Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late

Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late
Event-related Potential Modulation

Andrew H. Farkas and Dean Sabatinelli

Astratto

■ The early posterior negativity (EPN) is a mid-latency ERP
component that is reliably enhanced by emotional cues, con
a deflection beginning between 150 E 200 msec after stimu-
lus onset. The brief, bilateral occipital EPN is followed by the
centroparietal late positive potential (LPP), a long duration
slow-wave that is strongly associated with emotional arousal
ratings of scenes. A recent study suggests that the EPN is par-
ticularly sensitive to human bodies in scenes, independent of
emotional intensity. Here, we directly investigate the influence
of human body features on EPN modulation, using emotional
and neutral scenes depicting people across a range of body

exposures and orientations, in addition to scenes of pleasant,
neutro, and unpleasant animals. The results demonstrate that
the EPN is quite sensitive to human body features and weakly
related to arousal ratings, whereas the LPP is strongly modu-
lated by scenes that receive high arousal ratings. Based on these
results and relevant work on body-specific visual perception, we
speculate that modulation of the EPN may strongly reflect the
early detection of human bodies, which serves as a predictor of
emotional significance, whereas LPP modulation is more closely
associated with the extended elaborative processing of scenes
that are explicitly judged to be emotionally arousing.

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
5
6
9
4
1
2
0
8
2
9
2
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
9
8
4
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
7
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

INTRODUCTION

The collection and interpretation of scene-evoked cortical
potentials is a powerful paradigm in the effort to under-
stand the mental processes supporting the perception of
emotional cues. Two of the most well-studied ERPs asso-
ciated with emotional processing are the early posterior
negativity (EPN) and the late positive potential (LPP).
The amplitudes of these potentials have been shown to
be enhanced by emotionally arousing, relative to neutral
content (Olofsson, Nordin, Sequeira, & Polich, 2008;
Schupp, Stockburger, et al., 2006; Pastor et al., 2008). Questo
arousal effect can be reliably found during naturalistic
scene perception, and it is also evident (although to a
reduced extent) during the perception of emotional faces
(Langeslag, Gootjes, & van Strien, 2018; Yoon, Shim, Kim,
& Lee, 2016; Herbert, Sfärlea, & Blumenthal, 2013;
Jaworska et al., 2012; Schupp, Öhman, et al., 2004), hand
gestures (Flaisch, Schupp, Renner, & Junghöfer, 2009),
and words (Schindler & Kissler, 2016; Kissler & Herbert,
2013; Herbert, Junghöfer, & Kissler, 2008; Kissler, Herbert,
Peyk, & Junghöfer, 2007).

Despite similarities in the pattern of modulation, IL
EPN and LPP represent different but related aspects of
emotional perception. The EPN and LPP are temporally
and spatially distinct, with the EPN appearing as a neg-
ative voltage deflection over left and right lateral occip-
ital sensors from 150 A 300 msec after scene onset

University of Georgia, Athens

© 2023 Istituto di Tecnologia del Massachussetts

(Schupp, Junghöfer, Weike, & Hamm, 2004; Junghöfer,
Bradley, Elbert, & Lang, 2001) whereas the LPP is a
slow-wave positivity over central-parietal electrodes with
a peak approximately 400–900 msec after scene onset
(Schuppetal.,2007;Cuthbert,Schupp,Bradley,Birbaumer,
& Lang, 2000). Studies that have attempted to localize
the neural sources of these ERPs find that the EPN is likely
driven by lateral occipital extrastriate activity ( Junghöfer
et al., 2006; Schupp, Stockburger, et al., 2006) whereas
the LPP may represent a combination of activity from
multiple dorsal and ventral visual cortical regions, COME
well as anterior cortical sources (Sabatinelli, Keil, Frank,
& Lang, 2013; Liu, Hairston, Schrier, & Fan, 2011; Moratti,
Saugar, & Strange, 2011). Taken together, these ERPs
likely represent different stages involved in the discrim-
ination and perception of emotional cues, with the EPN
possibly reflecting the initial effects of emotional con-
tent in the secondary visual cortex, whereas the LPP
indexes the activity of widespread networks of activity
associated with the conscious experience of emotional
arousal.

Some studies have identified scene factors that differen-
tially modulate the EPN and LPP. Bradley, Hamby, Löw,
and Lang (2007) used principal components analysis to
identify clusters of electrodes that distinguished simple
(figure-ground) from complex emotional scenes, locating
a posterior occipital region that overlaps with the scalp
location and latency (150–250 msec) of the EPN. This clus-
ter showed enhanced voltage negativity during simple
scene perception, and no sensitivity to emotional content.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 35:6, pag. 941–956
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01984

A centroparietal cluster of sensors showed strong emotional
modulation from 400 A 700 msec, equivalent with the LPP,
and showed no sensitivity to scene complexity. This impor-
tant article obligated all future study of the EPN to employ
scenes that are balanced for complexity across emotional
and neutral content. Particularly relevant for the current
research question, Löw, Bradley, and Lang (2013) manipu-
lated emotional scene complexity and the inclusion of
humans in scenes and assessed the impact of these factors
on the EPN, as recorded using rapid serial visual presenta-
tion of scenes at 3 Hz. This study identified the largest
EPN modulation for emotional people depicted in simple,
figure-ground scenes, followed by the EPN in response
to neutral people in figure-ground scenes, followed by the
EPN in response to emotional people in complex
scenes. Così, the presence of clearly identifiable people in
scenes was more meaningful to EPN modulation than
whether those people were engaged in an emotional act.
Further evidence for the impact of clearly identifiable
people on EPN modulation, a recent study (Farkas,
Oliver, & Sabatinelli, 2020) found that scenes of upright,
unclothed people (nudists) evoke a larger EPN than do
scenes of erotic couples, despite garnering weak arousal
ratings. Despite enhancing the EPN, nudist scenes led to
reduced LPP amplitudes relative to that evoked by erotic
scenes. This unexpected finding necessitated an
experiment that more explicitly tests the effects of human
body exposure and orientation on EPN modulation.

In the current experiment, we aimed to replicate the
sensitivity of the EPN to nudist, compared with erotic
scenes (Farkas et al., 2020), while adding new scene cate-
sanguinose, additional measures of scene features (SFs), E
targeted analyses to parcel the potential relationships
between these factors and modulation of the EPN and
LPP. We consider body exposure to be a comparatively
subordinate SF relative to emotional scene content cate-
sanguinose. We employed the nudist and erotic scenes from
our previous experiment, and added seven additional
scene categories with varied levels of body exposure
across emotional and neutral contents. We included
scenes depicting pleasant, neutro, and unpleasant animals
to provide a nonbody comparison. Our intention is to
begin to disentangle the specific effects of body exposure
from higher-level emotional features of scene perception
(pleasantness, arousal) and subordinate features (animal/
human, clothed/unclothed, upright/supine).

We quantified characteristics of the scenes using
self-report and objective image values. These SFs included
self-reported valence, arousal, body exposure, and body
orientation, as well as digitally derived aspects of each
scene, including the number of pixels containing
unclothed body parts, energy in low and high spatial
frequency bands, and Shannon’s entropy. The study
employed two parallel analyses to understand how these
SFs are associated with the modulation of the EPN and LPP.
The first was a traditional analysis in which each partici-
pant’s average ERP across the 15 exemplars of each scene

category was compared. To evaluate the contribution of
SFs more precisely, a by-scene analysis assessed which
SFs were associated with EPN and LPP amplitudes, irrespec-
tive of scene category. In the by-scene analyses, we used
multiple regression and four variable-selection or regulari-
zation ( VSR) metodi. Our research question concerned
the relative impact of multiple scenes features, many of
which have been shown to independently influence ERP
magnitude in prior studies. To simplify data interpretation,
a multi-analysis approach was chosen to define which var-
iables had the strongest association with ERP modulation
and which variables and models would be the most stable
and predictive in a new data set. We hypothesized that
scenes containing people would evoke the largest EPN
(Löw et al., 2013), and that erotic ( Weinberg & Hajcak,
2010; Schupp et al., 2007) and nudist scenes (Farkas
et al., 2020) will elicit the greatest EPN amplitudes. We also
expect that scenes receiving elevated ratings of arousal will
be associated with enhanced EPN modulation (Frank &
Sabatinelli, 2019; Junghöfer et al., 2001). When analyzed
by scene exemplar, we hypothesize that scenes containing
upright, exposed bodies would better predict the EPN than
the LPP, which should have a stronger association with
average self-reported arousal for each scene (Thigpen,
Keil, & Freund, 2018; Cuthbert et al., 2000).

METHODS

Participants

Fifty-seven participants were recruited from the University
of Georgia student body and compensated with course
credit. All participants gave informed consent after receiv-
ing a description of the study approved by the University
of Georgia human subjects institutional review board.
Seven participants were excluded from the final analysis
after ERP preprocessing revealed excessive artifact leading
to the loss of more than 50% of the trials from any one
scene category. The remaining 50 participants were
between the ages of 18 E 26 years (M = 19.7 years,
SD = 1.40 years), con 28 self-reporting as female (self-
reported ethnicity: Asian 7.1%, Black 7.1%, White
85.7%) E 22 as male (Asian 9.1%, Black 13.6%, Hispanic
4.6%, Hispanic & White 4.6%, Multiracial 4.6%, White
63.6%). Participants identified as right-handed and
reported no history of mental illness. The sample size
was predetermined based on previous work using most
of the same scenes (Farkas et al., 2020).

A second online sample was recruited through Ama-
zon’s Mechanical Turk (Mturk) to rate the extent of body
exposure and body orientation of the scene stimuli. Partic-
ipants were compensated $1.50 to provide these ratings.
Participants were presented with an informed consent
approved by the University of Georgia institutional review
board. Mturk participants (n = 79) had to live in the
United States and be between the ages of 18 E 26 years.
Seven online participants consistently used the same

942

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Volume 35, Numero 6

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
5
6
9
4
1
2
0
8
2
9
2
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
9
8
4
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
7
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

number for every scene rating and were excluded from the
final analysis. Two participants appeared to give random
ratings and were also excluded. After exclusions, there
were 70 participants in the final online ratings sample.

Naturalistic Scenes

Nine categories of 135 naturalistic scenes were selected,
based on valence and arousal ratings from previous studies
in our laboratory. A minority of scenes were taken from
the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang,
Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997) whereas the remainder were
gathered through uncopyrighted sources and are consis-
tent in nature and composition with the IAPS. Scenes were
presented at an 800 × 600 pixel resolution at 90% JPEG
quality (https:// JPEG.org). Luminance and complexity
were selected to be equivalent between categories, COME
measured by independent t tests with p values greater
di .20. Scenes that were outliers in luminance and
complexity were mildly edited with GNU Image Manipula-
tion Program (https://www.gimp.org/). Complexity was
measured by the file size of the JPEG images at 90%
quality, which has been reported as a suitable measure
of complexity in human perception (Donderi, 2006). If
a scene file was too large at 90% quality, UN 1 × 1 pixel
Gaussian blur was used, which has a negligible effect on
perceived sharpness of the content.

The nine scene categories included erotica, pleasant
animals, victorious athletes, nudists, neutral animals,
neutral people (fully clothed), threatening animals, threat-
ening people, and mutilations. An example scene for each
category can be seen in Figure 1. Erotic scenes depicted an
attractive male–female couple engaged in consensual
intercourse. The pleasant animal category contained
scenes of baby ducks, puppies, and kittens. Victorious
scenes depicted athletes in expressive moments of

success. Nudist scenes contained unclothed couples of
various ages walking on beaches or parks, not engaged
in sexual activity. The neutral animal category depicted
adult animals such as cows, squirrels, and chickens. IL
neutral people category depicted clothed people engaged
in normal activities like riding in buses or talking to each
other. The threatening people category featured one or
more persons with aggressive body positions and facial
expressions, often brandishing weapons. Threatening
animal scenes contained predators such as wolves, jaguars,
and sharks aggressively posturing or attacking. Mutilation
scenes displayed graphic injuries of open wounds or disfig-
ured body parts. The scenes were chosen to roughly equate
the amount of body exposure within each valence subset.
Erotica, nudists, and mutilations scenes often have uncov-
ered skin making up a large portion of the scene as com-
pared with the victorious athletes, neutral people, E
threatening people categories that contain clothed individ-
uals. Finalmente, the three animal categories allowed for tests of
emotional ERP modulation effects independent of any
human bodies being present in the scene.

Procedure

After providing informed consent, participants were
brought into an electrically shielded chamber for EEG
cap fitting, which lasted about 10 min. IL 135 scenes
were then presented to each participant for 2 sec each,
with an intertrial interval between 3.5 E 5.5 sec. Partici-
pants were asked to keep their eyes fixated on a small
red dot in the middle of the scene. The complete set of
135 scenes were organized into two pseudorandom sets.
Half of the participants passively viewed the first set,
whereas the other half viewed the second set. To keep
the nine categories of content evenly spread throughout
il set, every 18 scenes had to feature two scenes from

Figura 1. Example scenes from
each category. Starting from
the top and moving from left
to right, displayed here are
example scenes from the
categories of erotica, pleasant
animals, victorious athletes,
nudists, neutral animals, neutro
people, mutilations, threatening
animals, and threatening
people.

Farkas and Sabatinelli

943

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
5
6
9
4
1
2
0
8
2
9
2
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
9
8
4
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
7
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

each category. There were no statistically significant cate-
gory differences between participants that saw Set 1 versus
Set 2. Scenes were presented on a 32-in. LCD monitor
that occupied 31° of the horizontal visual field of view.

Scene Self-reports

After data collection, the EEG cap was removed and partic-
ipants were seated outside of the chamber in a quiet room.
The University of Georgia, Athens (UGA) participants
reported how pleasant and aroused each scene made
them feel on a 9-point scale (with half-increments) using
the Self-Assessment Manikin (Bradley & Lang, 1994). Par-
ticipants were seated and read a script describing the rat-
ing procedure. Participants then viewed a printed version
of each scene in a binder and recorded their responses on
a worksheet. The worksheet had the Self-Assessment
Manikin figures and scales with emotion nouns at the
top of the page for participants to reference.

To quantify the additional SFs of body exposure and ori-
entation, an online sample was recruited to rate the 90
scenes depicting humans on each dimension. Participants
were instructed that the study could last an hour, but most
participants finished within 30 min. Participants accessed
the study through a Qualtrics link, which presented the
consent form and required their approval. If participants
agreed to take part in the study, they began rating each
scene in a pseudorandom order by using a click and drag
scale that ranged from 1 A 9. Each scale was displayed one
at a time underneath the scene the participant was view-
ing. The marker started in the middle of the scale on the
number 5, and participants were required to move the
marker before they could move on to the next scene
and scale. The scale allowed for precision to the tenth
decimal place (per esempio., 1.1, 1.2). For the body exposure scale,
a written question stated, “How much exposed or un-
clothed body parts are depicted in the scene?” The scale
presented below the question ranged from 1 being all
exposed body parts to 9 being no exposed body parts.
The body orientation scale stated, “How upright, O
normally oriented are the people in the scene?” with the
scale ranging from 1 indicating completely upright to 9
indicating very unusually oriented.

Quantifying Scene Features

To further quantify the extent to which exposed body
parts were present in scenes, the sum of pixels containing
exposed body parts was counted for each scene. Using
GNU Image Manipulation Program, a transparent layer
was made over each JPEG scene. On this layer, the LASSO
tool was used to manually trace the exposed body parts in
each scene. Body parts that were covered by clothes or
accessories such as arm bands and sunglasses were
excluded. After tracing, the paint bucket tool was used
to fill in the shapes with a solid color. The histogram tool
was then used to record how many pixels had been filled

by the paint bucket tool. The sum of pixels containing
exposed body parts for each scene could then be com-
pared with the total scene size.

As described above, scenes were chosen to be compa-
rable in perceptual complexity, placed in natural environ-
menti, and balanced across categories to be equivalent in
JPEG file size. The process of fully quantifying scene com-
plexity is quite complicated and beyond the scope of this
study (Donderi, 2006); Tuttavia, to expand our assess-
ment of SFs, Shannon information-entropy (from this
point simply entropy) was also calculated for each scene
using MATLAB (Mathworks.com). The entropy measure
is found by using the first order histogram frequencies
of gray values as probabilities. If every gray value is equally
likely to occur, entropy is maximized, whereas if the scene
contains only one value, then entropy is minimized. Noi
also assessed any remaining effects of JPEG file size.

Spatial frequency was assessed using the SHINE
MATLAB-based toolbox ( Willenbockel et al., 2010). For
each scene, the relative energy was found for all possible
integers of cycles per image from the lowest frequency of
1 to the maximum of 300 (the highest frequency complete
cycle for an image with a height of 600 pixels). Low spatial
frequencies have exponentially higher relative energies
than high spatial frequencies, so energy was transformed
logarithmically with a base of 10 for analyses. Low spatial
frequency was defined as the average transformed energy
from 1 A 40 cycles, and high spatial frequency was the
average from 41 A 300 cycles. This is consistent with prior
definitions of scene spatial frequency (De Cesarei &
Codispoti, 2013). Analyses that involved spatial frequency
were also computed with thresholds at 50, 60, 70, E 80
cycles for separation between low and high spatial fre-
quencies, and the results did not meaningfully change.

EEG Data Acquisition and Reduction

Continuous EEG data were recorded using a 64-channel
BioSemi ActiveTwo system (BioSemi), which has pre-
amplified electrodes positioned according to the 10/20
system. The electrode voltage was referenced to two addi-
tional common mode electrodes (Common Mode Sense
and Driven Right Leg). The sampling rate was 512 Hz. Acti-
View acquisition software (actiview.org) was used to
ensure offsets between 50 and −50 millivolts during
EEG set up, and to monitor online data acquisition.

Offline, EEG data were segmented and preprocessed
using the Electro Magnetic Encephalography Software
analysis package for MATLAB (EMEGS; emegs.org; Peyk,
De Cesarei, & Junghöfer, 2011). Data were processed
in close accordance with the guidelines of Junghöfer,
Elbert, Tucker, and Rockstroh (2000) through a method
known as statistical correction of artifacts in dense array
studies (SCADS). The data were adjusted using a low-pass
Butterworth filter with a stopband of 40 Hz and a passband
Di 30 Hz to control for high-frequency noise. A high-pass
Butterworth filter was also implemented with a passband

944

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Volume 35, Numero 6

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
5
6
9
4
1
2
0
8
2
9
2
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
9
8
4
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
7
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Di 0.1 Hz and stopband of 0.05 Hz. Data were segmented
from 100 msec before to 2000 msec after scene onset.
IL 100 msec of data before scene onset was used to
baseline-adjust the ERPs. Sensors and trials were screened
for high voltage artifacts identified through an automated
analysis of each electrode per trial in EMEGS in which
the median values of maximum amplitude, standard
deviation, and the maximum first derivative were used to
find unusable trials and unreliable electrodes. Data were
then transformed to an average reference (Peyk et al.,
2011) and was again screened for artifact contaminated
trials and sensors in the same automated process. Contam-
inated sensors were removed and replaced with values
calculated through a spherical spline interpolation
allowing the least noisy and closest electrodes to contrib-
ute the most to the new replacement amplitude. IL
data were kept in an average-reference that allows for
simple visualization of the EPN, which can be difficult
to identify with other reference positions such as the
mastoid (Junghöfer et al., 2006).

After the data were preprocessed, the ERPs for each
participant were extracted for each scene and an average
was calculated for each scene category. Electrodes and
latency windows representing the EPN and LPP were
derived by a previous study that used the same scene pre-
sentation procedure (Farkas et al., 2020). The EPN was
measured by extracting voltage from the lateral-posterior
electrodes P7, PO7, O1, P8, PO8, and O2 from 150 A
300 msec after scene onset (shown in Figure 4). The LPP
was recorded over midline parietal electrodes Cz, CPz, Pz,
CP1, and CP2 from 400 A 900 msec after scene onset
(shown in Figure 5).

Statistical Analyses

Omnibus repeated-measures ANOVAs were used to assess
the overall effect of scene content on the ERP measures,
with paired t tests used to break down specific category
differences. This was done for the variables of valence,
arousal, rated body exposure, rated body orientation,
and EPN and LPP amplitude as μvolt change from baseline.
All statistical tests were done with R software (r-project
.org). To address our hypotheses, we planned to use
paired t tests as a tool to make clear to the reader which
scene categories had a differential impact on ERP ampli-
tudes. Così, these are not post hoc comparisons in need
of a correction for multiple comparisons. Tuttavia, we
have included in the figure descriptions which compari-
sons would not survive Bonferroni correction (.001).

Our study question required the comparative assess-
ment of multiple SFs, which cannot be assumed to be
independent. To assess the impact of our many variables
by-scene conservatively, we used multiple regression and
four VSR regression methods to discern if and how SFs are
related to EPN and LPP modulation. Multiple regression
allowed us to use all SFs as predictors for each ERP within

the same model, where each variable acts as a covariate.
This can help to identify which variables account for com-
mon or unique variance. The VSR methods were used to
identify which SFs have the most meaningful relationships
with ERP modulation, and to minimize potential impact of
collinearity of our many variables. Whereas multiple
regression maximizes the amount of variance explained,
VSR methods use additional information to find the sim-
plest and most predictive multiple regression models for
a hypothetical future data set. In other words, the VSR
models allow us to identify which variables are most
meaningful by assessing whether these variables warranted
inclusion in the found models. By using four VSR methods,
we can assess the consistency of variable relationships that
were meaningful across models. Altogether, this multistep
approach should yield a more reliable estimate of the ways
in which our SFs are associated with EPN and LPP modula-
zione. These analyses were only used for the scenes that
depicted human content, where body-related SFs are rele-
vant to our hypotheses. The SFs used for the 90 human
scenes were valence, arousal, rated body exposure, rated
body orientation, body exposure pixels, low spatial fre-
quency, high spatial frequency, JPEG file size, and entropy.
Standard multiple regression and VSR models are
shown together in Table 2. The values for each variable
were transformed into standardized beta coefficients to
simplify interpretation of the strength of the relationship.
Statistical significance for each coefficient is presented
only for the standard multiple regression results. For
VSR models, only those variables that warranted inclusion
in the final model list a coefficient. The four VSR regression
methods used include (1) best-subset ranked by Bayesian
information criteria (BIC), (2) best-subset ranked by cross
validation, (3) LASSO regression, E (4) sparse LASSO
regression. Best-subset regression involves computing a
multiple regression for every combination of the predictor
variables. The best multiple regression model can then
be found based on an information criterion or cross valida-
tion prediction accuracy. Our first best-subset model was
found based on BIC, which balances the variance
explained by the model with a penalty for model complex-
ità. This process was implemented with the regsubsets
function contained in the leaps package (Lumley, 2017).
The next best-subset model was found with a fivefold cross
validation procedure repeated 1000 times. In this proce-
dure, each scene was randomly assigned to one of five
groups. All possible multiple regression models were then
fit to four of the groups. Prediction accuracy was then
found based on how well the models predicted ERP ampli-
tude for the fifth test group of scenes as measured by the
root-mean-square error (RMSE), which is the absolute pre-
diction error. The process was repeated with each of the
groups acting as the test data. The fivefold procedure was
repeated 1000 times to increase the reliability of the
risultati. The model with the lowest average RMSE from
all folds through 1000 iterations is presented here as the
best model. For both best-subset regression models, we

Farkas and Sabatinelli

945

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
5
6
9
4
1
2
0
8
2
9
2
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
9
8
4
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
7
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

9
4
6

J
o
tu
R
N
UN

l

o
F

C
o
G
N
io
T
io
v
e
N
e
tu
R
o
S
C
io
e
N
C
e

V
o
l
tu
M
e

3
5
,

N
tu
M
B
e
R

6

Tavolo 1. The Mean Values for Valence, Arousal, EPN, LPP, Body Exposure Rating, and Body Orientation Rating

Erotica

Sports
Victory

Neutro
Nudists

Neutro
People

Threatening
People

Mutilations

Pleasant
Animals

Neutro
Animals

Threatening
Animals

Repeated-measures
ANOVA Result

UGA sample N = 50
Valence (1–9)

5.64 (0.24)C

7.02 (0.18)B

4.80 (0.15)D

5.03 (0.48)D

2.90 (0.14)F

2.40 (0.18)G

8.09 (0.16)UN

5.86 (0.10)C

4.27 (0.15)E

Arousal (1–9)

EPN (μV)

6.52 (0.20)UN

5.86 (0.24)B

4.76 (0.15)C

3.80 (0.17)E

5.97 (0.20)B

6.15 (0.30)AB

4.97 (0.33)C

4.07 (0.18)D

6.03 (0.18)D

3.93 (0.68)B

6.89 (0.58)C

2.54 (0.64)UN

9.08 (0.58)E

7.11 (0.60)CD

7.97 (0.63)D

7.82 (0.57)D

8.87 (0.62)E

6.91 (0.64)C

LPP (μV)

4.96 (0.43)UN

0.40 (0.30)F

3.06 (0.44)B −1.54 (0.26)G

1.07 (0.34)DE

1.93 (0.44)C

1.03 (0.24)DE

0.47 (0.34)EF

1.56 (0.36)CD

Mturk sample N = 70

Body exposure

(1–9)

Body orientation

(1–9)

8.48 (0.05)B

3.85 (0.13)D

8.75 (0.04)UN

1.88 (0.10)F

2.17 (0.09)E

6.30 (0.21)C

3.63 (0.20)D

7.51 (0.15)AB

7.79 (0.21)UN

7.30 (0.13)B

5.99 (0.25)C

3.21 (0.14)E

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

F(8, 392), = 137.81,
p = .001; ηp
2 = .738

F(8, 392) = 25.02,
p = .001; ηp

2 = .338

F(8, 392) = 55.66,
p = .001; ηp

2 = .532

F(8, 392) = 46.75,
p = .001; ηp

2 = .488

F(5, 345) = 857.16,
p = .001; ηp

2 = .925

F(5, 345) = 167.66,
p = .001; ηp

2 = .708

Standard error for each is within the parentheses. Results of pairwise comparisons are depicted with the subscript letters. If two categories in the same row share the same letter, the categories were found
to not be statistically different at a critical p value of .05. The repeated-measures ANOVA results are presented in the final column.

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
5
6
9
4
1
2
0
8
2
9
2
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
9
8
4
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
7
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

report how the model fits on the overall set of scenes. Questo
is because best-subset regression is a variable selection
algorithm and does not return a model with coefficients
tuned for future use. The third VSR method used was
LASSO regression, which is a regularization method that
shrinks slope coefficients by a L1 penalty scaled by a tuning
parameter. The optimum tuning parameter was found
based on a fivefold × 1000 iteration cross validation pro-
cedure. Similar to best-subset regression, this method can
remove variables from the model that do not improve pre-
diction accuracy. Tuttavia, it is computationally less
expensive and returns a model that should be more pre-
dictive in a new data set. Sparse LASSO regression is the
model with the largest L1 penalty, which was still within
1 SE of the RMSE of the best LASSO regression. The sparse
model is nearly equivalent in accuracy, but yields fewer,
more essential predictor variables. A simpler model is gen-
erally preferred because it is easier to interpret and is less
susceptible to overfitting.

RESULTS

Scene Category AnalysesSelf Report

Valence was significantly different across scene categories
as found by a repeated-measures ANOVA, F(8, 392) =
137.81, P < .001; ηp 2 = .738. Overall category results can be seen in Table 1. Results of pairwise comparisons are depicted with the subscript letters, such that if two categories were statistically equivalent, they share the same letter. Figure 2 shows individual participant valence and arousal self-reports as well as the overall sample mean for each category. Pleasant animal scenes elicited the high- est valence ratings, which differed from the second-most pleasant category of victory scenes, t(49) = 7.28, p < .001, d = 1.03. Victory scenes elicited more rated pleasantness than the neutral animal category, t(49) = 6.48, p < .001, d = 0.92, as well as erotica scenes, t(49) = 4.70, p < .001, d = 0.67. Erotic scenes where not different than neutral animals valence ratings, but received higher pleasantness ratings than scenes of neutral people, t(49) = 2.43, p = .019, d = 0.34. Valence ratings did not differ between neu- tral people and nudist scenes. Nudist scenes elicited higher valence ratings than threatening animal scenes, t(49) = 2.77, p = .007, d = 0.39. However, the threatening animals evoked less valence ratings than threatening peo- ple scenes. Finally, mutilation scenes elicited the lowest valence ratings as compared with the second most unpleasant scene category of threatening people, t(49) = 2.98, p = .004, d = 0.42 (Table 2). A repeated-measures ANOVA was significant for the self-reports of emotional arousal across the nine scene categories, F(8, 392) = 25.02, p < .001; ηp 2 = .338. Erotic and mutilation scenes elicited the highest ratings of arousal and were not statistically different. Erotica scenes evoked more rated arousal than threatening animal scenes, t(49) = 2.51, p = .016, d = 0.35; threatening people scenes, t(49) = 2.23, p = .030, d = 0.32; and victorious l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 Figure 2. Self-reported valence and arousal for each category. Each gray dot is the category average for a participant (n = 50). Colored dots and error bars are the overall category average and standard error for all participants. Letters on the key indicate which categories were statistically different by paired t tests. If two categories share the same letter, there was not a significant difference between the categories at critical p value of .05. If corrected for multiple comparisons to a critical p value of .001, valence ratings would not be different between the pairs of erotica versus neutral people, nudists versus threatening animals, and mutilation versus threatening people. Using the same threshold, arousal ratings would not have differed for erotica versus threatening animals, erotica versus threatening people, erotica versus victorious athletes, victorious athletes versus pleasant animals, and neutral people versus neutral animals. / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Farkas and Sabatinelli 947 9 4 8 J o u r n a l o f C o g n i t i v e N e u r o s c i e n c e V o l u m e 3 5 , N u m b e r 6 Table 2. Multiple Regression and VSR Models Result for the 90 Human Scenes Multiple Regression Standardize Beta Coefficients Model Statistics Valence Arousal JPEG File Size Entropy Body Exposure Rating Body Orientation Rating Body Exposure Pixels Low-spatial Frequency High-spatial Frequency F p R 2 adj BIC RMSE EPN Standard multiple −.17 −.14 .01 .10 −.48** −.28* −.14 −.23 −.13 11.90 < .001 .52 410 −.74 −.64 −.49 −.46 .10 .08 −.27 −.37 −.22 −.10 .22 .19 −.01 −.12 −.10 392 396 1.93 2.08 2.16 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 −.00 .33** −.04 .11 .78** −.07 −.20* −.18 .06 29.58 < .001 .74 317 .36 .38 .32 .23 .69 .79 .66 .57 −.15 −.04 −.04 −.05 .01 297 297 1.14 1.21 1.25 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Model and variable statistical significance is only applicable for the standard multiple regression. For Models 2 through 5, variables can be deemed important if they warranted inclusion in the final reported model. * p < .05. ** p < .001. regression Best-subset regression (BIC) Best-subset regression (CV) −.17 LASSO regression −.15 −.10 Sparse LASSO regression LPP Standard multiple regression Best-subset regression (BIC) Best-subset regression (CV) LASSO regression Sparse LASSO regression athlete scenes, t(49) = 2.49, p = .016, d = 0.35. Mutila- tion, threatening animals, threatening people, and victori- ous athletes scenes were not different in self-reported arousal. Pleasant animal scenes evoked less arousal than victorious athlete scenes, t(49) = 2.32, p = .024, d = 0.33, but were not different in rated arousal from nudist scenes. Nudist scenes elicited more arousal than neutral animal scenes, t(49) = 3.39, p = .001, d = 0.48. Lastly, participants reported that neutral animal scenes were more arousing than the neutral people scenes, t(49) = 2.45, p = .018, d = 0.35. The Mturk sample reported the six human scene cate- gories had a different amount of exposed body parts as found through a repeated-measures ANOVA, F(5, 345) = 857.16, p < .001; ηp 2 = .925. Mturk ratings of body expo- sure and body orientation can be seen in Table 1 and Figure 3. The nudist category was perceived as having the most exposed body parts and were significantly differ- ent than erotic scenes, t(69) = 6.30, p < .001, d = 0.75. Erotic scenes were perceived as having more exposed body parts than mutilation scenes, t(69) = 10.93, p < .001, d = 1.31. Participants felt mutilation scenes had more body part exposure than victorious athlete scenes, t(69) = 12.25, p < .001, d = 1.46. In turn, victorious ath- lete scenes were rated as including less exposed body than threatening people scenes, t(69) = 19.30, p < .001, d = 2.31. Threatening people scenes were reported as more clothed than the final scene category of neutral people, t(69) = 6.51, p < .001, d = 0.78. Mturk reports of body orientation were different between scene categories, F(5, 345) = 167.66, p < .001, ηp 2 = .708. Participants rated the nudist category as having the most normally oriented depicted people, but the nudists scenes were not statistically different to the second highest rated category of victorious athletes. Nudist scenes were more normally oriented than neutral people scenes, t(69) = 2.16, p = .034, d = 0.26, but victorious ath- lete scenes were rated as not different than the neutral people category. The neutral people were more normally oriented than the threatening people scenes, t(69) = 5.26, p < .001, d = 0.63. The threatening people category was rated higher than the erotic scenes, t(69) = 7.77, p < .001, d = 0.93. Finally, the erotic scenes were reported as more normally oriented as the people in the mutilation scenes, t(69) = 2.18, p = .033, d = 0.26. Scene Category Analyses EPN. A repeated-measures ANOVA found significant modulation of the EPN across the nine scene categories, F(8, 392) = 55.66, p < .001; ηp 2 = .532. The EPN waveforms can be seen in Figure 4, whereas the amplitudes are shown in Table 1, including significant contrast effects, indicated by different subscript letters. Nudist scenes modulated the EPN the most and were significantly different than modulation evoked by erotic scenes, t(49) = 4.41, p < .001, d = 0.62. EPN modulation from erotic scenes were greater than modulation elicited by victorious athletes, l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 Figure 3. Perceived body exposure and body orientation per each category from the Mturk sample (n = 70). Each gray dot represents the average rating for each participant, whereas the colored dots and error bars represent the overall average and standard error, respectively. In the legend, if categories do not share a letter, they were statistically different at a critical p value of .05. If corrected for multiple comparisons to a critical p value of .001, all category differences in body exposure ratings would still be statistically significant. Using the same threshold, body orientation ratings would not have statistically differed for nudists versus neutral people and erotica versus mutilation scenes. / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Farkas and Sabatinelli 949 Figure 4. The top depicts the average participant and category EPN amplitude by category. The bottom displays the EPN waveform by category. In the legend, if categories do not share a letter, they were statistically different at a critical p value of .05. If corrected for multiple comparisons to a critical p value of .001, the EPN would not have statistically differed for pleasant animals versus victorious athletes, pleasant animals versus threatening animals, pleasant animals versus neutral animals, mutilation versus neutral animals, and mutilation versus neutral people. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 t(49) = 8.00, p < .001, d = 1.13; threatening animals, t(49) = −7.50, p < .001, d = 1.06; and threatening people scenes, t(49) = 8.59, p < .001, d = 1.21. However, the three categories of victorious athletes, threatening animals, and threatening people did not differ significantly in EPN modulation. Pleasant animal scenes modulated the EPN less than victorious athlete scenes, t(49) = 2.61, p = .012, d = 0.37, and the threatening animal scenes, Figure 5. The top depicts the average participant and category LPP amplitude by category. The bottom displays the LPP waveform by category. In the legend, if categories do not share a letter, they were statistically different at a critical p value of .05. If corrected for multiple comparisons to a critical p value of .001, the LPP would not have statistically differed for nudists versus mutilation, nudists versus threatening animals, mutilation versus threatening people, mutilation versus pleasant animals, threatening animals versus neutral animals, threatening animals versus victorious athletes, and threatening people versus victorious athletes. 950 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 35, Number 6 Figure 6. Scene category topographies for the EPN (150–300 msec) and LPP (400–900 msec) visualized as the difference in microvolts from the neutral people and animal categories. Each color represents a range of 1 microvolt, such that the darkest blue represents a microvoltage difference between −10 and −9. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Farkas and Sabatinelli 951 t(49) = 2.54, p = .014, d = 0.36. However, the pleasant animal, threatening people, and mutilation scenes did not differ in EPN amplitude significantly. Mutilation scenes modulated the EPN more than neutral animals, t(49) = 2.34, p = .023, d = 0.33, and neutral people scenes, t(49) = 2.45, p = .018, d = 0.35. Neutral animal and neu- tral people scenes did not differ in EPN modulation. LPP. A repeated-measures ANOVA found a difference in LPP modulation across the nine scene categories, F(8, 392) = 46.75, p < .001; ηp 2 = .488. The LPP waveforms can be seen in Figure 5, whereas the amplitudes are shown in Table 1, including contrast effects. Erotic scenes modulated the LPP the most and was statistically different than the next most potent scene category of nudists, t(49) = 6.11, p < .001, d = 0.86. Nudist scenes modulated the LPP more than mutilation, t(49) = 2.94, p = .005, d = 0.42, and threatening animal scenes, t(49) = 3.21, p = .002, d = 0.45. However, mutilation and threatening animal scenes statistically did not differ in LPP amplitude. Mutilation scenes evoked more LPP modulation than threatening people scenes, t(49) = 2.85, p = .006, d = 0.40, and pleasant animals scenes, t(49) = 2.14, p = .037, d = 0.30. Threatening animal modulation was not different to the modulation of threatening people and pleasant animal categories. Neutral animal scenes elicited less LPP modulation than the threatening animal scenes, t(49) = 2.82, p = .007, d = 0.40, but the neutral animals were not different than threatening people and happy animal scenes. Victorious athlete scenes modulated the LPP less than threatening people, t(49) = 2.15, p = .037, d = 0.30, and threatening animal scenes, t(49) = 3.29, p = .002, d = 0.46, whereas the victorious athlete scenes were not different to modulation by neutral animal scenes. Neutral people scenes modulated the LPP less than all other cate- gories including the next closest category of victorious athletes, t(49) = 7.30, p < .001, d = 1.03 (Figure 6). By Scene Analyses EPN. The EPN standard multiple regression was signifi- cant, F(9, 80) = 11.90, p < .001, R2 adj = .52. Notably, rated arousal was not significantly associated with EPN modula- tion (β = −.17, p = .164). Scenes, which were rated with high body exposure (β = −.48, p < .001) and upright ori- entation (β = −.28, p = .024) were associated with greater EPN modulation. The best-subset regression based on BIC included only rated body exposure (β = −.74) and body orientation (β = −.27) as meaningful. The best-subset regression based on prediction accuracy (RMSE =1.93) included arousal (β = −.17), rated body exposure (β = −.64), rated body orientation (β = −.37), low-spatial fre- quency (β = .22), and high-spatial frequency (β = −.12) as meaningful. The most predictive LASSO regression (RMSE = 2.08) included all predictor variables except JPEG file size. The sparse LASSO regression (RMSE = 2.16) included body exposure ratings (β = −.46), entropy (β = .08), body orientation ratings (β = −.01), and low- spatial frequency (β = .01) as meaningful. LPP. A standard multiple regression of LPP modulation across the 90 human scene set was significant, F(9, 80) = adj = .74. Higher rated arousal (β = 29.58, p < .001, R2 .33, p < .001) and rated body exposure (β = .78, p < .001) was associated with a larger LPP amplitude, whereas the sum of body exposure pixels (β = −.20, p = .024) had a negative relationship with the LPP. The best-subset model based on BIC included rated body exposure (β = .69) and rated arousal (β = .36). The best-subset model based on prediction accuracy (RMSE = 1.14) included arousal (β = .38), rated body exposure (β = .79), and body exposure pixels (β = −.15). The most predictive LASSO regression (RMSE = 1.21) included rated arousal (β = .32), body expo- sure ratings (β = .66), body orientation ratings (β = −.04), body exposure pixels (β = −.04), low-spatial frequency (β = −.05), and high-spatial frequency (β = .01). The sparse LASSO regression (RMSE = 1.25) included body exposure ratings (β = .57) and arousal ratings (β = .23). DISCUSSION In this study, we directly assessed the extent to which the emotion-modulated EPN may be sensitive to the presence of exposed body parts in scenes, independent from rated arousal. Across the wide range of scenes selected, the results replicated our initial study (Farkas et al., 2020) and supported the hypothesis that highly exposed bodies and upright body orientation together are strong predic- tors of EPN amplitude, whereas the rated arousal of scenes was weakly predictive. This pattern contrasts with modu- lation of the LPP, which consistently reflected scene arousal ratings. The scene category effect size differences of the EPN and LPP suggest a different hierarchy of sensitivity. Nudist scenes clearly elicited the largest EPN, showing a medium/large 0.62 effect size over erotica, the second- most effective category. This difference is equivalent to the 0.61 effect size between the remaining emotional scene categories and neutral scenes. Averaged together, the nudist and erotica scenes showed a very large 1.38 effect size compared with all other emotional categories. Conversely, erotic scenes elicited the largest LPP, showing a 0.86 effect size over nudists, the second-most effective category. This difference is similar in size to the 1.00 effect between the remaining emotional scene categories and neutral scenes. Averaged together, LPP modulation to the nudist and erotica scenes showed a 1.26 effect size dif- ference as compared with all other emotional categories. The regression models fit to the 90 individual scenes showed that body exposure ratings were a reliable predic- tor of EPN and LPP amplitude, whereas standardized beta- coefficients indicate that body exposure ratings had the strongest relationship with the EPN. Notably, rated arousal was not significantly related to EPN modulation in the 952 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 35, Number 6 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 standard multiple regression, and was selected in just two of the four VSR models. For LPP modulation, rated arousal and body exposure ratings were consistently strong predictors. These data suggest that the EPN may reflect the early activation of brain networks that function to enhance the perceptual processing of classes of stimuli that reliably predict relevant outcomes (Frank & Sabatinelli, 2017; Bradley, 2009; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1998). Specifi- cally, scenes clearly featuring people may enhance EPN modulation because people are associated with aversive and appetitive experience, and it may be that the more clearly a person is depicted in a scene (e.g., upright, unclothed), the larger the EPN, possibly reflecting reen- trant feedback from downstream ventral visual structures (Kravitz, Saleem, Baker, Ungerleider, & Mishkin, 2013). After this potentially meaningful cue is further processed across the brain, LPP modulation strongly reflects the rated emotional intensity of the scene, while being less sensitive to clearly visible people. This might explain the pronounced sensitivity of the EPN to nudist, relative to erotic scenes, whereas the LPP shows the reverse pattern. Despite the apparent bias toward bodies, the EPN remains sensitive to nonbody emotional cues as well. This is evident in the EPN modulation by pleasant (D = 0.39) and unpleasant (D = 0.63) animals scenes relative to neu- tral animal scenes in the current study, and in published work using emotional faces (Schupp, Öhman, et al., 2004), hand gestures (Flaisch et al., 2009), and even words (Kissler et al., 2007). Body-specific Activity in the Visual System There is considerable evidence that body percepts have a privileged status in the human visual system. Studies with newborns have found that upright bodies draw attention more than inverted bodies or other stimuli (Filippetti, Johnson, Lloyd-Fox, Dragovic, & Farroni, 2013). fMRI studies have identified a region of lateral occipital cortex that is particularly sensitive to line-drawings of bodies relative to tools (Peelen & Downing, 2007). In the ERP literature, the perception of a headless body on a blank background can elicit a potential similar to the N170 elicited by faces. Although this body-evoked N170 appears at a more anterior scalp location to the EPN and dissipates by 210 msec (Thierry et al., 2006), it has been shown that a greater proportion of skin exposure increases the ampli- tude of this potential (Alho, Salminen, Sams, Hietanen, & Nummenmaa, 2015; Hietanen, Kirjavainen, & Nummenmaa, 2014; Hietanen & Nummenmaa, 2011). These findings suggest that the human visual system is especially sensi- tive to body cues. If bodies elicit a pronounced EPN amplitude because the visual system is tuned to identify these cues, then the EPN could be a useful metric of other features for which the visual system shows a similar processing bias. In other words, the EPN may be less an index of emotional perception, and more a measure of the degree of early recognition of features that predict emotional percepts. Consistent with this distinction, snakes also appear to show particularly efficient processing in the visual system (Soares, Lindstrom, Esteves, & Öhman, 2014; Öhman & Mineka, 2001), and also elicit larger EPN amplitudes than would be expected as compared with arousal ratings and LPP mod- ulation (Van Strien, Eijlers, Franken, & Huijding, 2014; Van Strien, Franken, & Huijding, 2014). Naturally, those cues that predict emotional percepts will most often be emo- tional, and thus elicit similar EPN and LPP modulation, in keeping with rated arousal. Because of its early latency and broad reactivity to naturalistic scenes, the EPN may enable research to identify the nature and extent of these visual features, and thus help to define the subtle mecha- nisms of emotional perception in large human samples. Limitations and Future Directions Although this work expanded the variety of scene stimuli and feature quantification from earlier efforts, it could still benefit from more heterogeneity of scenes, additional measures of experienced arousal, and more precise calcu- lations of exposed body parts. To understand how specific scene content and features affected EPN modulation, we curated scenes that were quite similar in the content within each category. Although this was useful and inten- tional for the main objectives of this study, it is possible that discrete content categories affect our by-scene analy- ses. Potentially, this could lead to what is referred to as a Simpson’s paradox, in which the overall relationship does not reflect associations within each category. This concern could be addressed in future studies by evaluating the rela- tionship between perceived body exposure and EPN amplitude for a larger, more diverse scene set. Separately, the LPP results suggest that nudist scenes may have been more arousing than the participants self-reported. This is a difficult issue, but future studies could include physiolog- ical measures of arousal such as skin conductance or pupil diameter. Lastly, although the sum of pixels that contained exposed body parts was associated with EPN modulation, it did not capture the effect as well as self-reported body exposure. Although we aimed to keep the action of a scene consistently placed in the middle, and within a stan- dard depth of field, the distance of body parts from the camera lens does vary across scenes, and may have an impact on the utility of this index. Future studies could also investigate if there are intermediate-level visual fea- tures of a body that are especially potent modulators of the EPN. A computer simulation study suggests that small fragments of images, such as a part of a face, can provide useful information for accurate classification (Ullman, Vidal-Naquet, & Sali, 2002). Using a similar approach with fragmented pictures of bodies may reveal which features are most influential in body classification, and if the EPN is modulated by specific body features, or the recognition of a full body. Farkas and Sabatinelli 953 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Consistent with prior work that manipulated scene complexity (Nordström & Wiens, 2012; Löw et al., 2013; Bradley et al., 2007), a subset of our analyses suggest that spatial frequency of scenes modulated EPN amplitude, despite the fact that the scene set was composed to be equivalent in JPEG file size across categories. The use of additional indices of scene complexity will be needed to resolve this issue more fully, but we are encouraged by the relationship between body exposure ratings and ERP modulation, which strongly surpassed the modest relationship between low spatial frequency and the ERPs. Considering that the original EPN study (Junghöfer et al., 2001) employed nearly the entire IAPS set (1000+ scenes) and did not identify any reliable relationships with scene color, luminance, or spatial frequency, we are rea- sonably confident that the nudist-erotica EPN effect is not a result of low-level perceptual differences. Conclusion These data suggest a refinement in a common interpreta- tion of what the EPN component represents in visual per- ception. Researchers have generally concluded that the EPN is modulated primarily by arousing content, much like the LPP (Frank & Sabatinelli, 2019; Sabatinelli et al., 2013; Olofsson et al., 2008; Schupp, Flaisch, Stockburger, & Junghöfer, 2006). The current findings suggest that scenes depicting unclothed bodies in upright postures elicit a greater EPN independent of arousal ratings and LPP amplitude. Thus, in addition to reflecting motivational relevance in general, this EPN sensitivity might be a result of a learned or phylogenetic tendency to attend to human bodies. This ability to distinguish the impact of SFs on early and late stage emotion-modulated ERPs could enable future studies to identify other visual features that show this dis- tinction, thus providing a means to differentiate the mech- anisms of emotional perception with noninvasive measures. Acknowledgments We sincerely thank Heather DiGregorio, Hannah Myers, and Ansley Quinn Delaney for their help on data acquisition and management. Reprint requests should be sent to Andrew H. Farkas, Depart- ment of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, or via e-mail: andrew.farkas@uga.edu. Data Availability Statement Data and experimental materials from this study will be made available upon request to the authors. Author Contributions Andrew H. Farkas: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Project administration; Software; Supervision; Validation; Visualization; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review & editing. Dean Sabatinelli: Conceptualization; Formal anal- ysis; Funding acquisition; Methodology; Project adminis- tration; Resources; Supervision; Validation; Visualization; Writing—Review & editing. Diversity in Citation Practices Retrospective analysis of the citations in every article pub- lished in this journal from 2010 to 2021 reveals a persistent pattern of gender imbalance: Although the proportions of authorship teams (categorized by estimated gender iden- tification of first author/last author) publishing in the Jour- nal of Cognitive Neuroscience ( JoCN) during this period were M(an)/M = .407, W(oman)/M = .32, M/ W = .115, and W/ W = .159, the comparable proportions for the arti- cles that these authorship teams cited were M/M = .549, W/M = .257, M/ W = .109, and W/ W = .085 (Postle and Fulvio, JoCN, 34:1, pp. 1–3). Consequently, JoCN encour- ages all authors to consider gender balance explicitly when selecting which articles to cite and gives them the oppor- tunity to report their article’s gender citation balance. The authors of this article report its proportions of citations by gender category to be as follows: M/M = .521; W/M = .271; M/ W = .125; W/ W = .083. REFERENCES Alho, J., Salminen, N., Sams, M., Hietanen, J. K., & Nummenmaa, L. (2015). Facilitated early cortical processing of nude human bodies. Biological Psychology, 109, 103–110. https://doi.org /10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.04.010, PubMed: 25960070 Bradley, M. M. (2009). Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology, 46, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1111 /j.1469-8986.2008.00702.x, PubMed: 18778317 Bradley, M. M., Hamby, S., Löw, A., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Brain potentials in perception: Picture complexity and emotional arousal. Psychophysiology, 44, 364–373. https://doi.org/10 .1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00520.x, PubMed: 17433095 Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9, PubMed: 7962581 Caplar, N., Tacchella, S., & Birrer, S. (2017). Quantitative evaluation of gender bias in astronomical publications from citation counts. Nature Astronomy, 1, 1–5. https://doi.org/10 .1038/s41550-017-0141 Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology, 52, 95–111. https://doi.org/10 .1016/S0301-0511(99)00044-7, PubMed: 10699350 De Cesarei, A., & Codispoti, M. (2013). Spatial frequencies and emotional perception. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 24, 89–104. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2012-0053, PubMed: 23183741 Dion, M. L., Sumner, J. L., & Mitchell, S. M. (2018). Gendered citation patterns across political science and social science methodology fields. Political Analysis, 26, 312–327. https:// doi.org/10.7910/DVN/R7AQT1 954 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 35, Number 6 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Donderi, D. C. (2006). An information theory analysis of visual complexity and dissimilarity. Perception, 35, 823–835. https:// doi.org/10.1068/p5249, PubMed: 16836047 Dworkin, J. D., Linn, K. A., Teich, E. G., Zurn, P., Shinohara, R. T., & Bassett, D. S. (2020). The extent and drivers of gender imbalance in neuroscience reference lists. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 918–926. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593 -020-0658-y 5, PubMed: 32561883 Farkas, A. H., Oliver, K. I., & Sabatinelli, D. (2020). Emotional and feature-based modulation of the early posterior negativity. Psychophysiology, 57, e13484. https://doi.org/10 .1111/psyp.13484, PubMed: 31573679 Filippetti, M. L., Johnson, M. H., Lloyd-Fox, S., Dragovic, D., & Farroni, T. (2013). Body perception in newborns. Current Biology, 23, 2413–2416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10 .017, PubMed: 24268410 Flaisch, T., Schupp, H. T., Renner, B., & Junghöfer, M. (2009). Neural systems of visual attention responding to emotional gestures. Neuroimage, 45, 1339–1346. https://doi.org/10 .1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.073, PubMed: 19349245 Frank, D. W., & Sabatinelli, D. (2017). Primate visual perception: Motivated attention in naturalistic scenes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 226. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00226, PubMed: 28265250 Frank, D. W., & Sabatinelli, D. (2019). Hemodynamic and electrocortical reactivity to specific scene contents in emotional perception. Psychophysiology, 56, e13340. https:// doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13340, PubMed: 30776100 Fulvio, J. M., Akinnola, I., & Postle, B. R. (2021). Gender (im)balance in citation practices in cognitive neuroscience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 33, 3–7. https://doi.org /10.1162/jocn_a_01643, PubMed: 33078992 Herbert, C., Junghöfer, M., & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology, 45, 487–498. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469 -8986.2007.00638.x, PubMed: 18221445 Herbert, C., Sfärlea, A., & Blumenthal, T. (2013). Your emotion or mine: Labeling feelings alters emotional face perception— An ERP study on automatic and intentional affect labeling. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 378. https://doi.org/10 .3389/fnhum.2013.00378, PubMed: 23888134 Hietanen, J. K., Kirjavainen, I., & Nummenmaa, L. (2014). Additive effects of affective arousal and top-down attention on the event-related brain responses to human bodies. Biological Psychology, 103, 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .biopsycho.2014.09.003, PubMed: 25224182 Hietanen, J. K., & Nummenmaa, L. (2011). The naked truth: The face and body sensitive N170 response is enhanced for nude bodies. PLoS One, 6, e24408. https://doi.org/10.1371 /journal.pone.0024408, PubMed: 22110574 Jaworska, N., Thompson, A., Shah, D., Fisher, D., Ilivitsky, V., & Knott, V. (2012). Acute tryptophan depletion effects on the vertex and late positive potentials to emotional faces in individuals with a family history of depression. Neuropsychobiology, 65, 28–40. https://doi.org/10.1159/000328992, PubMed: 22094352 Junghöfer, M., Bradley, M. M., Elbert, T. R., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Fleeting images: A new look at early emotion discrimination. Psychophysiology, 38, 175–178. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469 -8986.3820175, PubMed: 11347862 Junghöfer, M., Elbert, T., Tucker, D. M., & Rockstroh, B. (2000). Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. Psychophysiology, 37, 523–532. https://doi.org/10 .1111/1469-8986.3740523, PubMed: 10934911 Junghöfer, M., Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M. M., Schupp, H. T., Elbert, T. R., & Lang, P. J. (2006). Fleeting images: Rapid affect discrimination in visual cortex. NeuroReport, 17, 225–229. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000198437 .59883.bb, PubMed: 16407776 Kissler,J.,Herbert,C.,Peyk,P.,&Junghöfer,M.(2007).Buzzwords: Early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychological Science, 18, 475–480. https://doi.org/10.1111/j .1467-9280.2007.01924.x, PubMed: 17576257 Kissler, J., & Herbert, C. (2013). Emotion, Etmnooi, or Emitoon?–faster lexical access to emotional than to neutral words during reading. Biological Psychology, 92, 464–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.09.004, PubMed: 23059636 Kravitz, D. J., Saleem, K. S., Baker, C. I., Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (2013). The ventral visual pathway: An expanded neural framework for the processing of object quality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 26–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics .2012.10.011, PubMed: 23265839 Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248–1263. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00275-3, PubMed: 9861468 Langeslag, S. J., Gootjes, L., & van Strien, J. W. (2018). The effect of mouth opening in emotional faces on subjective experience and the early posterior negativity amplitude. Brain and Cognition, 127, 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .bandc.2018.10.003, PubMed: 30316954 Liu, X., Hairston, J., Schrier, M., & Fan, J. (2011). Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1219–1236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.012, PubMed: 21185861 Löw, A., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2013). Perceptual processing of natural scenes at rapid rates: Effects of complexity, content, and emotional arousal. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 860–868. https:// doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0179-1, PubMed: 23780520 Lumley, T. (2017). Leaps: Regression subset selection. R package version 3.0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=leaps Maliniak, D., Powers, R., & Walter, B. F. (2013). The gender citation gap in international relations. International Organization, 67, 889–922. https://doi.org/10.1017 /S0020818313000209 Mitchell, S. M., Lange, S., & Brus, H. (2013). Gendered citation patterns in international relations journals. International Studies Perspectives, 14, 485–492. https://doi.org/10.1111 /insp.12026 Moratti, S., Saugar, C., & Strange, B. A. (2011). Prefrontal- occipitoparietal coupling underlies late latency human neuronal responses to emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 17278–17286. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2917 -11.2011, PubMed: 22114294 Nordström, H., & Wiens, S. (2012). Emotional event-related potentials are larger to figures than scenes but are similarly reduced by inattention. BMC Neuroscience, 13, 49. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-13-49, PubMed: 22607397 Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483. https://doi.org/10 .1037//0033-295X.108.3.483, PubMed: 11488376 Olofsson, J. K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., & Polich, J. (2008). Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings. Biological Psychology, 77, 247–265. https://doi.org /10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006, PubMed: 18164800 Pastor, M. C., Bradley, M. M., Löw, A., Versace, F., Moltó, J., & Lang, P. J. (2008). Affective picture perception: Emotion, Farkas and Sabatinelli 955 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 context, and the late positive potential. Brain Research, 1189, 145–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.072, PubMed: 18068150 Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 636–648. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2195, PubMed: 17643089 Peyk, P., De Cesarei, A., & Junghöfer, M. (2011). ElectroMagnetoEncephalography software: Overview and integration with other EEG/MEG toolboxes. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 861705. https://doi.org /10.1155/2011/861705, PubMed: 21577273 Sabatinelli, D., Keil, A., Frank, D. W., & Lang, P. J. (2013). Emotional perception: Correspondence of early and late event-related potentials with cortical and subcortical functional MRI. Biological Psychology, 92, 513–519. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.04.005, PubMed: 22560889 Schindler, S., & Kissler, J. (2016). Selective visual attention to emotional words: Early parallel frontal and visual activations followed by interactive effects in visual cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 3575–3587. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23261, PubMed: 27218232 Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2004). The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: An ERP analysis. Psychophysiology, 41, 441–449. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00174.x, PubMed: 15102130 Schupp, H. T., Öhman, A., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., Stockburger, J., & Hamm, A. O. (2004). The facilitated processing of threatening faces: An ERP analysis. Emotion, 4, 189–200. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.189, PubMed: 15222855 Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention: Event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31–51. https://doi .org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56002-9, PubMed: 17015073 Schupp, H. T., Stockburger, J., Codispoti, M., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2006). Stimulus novelty and emotion perception: The near absence of habituation in the visual cortex. NeuroReport, 17, 365–369. https://doi.org/10 .1097/01.wnr.0000203355.88061.c6, PubMed: 16514360 Schupp, H. T., Stockburger, J., Codispoti, M., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2007). Selective visual attention to emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1082–1089. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3223-06 .2007, PubMed: 17267562 Soares, S. C., Lindstrom, B., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (2014). The hidden snake in the grass: Superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PLoS One, 9, e114724. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114724, PubMed: 25493937 Thierry, G., Pegna, A. J., Dodds, C., Roberts, M., Basan, S., & Downing, P. (2006). An event-related potential component sensitive to images of the human body. Neuroimage, 32, 871–879. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.060, PubMed: 16750639 Thigpen, N. N., Keil, A., & Freund, A. M. (2018). Responding to emotional scenes: Effects of response outcome and picture repetition on reaction times and the late positive potential. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 24–36. https://doi.org/10.1080 /02699931.2016.1266305, PubMed: 27922339 Ullman, S., Vidal-Naquet, M., & Sali, E. (2002). Visual features of intermediate complexity and their use in classification. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 682–687. https://doi.org/10.1038 /nn870, PubMed: 12055634 Van Strien, J. W., Eijlers, R., Franken, I. H. A., & Huijding, J. (2014). Snake pictures draw more early attention than spider pictures in non-phobic women: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biological Psychology, 96, 150–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.12.014, PubMed: 24374241 Van Strien, J. W., Franken, I. H., & Huijding, J. (2014). Testing the snake-detection hypothesis: Larger early posterior negativity in humans to pictures of snakes than to pictures of other reptiles, spiders and slugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 691. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014 .00691, PubMed: 25237303 Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Beyond good and evil: The time-course of neural activity elicited by specific picture content. Emotion, 10, 767–782. https://doi.org/10.1037 /a0020242, PubMed: 21058848 Willenbockel, V., Sadr, J., Fiset, D., Horne, G. O., Gosselin, F., & Tanaka, J. W. (2010). Controlling low-level image properties: The SHINE toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 671–684. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.671, PubMed: 20805589 Yoon, S., Shim, M., Kim, H. S., & Lee, S. H. (2016). Enhanced early posterior negativity to fearful faces in patients with anxiety disorder. Brain Topography, 29, 262–272. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-015-0456-0, PubMed: 26476635 956 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 35, Number 6 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 5 6 9 4 1 2 0 8 2 9 2 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 9 8 4 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image
Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image
Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image
Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image
Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image
Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image
Emotional Perception: Divergence of Early and Late image

Scarica il pdf