking transient
event dynamics in electrophysiological power spectra. Cerebro
Topography, 32, 1020–1034. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548
-019-00745-5, PubMed: 31754933
Rerko, l., & Oberauer, k. (2013). Focused, unfocused, y
defocused information in working memory. Diario de
Psicología experimental: Aprendiendo, Memoria, y
Cognición, 39, 1075–1096. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031172,
PubMed: 23421511
Richter, F. r., Cooper, R. A., Laureles, PAG. METRO., & simons, j. S. (2016).
Distinct neural mechanisms underlie the success, precisión,
and vividness of episodic memory. eVida, 5, e18260. https://
doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18260, PubMed: 27776631
Rose, norte. S., LaRocque, j. J., Riggall, A. C., Gosseries, o., Starrett,
METRO. J., Meyering, mi. MI., et al. (2016). Reactivation of latent
working memories with transcranial magnetic stimulation.
Ciencia, 354, 1136–1139. https://doi.org/10.1126/science
.aah7011, PubMed: 27934762
Rypma, B., Berger, j. S., & D'Esposito, METRO. (2002). The influence
of working-memory demand and subject performance on
prefrontal cortical activity. Revista de Cognitivo
Neurociencia, 14, 721–731. https://doi.org/10.1162
/08989290260138627, PubMed: 12167257
Schneider, D., Barth, A., & Wascher, mi. (2017). Sobre el
contribution of motor planning to the retroactive cuing
benefit in working memory: Evidence by mu and beta
oscillatory activity in the EEG. Neuroimagen, 162, 73–85.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.057, PubMed:
28847491
Scoville, W.. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after
bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20, 11–21. https://doi.org/10
.1136/jnnp.20.1.11, PubMed: 13406589
Sperling, GRAMO. (1960). The information available in brief visual
presentations. Psychological Monographs: generales y
Applied, 74, 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093759
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory.
Ciencia, 153, 652–654. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.153
.3736.652, PubMed: 5939936
alimenta, METRO. GRAMO. (2015). ‘Activity-silent’ working memory in
corteza prefrontal: A dynamic coding framework. Tendencias en
Cognitive Sciences, 19, 394–405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics
.2015.05.004, PubMed: 26051384
van Ede, F. (2020). Visual working memory and action:
Functional links and bi-directional influences. Visual
Cognición, 28, 401–413. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285
.2020.1759744, PubMed: 33223921
van Ede, F., Board, A. GRAMO., & Nobre, A. C. (2020). Goal-directed
and stimulus-driven selection of internal representations.
procedimientos de la Academia Nacional de Ciencias, EE.UU.,
117, 24590–24598. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2013432117,
PubMed: 32929036
van Ede, F., Chekroud, S. r., & Nobre, A. C. (2019). Humano
gaze tracks attentional focusing in memorized visual space.
Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 462–470. https://doi.org/10
.1038/s41562-019-0549-y, PubMed: 31089296
van Ede, F., Chekroud, S. r., alimenta, METRO. GRAMO., & Nobre, A. C.
(2019). Concurrent visual and motor selection during visual
working memory guided action. Neurociencia de la naturaleza, 22,
477–483. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0335-6, PubMed:
30718904
van Ede, F., Niklaus, METRO., & Nobre, A. C. (2017). Temporal
expectations guide dynamic prioritization in visual working
memory through attenuated α oscillations. Diario de
58
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva
Volumen 35, Número 1
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
Neurociencia, 37, 437–445. https://doi.org/10.1523
/JNEUROSCI.2272-16.2016, PubMed: 28077721
van Ede, F., & Nobre, A. C. (2021). Toward a neurobiology of
internal selective attention. Trends in Neurosciences, 44,
513–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.04.010, PubMed:
33992457
van Ede, F., & Nobre, A. C. (2023). Turning attention inside out:
How working memory serves behavior. Annual Review of
Psicología, 74. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-021422
-041757, PubMed: 35961038
Vogel, mi. K., & Machizawa, METRO. GRAMO. (2004). Neural activity predicts
individual differences in visual working memory capacity.
Naturaleza, 428, 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02447,
PubMed: 15085132
Wang, X.-J. (2001). Synaptic reverberation underlying
mnemonic persistent activity. Trends in Neurosciences, 24,
455–463. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01868-3,
PubMed: 11476885
Wang, X.-J. (2021). 50 years of mnemonic persistent activity:
Quo vadis? Trends in Neurosciences, 44, 888–902. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.09.001, PubMed: 34654556
Warrington, mi. K., & Shallice, t. (1972). Neuropsychological
evidence of visual storage in short-term memory tasks.
Revista trimestral de psicología experimental, 24, 30–40.
https://doi.org/10.1080/14640747208400265, PubMed: 5017505
Watanabe, K., & Funahashi, S. (2014). Neural mechanisms of
dual-task interference and cognitive capacity limitation in the
corteza prefrontal. Neurociencia de la naturaleza, 17, 601–611. https://
doi.org/10.1038/nn.3667, PubMed: 24584049
Wolff, METRO. J., Ding, J., miers, norte. MI., & alimenta, METRO. GRAMO. (2015).
Revealing hidden states in visual working memory using
electroencephalography. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience,
9, 123. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00123, PubMed:
26388748
Wolff, METRO. J., Jochim, J., Akyürek, mi. GRAMO., & alimenta, METRO. GRAMO. (2017).
Dynamic hidden states underlying working-memory-guided
comportamiento. Neurociencia de la naturaleza, 20, 864–871. https://doi.org
/10.1038/nn.4546, PubMed: 28414333
Xu, Y. (2017). Reevaluating the sensory account of visual
working memory storage. Tendencias en Ciencias Cognitivas, 21,
794–815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.013, PubMed:
28774684
S.M, C., Hu, Z., Ristaniemi, T., Gendron, METRO., & Liu, q. (2016).
Retro-dimension-cue benefit in visual working memory.
Informes Científicos, 6, 35573. https://doi.org/10.1038
/srep35573, PubMed: 27774983
zhang, w., & Luck, S. j. (2008). Discrete fixed-resolution
representations in visual working memory. Naturaleza, 453,
233–235. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06860, PubMed:
18385672
Zhigalov, A., Herring, j. D., Herpers, J., Bergmann, t. o., &
Jensen, oh. (2019). Probing cortical excitability using rapid
frequency tagging. Neuroimagen, 195, 59–66. https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.056, PubMed: 30930309
Zokaei, NORTE., Board, A. GRAMO., Manohar, S. GRAMO., & Nobre, A. C. (2019).
Modulation of the pupillary response by the content of visual
working memory. Actas de la Academia Nacional de
Ciencias, EE.UU., 116, 22802–22810. https://doi.org/10.1073
/pnas.1909959116, PubMed: 31636213
Zokaei, NORTE., Nour, METRO. METRO., silencio, A., Dibujó, D., Adcock, J.,
Stacey, r., et al. (2019). Binding deficits in visual short-term
memory in patients with temporal lobe lobectomy.
Hippocampus, 29, 63–67. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo
.22998, PubMed: 30069971
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
mi
d
tu
/
j
/
oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
yo
F
/
/
/
/
3
5
1
4
9
2
0
8
8
2
1
5
/
j
oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
9
2
0
pag
d
.
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
Nobre
59