Creative Connections: Computational Semantic

Creative Connections: Computational Semantic
Distance Captures Individual Creativity and
Resting-State Functional Connectivity

William Orwig1

, Ibai Diez1, Patrizia Vannini1,2, Roger Beaty3

, and Jorge Sepulcre1

Astratto

■ Recent studies of creative cognition have revealed interac-
tions between functional brain networks involved in the gener-
ation of novel ideas; Tuttavia, the neural basis of creativity is
highly complex and presents a great challenge in the field of
cognitive neuroscience, partly because of ambiguity around
how to assess creativity. We applied a novel computational
method of verbal creativity assessment—semantic distance—
and performed weighted degree functional connectivity analy-
ses to explore how individual differences in assembly of
resting-state networks are associated with this objective creativ-
ity assessment. To measure creative performance, a sample of
healthy adults (n = 175) completed a battery of divergent think-
ing (DT) compiti, in which they were asked to think of unusual
uses for everyday objects. Computational semantic models were
applied to calculate the semantic distance between objects and

responses to obtain an objective measure of DT performance.
All participants underwent resting-state imaging, from which
we computed voxel-wise connectivity matrices between all gray
matter voxels. A linear regression analysis was applied between
DT and weighted degree of the connectivity matrices. Our anal-
ysis revealed a significant connectivity decrease in the visual-
temporal and parietal regions, in relation to increased levels of
DT. Link-level analyses showed higher local connectivity within
visual regions was associated with lower DT, whereas projec-
tions from the precuneus to the right inferior occipital and
temporal cortex were positively associated with DT. Our results
demonstrate differential patterns of resting-state connectivity
associated with individual creative thinking ability, extending
past work using a new application to automatically assess crea-
tivity via semantic distance.

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
3
3
4
9
9
1
8
6
2
6
1
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
6
5
8
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

INTRODUCTION

Creative thinking is essential to all human progress and in-
novation. Recent studies of functional neuroimaging and
network neuroscience have revealed interaction between
large-scale brain networks associated with creative cogni-
zione. The neuroscience of creativity seeks to disentangle
these complex brain processes that facilitate the generation
of novel ideas. Creativity has been defined as the produc-
tion of novel and useful ideas to solve problems (Guilford,
1967), and it is often assessed with tasks of divergent think-
ing (DT), which require the production of multiple solu-
tions to open-ended problems. A classic test of DT, IL
Alternative Uses Task (AUT), prompts participants to gen-
erate alternative uses for a common object. To assess the
creative quality of ideas, traditional approaches to DT as-
sessment have largely relied on human raters, a subjective
and labor-intensive procedure. Recent developments in
creativity assessment have sought to standardize and
automate creativity assessment by applying computational
measures of semantic distance (Beaty & Johnson, 2020;

1Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School,
2Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School,
3Pennsylvania State University

© 2020 Istituto di Tecnologia del Massachussetts

Dumas, Organisciak, & Doherty, 2020; Kenett & Faust,
2019; Heinen & Johnson, 2018; Prabhakaran, Verde, &
Gray, 2014). The application of semantic distance to creativ-
ity assessment is based on the associative theory of
creativity (Kenett & Faust, 2019; Mednick, 1962), Quale
characterizes creative thought as a novel and useful recom-
bination of semantic knowledge; così, the integration of
more semantically distant concepts is considered more
creative. This study applies semantic distance models to au-
tomatically and objectively assess DT. Past work has largely
focused on human ratings of creativity, so it is unknown
whether similar neural mechanisms underlie objective as-
sessments of creative thinking that do not rely on the sub-
jective judgments of human raters. Given high correlations
reported between human ratings and semantic distance
(Beaty & Johnson, 2020; Dumas et al., 2020), we expect
to find similar (though perhaps nonredundant) neural pat-
terns for human ratings and semantic distance measures of
DT. We measure semantic distance via a latent semantic dis-
tance factor—a combination of computational semantic
models that show a high correspondence to human ratings
of novelty and creativity (Beaty & Johnson, 2020).
Automated assessments of creativity via semantic distance
represents a new direction in the field of creativity research,
permitting an analysis of the extent to which neural

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 33:3, pag. 499–509
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01658

correlates of creativity overlap when creativity is assessed
by humans versus machines.

Advances in neuroimaging techniques have enabled re-
searchers to study functional networks involved in creative
cognition. One approach that has been employed is func-
tional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging, Quale
allows researchers to measure dynamic interactions be-
tween brain regions. Resting-state functional connectivity
magnetic resonance imaging studies have begun to disso-
ciate large-scale functional networks underlying cognitive
and attentional control processes relevant for creative cog-
nition (Beaty, Benedek, Silvia, & Schacter, 2016). Among
the most well studied of these functional networks, IL
Default Mode Network (DMN)—composed of a set of
midline and posterior inferior parietal regions—shows
increased activation in the absence of an externally pre-
sented stimulus (Raichle et al., 2001). DMN activity is
associated with self-generated thought, such as mind
wandering and imagination (Andrews-Hanna, Smallwood,
& Spreng, 2014; Buckner, Andrews-Hanna, & Schacter,
2008). It has been suggested that activity in the DMN
contributes to the generation of candidate ideas, whereas
executive control networks exert top–down monitoring to
meet specific task goals or constraints (Beaty et al., 2016;
Beaty, Silvia, Nusbaum, Jauk, & Benedek, 2014). Task-based
approaches have helped to uncover the dynamic relation-
ship of these large-scale networks during creative task per-
formance (Beaty, Kenett, et al., 2018; Shi et al., 2018). In
aggiunta, resting-state studies have shown that temporal
variability of DMN connectivity correlates with DT and fre-
quency of transitions between functional connectivity-states
are associated with creative ability (Feng et al., 2019; Sun
et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2018; Gao et al., 2017; Li et al., 2017;
Takeuchi et al., 2012). Seed-based connectivity analyses in
resting-state fMRI data have revealed greater connectivity
between inferior pFC and DMN to be associated with idea
generation (Vartanian et al., 2018; Wei et al., 2014). IL
objective of the present work was not necessarily to replicate
previous findings of the interaction between default mode
and executive control networks; we do not expect to find
consistent patterns of such network connectivity previously
reported in task-based studies. Piuttosto, we apply novel
semantic distance measures and data-driven graph theory
methods to resting-state data, exploring the overlap between
human- and computationally derived creativity metrics.

Illuminating the specific contributions of DMN connec-
tivity to the production of novel ideas continues to be a
topic of great interest in the neuroscience of creativity
literature. Internal attention requires the disengagement
from immediate sensory information and may reflect a
more abstract cognition associated with creativity.
Internally directed cognition, which involves the shielding
of internal processes from external stimuli, has been associ-
ated with extended deactivation of occipital and parietal
regions (Benedek et al., 2016). A recent series of neuroim-
aging studies have focused on the role of internally directed
attention in creativity and mental imagery (Fink & Benedek,

2019; Benedek, 2018; Fink et al., 2014). Attenuation of visual
input has also been associated with creative insight (Salvi
& Bowden, 2016; Salvi, Bricolo, Franconeri, Kounios, &
Beeman, 2015). Inoltre, occipital and parietal regions,
such as the lateral occipital cortex and the inferior parie-
tal lobule, have been critically involved in the integration
of sensory perception toward multisensory and associa-
tion cortices (Diez et al., 2019; Sepulcre, 2014; Sepulcre,
Sabuncu, Sì, Liu, & Johnson, 2012). Creative individuals
may exhibit a higher degree of internally directed cognition
at rest, reflected in lower connectivity within primary visual
systems and increased connectivity to areas of multimodal
integration.

Applications of graph theory in neuroscience have
emerged as an effective approach to studying functional
networks of the brain (van den Heuvel & Hulshoff Pol,
2010). Network analysis offers insight into the structural
and functional organization of human brain networks
(Sporns, Chialvo, Kaiser, & Hilgetag, 2004), with brain
regions represented as nodes and the relationships
between nodes (cioè., connectivity) represented as edges
(Sepulcre, Sabuncu, & Goñi, 2014; Bullmore & Bassett,
2011; Bullmore & Sporns, 2009). Individual variability in
functional network organization provides a lens to study
the relationship between brain morphology and cognitive
ability. Several network metrics have been developed to
quantify different properties of network architecture. One
such measure is the degree of a node, the number of edges
that connects it to the rest of the network (Sepulcre et al.,
2012; Bullmore & Bassett, 2011; Buckner et al., 2009).
Weighted degree ( WD) provides a measure of centrality,
quantifying the importance of each node within the
whole-brain architecture. Inoltre, link-level analyses
characterize the strength of specific links between discrete
brain regions. Although graph theory approaches have
been employed in previous studies of creativity—for
esempio, using regions of interest applied to task-based
fMRI data and DT assessed with human creativity ratings
(Beaty, Benedek, Kaufman, & Silvia, 2015)—the present
work contributes new insight, applying a data-driven WD
and link-level connectivity analysis to explore resting-state
network architecture related to DT assessed objectively via
computational semantic distance. Brain graphs provide a
powerful model to study the complex organization of the
human brain; application of graph theory metrics may
deepen our understanding of the network interactions,
which facilitate creative thinking.

Present Research

New approaches in network neuroscience have begun to
elucidate the cognitive mechanisms that underlie creative
thought; Tuttavia, more work is needed to disentangle the
complex network interactions involved in the production
of novel ideas and how these interactions relate to objec-
tive assessments of creativity. This study applies WD and
link-level analysis to represent individual differences in

500

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Volume 33, Numero 3

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
3
3
4
9
9
1
8
6
2
6
1
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
6
5
8
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

resting-state networks associated with DT ability. To objec-
tively quantify individual’s DT performance, we leverage
latent variable modeling and multiple computational
models of semantic distance, extracting a latent factor of
semantic distance from five semantic models previously
shown to have a strong (but non-unity) correlation with
human judgments of relatedness, novelty, and creativity
(Beaty & Johnson, 2020). Semantic distance has been
employed in a task-based fMRI study using a noun–verb
generation task, finding that the semantic distance
between nouns and verbs tracked increases in frontopolar
cortex activity and connectivity (Verde, Cohen, Raab,
Yedibalian, & Gray, 2015). We do not seek to replicate
these findings in our study, given differences in the tasks,
semantic distance calculations, and fMRI data; Tuttavia,
this work provides preliminary evidence that semantic
distance captures individual variation in task-relevant
neural response. To date, it is unknown how such auto-
mated assessments relate to individual differences in the
brain’s intrinsic functional architecture (cioè., during the
resting state) and whether similar neural correlates corre-
spond to human versus automated creativity assessment.
The present research thus aimed to extend research on
the neural basis of creative thinking by combining WD
analysis of resting-state fMRI data with both human and
automated creativity assessments.

METHODS

The data were collected as part of a larger study on individ-
ual differences in creativity and imagination (Beaty, Kenett,
et al., 2018). The larger project included a task-based fMRI
study of DT; the task fMRI data from this study are not
analyzed here but have been published elsewhere (Frith
et al., 2020; Adnan, Beaty, Silvia, Spreng, & Turner, 2019;
Beaty, Chen, et al., 2018). Here, we analyze resting-state
fMRI data from the full sample of the larger project. Noi
have previously published subsets of the resting-state
fMRI sample in studies of personality (Beaty, Chen, et al.,
2018) and human ratings on the AUT (Kenett, Betzel, &
Beaty, 2020), but no study has examined resting-state data
in relation to computational semantic distance. The total
sample consisted of 186 participants from the University
of North Carolina, Greensboro and members of the com-
munity; art, music, and science majors were oversampled
to broaden the representation of creative backgrounds.
From the 177 participants who completed a resting-state
scan, two were excluded because of distortion of the struc-
tural image. The final sample consisted of 175 participants
(127 women, mean age = 22.67 years, SD = 6.37 years).
All participants were right-handed with normal or
corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of any
neurological disorders, cognitive disabilities, or medica-
tions that affect the central nervous system (Beaty, Chen,
et al., 2018). All participants provided written informed
consent. The study was approved by the University of
North Carolina, Greensboro, institutional review board.

Behavioral Assessment

DT performance was assessed by the AUT, conducted
during a separate task-based fMRI scan, as well as on a com-
puter outside scanner. Note that the task-based fMRI data
are not presented here (only the verbal responses; Vedere
Beaty, Chen, et al., 2018). During the task-based fMRI scan,
participants were presented with a series of everyday ob-
jects (per esempio., brick) and asked to imagine new and unusual
uses for each object. Participants had 12 sec to think of a
single alternate use for a list of 23 objects and then had
5 sec to verbally report their response via an MRI-compatible
microphone (Benedek, Christensen, Fink, & Beaty, 2019;
Beaty, Chen, et al., 2018). For the computer-based assess-
ment, consistent with conventional procedures, partici-
pants had 3 min to generate as many alternative uses for
two objects as possible (box and rope). Inoltre, partic-
ipants completed a test of visuospatial intelligence (Gv) A
measure the ability to mentally manipulate visual stimuli,
assessed via three independent tasks: paper-folding,
block-rotations, and cube comparisons (Frith et al., 2020).
The MRI- and lab-based DT responses were pooled and
scored for creative quality by 1) trained human raters using
the subjective scoring method (Silvia, 2008) E 2) compu-
tational models using semantic distance (Beaty & Johnson,
2020). Regarding human ratings, four trained raters inde-
pendently scored the creative quality of each response
using a 1 (not at all creative) A 5 (very creative) scala
(Benedek, Mühlmann, Jauk, & Neubauer, 2013; Silvia, 2008).
Raters were instructed to provide a single rating for each
risposta, focusing on uncommonness, remoteness, E
cleverness; the scoring rubric and guidance for raters can be
found on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/vie7s/).
Regarding semantic distance, we followed the approach
described in Beaty and Johnson (2020). We thus computed
the semantic distance between each object cue word (per esempio.,
box) and each response using an on-line application called
SemDis, an open platform developed to automate creativ-
ity assessment via semantic distance (semdis.wlu.psu.edu).
SemDis leverages five compositional vector models to
compute the relatedness between inputted texts: three
continuous bag of words (CBOW) predict models and
two count models. CBOW/predict models were built using
a neural network architecture (Mandera, Keuleers, &
Brysbaert, 2017) that employs a sliding window to move
through text corpora and aims to predict a central word
from surrounding context words (cf. word2vec); count
models, in contrast to predict models, compute the co-
occurrence of words within these large text corpora. IL
three CBOW models included: 1) a concatenation of the
ukwac web crawling corpus (∼2 billion words) E
the subtitle corpus (∼385 million words; window size =
12 parole, 300 dimensions, most frequent 150,000 parole);
2) the subtitle corpus only (window size 12 parole, 300
dimensions, most frequent 150,000 parole); E 3) a con-
catenation of the British National Corpus (∼2 billion
parole), ukwac corpus, and the 2009 Wikipedia dump

Orwig et al.

501

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
3
3
4
9
9
1
8
6
2
6
1
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
6
5
8
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

(∼800 million tokens; window size = 11 parole, 400 di-
mensions, most frequent 300,000 parole). CBOW models
have previously demonstrated high correlations with
human relatedness judgments (Mandera et al., 2017) COME
well as human creativity ratings across a range of common
creativity tasks, including the AUT (Beaty & Johnson, 2020).
The two count models include: 1) a latent semantic analysis
modello, Touchstone Applied Science Associates, Quale
computes word co-occurrences within a text corpus
(37,000 documents, middle and high school textbooks
and literary words, 92,393 different words), followed by a
singular value decomposition on the resulting sparse
matrix (300 dimensions; cf. Prabhakaran et al., 2014); E
2) the global vectors (Glove; Pennington, Socher, &
Equipaggio, 2014) modello, which is trained on ∼6 billion
gettoni (300 dimensions, top 400,000 parole) and uses
weighted least squares to extract global information
across a concatenation of the 2014 Wikipedia dump and
the Gigaword corpus (news publications from 2009 A
2010).

All five spaces were used to compute the semantic dis-
tance between the AUT item (per esempio., box) and participants’
responses, where the cosine angle between the word
vectors represents semantic similarity; semantic distance
is then computed by subtracting this similarity from 1
(Kenett & Faust, 2019; Beaty, Christensen, Benedek,
Silvia, & Schacter, 2017; Verde, 2016; Prabhakaran et al.,
2014). Following Beaty and Johnson (2020), we used latent
variable modeling to extract the common variance from the
five semantic models. This approach has the benefit of
reducing the influence of any one model—which has been
shown to yield idiosyncratic values specific to the given
model and text corpus employed (Mandera et al., 2017)
thus boosting the reliability and generalizability of results.
Using Mplus 8, we specified a confirmatory factor analysis
(CFA) that modeled semantic distance, human creativity
ratings, and Gv as three latent variables using the full sample
of participants with available data (n = 186). For semantic
distance, the mean values of the five semantic models
served as indicators for three lower order latent variables,
corresponding to the three AUT tasks (box, rope, E
MRI); a higher order variable was indicated by these three
lower order variables. For human ratings, the mean values
of the four raters served as indicators for three lower order
latent variables (box, rope, and MRI); a higher order vari-
able was indicated by these three lower order variables.
For Gv, the summed scores of the three tasks were mod-
eled as indicators of a Gv factor. The variance of the latent
variables was fixed to one; all indicators were standardized.
For the fMRI analyses below, we extracted factor scores
from the two higher order semantic distance and human
creativity variables.

MRI Acquisition and Preprocessing

Resting-state MRI data were acquired for all participants on
UN 3 T Siemens Magnetom MRI system using a 16-channel

head coil (Figura 1). High-resolution T1 scans were ac-
quired for anatomical normalization. BOLD T2*-weighted
functional images were acquired with gradient EPI se-
quence with the following parameters: repetition time =
2000 msec, echo time = 30 msec, flip angle = 78°, 192-
mm field of view, 32 axial slices, 3.5 × 3.5 × 4.0 mm, inter-
leaved slice ordering, sequence length = 5 min.
Participants were instructed to relax awake in the scanner
with eyes closed for the duration of the scan.

MRI data for both anatomical and functional images were
preprocessed using FMRIB Software Library v5.0.7 and
MATLAB 2017a (The MathWorks Inc.). The anatomical
and functional preprocessing pipelines were adapted from
previous work (Diez et al., 2019). The anatomical T1 pre-
processing included: reorientation to right-posterior-
inferior; alignment to anterior and posterior commissures;
skull stripping; gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospi-
nal fluid segmentation; and computation of nonlinear
transformation between individual skull-stripped T1 and
2-mm resolution Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)
152 template images. The fMRI preprocessing pipeline
included: slice time correction, reorientation to right-
posterior-inferior, realigning functional volumes within
runs with a rigid body transformations (six parameters
linear transformation), computation of the transformation
between individual skull-stripped T1 and mean functional
images, intensity normalization, and removal of con-
founding factors from the data using linear regression—
including 12 motion-related covariates (rigid motion pa-
rameters and its derivatives), linear and quadratic terms,
and five components each from the lateral ventricles and
white matter. Global signal regression was not applied
because of the negative correlations this can introduce.
Transformation of resting-state data to MNI space was
performed, concatenating the transformation from func-
tional to structural and from structural to MNI, spatial
smoothing with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 6-mm
FWHM, and band-pass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz) to reduce
low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise were also ap-
plied. Head motion was quantified using realignment pa-
rameters obtained during image preprocessing, including
three translation and three rotation estimates. Scrubbing of
time points with excess head motion interpolated all time
points with a frame displacement > 0.2 mm was applied.
No participants demonstrated excessive head motion;
così, none was removed from the study based on these
criteria. The distributions of the correlations across time
series were reviewed for possible contamination; no out-
liers were observed from the whole-brain connectivity
distributions.

Weighted-Degree Functional Connectivity Analysis

Pearson correlation coefficients were used to calculate the
connectivity matrices of each participant using the time
series of all cortical gray matter voxels (Figura 1). A r-to-z
Fisher transformation was applied to the resulting

502

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Volume 33, Numero 3

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
3
3
4
9
9
1
8
6
2
6
1
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
6
5
8
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

l

D
o
w
N
o
UN
D
e
D

F
R
o
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
io
R
e
C
T
.

M

io
T
.

e
D
tu

/
j

/

o
C
N
UN
R
T
io
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
3
3
4
9
9
1
8
6
2
6
1
5

/
j

o
C
N
_
UN
_
0
1
6
5
8
P
D

.

F

B

G
tu
e
S
T

T

o
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Figura 1. Methods. A sample of healthy adults underwent resting-state fMRI scans, from which we computed voxel-wise connectivity matrices
between all gray matter voxels. Behavioral assessment enabled the analysis of individual differences in functional connectivity associated with creative
performance.

correlation matrix, and negative values were removed. A
minimize noise, we considered only the most significant
links using a false discovery rate at q-level < 0.005 (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). To evaluate the relative prominence of each voxel in the whole-brain architecture of each individual, voxel-level WD values were computed (Figure 1). After obtaining a high-resolution 52,769 × 52,769 connectivity matrix for each participant, we summed all the weighted connections of each voxel to generate a WD map showing the extent to which each voxel is func- tionally connected to the rest of the brain (Ortiz-Terán et al., 2017). A general linear model was used to compute the association between WD and DT score. All statistical analyses were corrected for participant age, sex, and Gv. Whole-brain correction for multiple comparisons was com- puted using Monte Carlo simulation with 10,000 iterations to estimate the probability of false-positive clusters with a two-tailed p < .05 (3dClustSim; afni.nimh.nih.gov). Link-Level Functional Connectivity Strength Analysis To further investigate relationships between DT and corti- cal regions identified in the WD analysis, we evaluated if DT performance correlated with link-level connectivity strength values across brain areas. To evaluate link-level functional connectivity strength, MRI data were down- sampled to 6-mm isotropic voxels to reduce dimension- ality, resulting in a 6620 × 6620 connectivity matrix for each participant. A general linear model was used at every link of the network to evaluate the association of the link weight and the DT scores (separately for seman- tic distance and human ratings). Whole-brain correction for multiple comparisons was computed adapting the Monte Carlo simulation method to networks. Ten thou- sand random networks were generated with the same smoothing properties, to compute a false-positive cluster size with a two-tailed p < .001. Compared to WD maps, where clusters were defined as contiguous voxels, here, clusters were defined as links that connect contiguous voxel groups. We reduced the dimensionality of the sur- viving links for visualization purposes. The statistically significant links positively/negatively associated with DT were then represented in a connectogram in Neuromarvl (https://immersive.erc.monash.edu/neuromarvl/). Cortical surfaces were visualized using the population-average landmark and surface-based projections of CARET soft- ware. We used Caret v5.65 software to represent the Orwig et al. 503 results in a three-dimensional Population-Average Landmark and Surface-based surface using the “enclosing voxel algorithm” and “fiducial and flat mapping” settings. Surface images were displayed using a color scale based on T-scores. RESULTS Correlation between Semantic Distance and Human Creativity Ratings Before moving to the fMRI analysis, we examined the rela- tionship between semantic distance and human creativity ratings. The CFA showed good fit: χ2 (396 df ) 643.242, p < .001; comparative fit index = .945; root mean square error of approximation = .058 (90% CI [0.05, .066]); stan- dardized root means square residual = .074 (Figure 2). We found a large latent correlation between semantic distance and human creativity ratings, r = .81 ( p < .001), consistent with the large effect reported in Beaty and Johnson (2020) using only the laboratory-based AUT data. This result indi- cates a high degree of overlap between semantic distance and human ratings (see Figure 3). Gv correlated significantly with human creativity ratings (r = .46, p < .001)—as previ- ously reported (see Frith et al., 2020)—but not semantic dis- tance (r = .11, p = .29), indicating that human ratings share more variance with general cognitive ability than with auto- mated assessments of creativity (cf. Beaty & Johnson, 2020). Brain Hubs Associated with DT Our first WD analysis aimed to identify cortical hubs related to individual differences in DT assessed via semantic distance. Results showed that DT semantic distance was negatively correlated with WD of voxels in the occipital cortex, along with parietal-occipital and temporal regions (Figure 3A). WD across lateral occipital and superior parietal regions was negatively correlated with DT. Similarly, lower WD along the temporo-parietal junction and right middle temporal gyrus was associated with higher DT performance. WD of the left inferior parietal sulcus was neg- atively associated with DT. There were no positive associa- tions between WD and DT. In addition, we performed WD analysis with human ratings of DT (Figure 3B). WD maps generated with human ratings of DT shared similar cortical distributions to the semantic distance measure, with 51% of voxels shared between the two maps. Results showed that human ratings of DT were negatively correlated with WD of voxels in the occipital cortex and right temporal regions. WD of the right temporal pole was negatively associated with DT. There were no positive associations between WD and human ratings of DT. Individual Connectivity Patterns and DT Link-level analysis revealed individual links between brain regions negatively associated with semantic distance of l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 3 3 4 9 9 1 8 6 2 6 1 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 6 5 8 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 2. CFA of semantic distance, human creativity ratings, and visuospatial intelligence. Observed variables are depicted as squares, and latent variables are depicted as circles. For interpretability, the observed variables for semantic distance and human creativity ratings are not shown in the diagram. b_semdis = box task, semantic distance; r_semdis = rope task, semantic distance; m_semdis = MRI tasks, semantic distance; b_human = box task, human ratings; r_human = rope task, human ratings; m_human = MRI tasks, human ratings; gv_block = visuospatial intelligence, block rotation; gv_cube = visuospatial intelligence, cube comparison; gv_paper = visuospatial intelligence, paper folding. n = 186. DT, corrected for multiple comparisons (Figure 4A). Strength of connectivity within the primary visual cortex was negatively correlated with DT. In other words, high local connectivity within the visual area was associated with lower DT performance. Projections from bilateral V1 and V2 to the precuneus were negatively associated with DT. Strength of connectivity between primary visual areas and precuneus was negatively associated with DT. In addition, we identified links positively associated with DT semantic distance (Figure 4B). The strength of the con- nection between the precuneus and right inferior temporal 504 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 33, Number 3 Figure 3. Weighted degree. WD across the lateral occipital and superior parietal cortex was negatively associated with DT. WD along the right middle temporal gyrus negatively correlated with DT. Semantic distance and human ratings of DT were consistent in these findings. gyrus was positively correlated with DT. Projections from the precuneus to right inferior occipital cortex (fusiform), right inferior temporal, and occipital cortices were posi- tively correlated with DT semantic distance. Link-level analysis with human ratings of DT revealed a negative association similar to that of semantic distance measures. Connectivity across the occipital cortex and projection to the right primary motor and somatosensory areas were negatively associated with human ratings of DT. We did not find any positive association with human ratings of DT. Despite the similar negative associations between the two measures, semantic distance reveals a positive association between individual connectivity strength and DT, which human ratings do not capture. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 3 3 4 9 9 1 8 6 2 6 1 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 6 5 8 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 4. Link level. Strength of connectivity within the primary visual cortex was negatively correlated with DT. Projections from the precuneus to the right inferior occipital and temporal cortices were positively correlated with DT. Orwig et al. 505 DISCUSSION This study applies graph theory techniques in resting-state functional connectivity MRI data to explore how individual differences in assembly of resting-state networks are related to creativity, using a new application to automat- ically assess creativity via semantic distance. Critically, WD results from the semantic distance measure were largely consistent with human ratings—producing similar functional connectivity maps—further validating this ob- jective assessment as a viable measure of creative perfor- mance. The findings thus contribute to the growing neuroscience literature on individual differences in crea- tive thinking and demonstrate the utility of automated scoring approaches to capture variance in creativity at the neural level. Lateral Visual Cortex Centralizes the Emergence of DT Among the most salient findings, our analysis revealed a negative association between DT and WD across lateral occipital, inferior parietal, and right middle temporal areas. Secondary link-level analyses revealed that func- tional connectivity between the left precuneus and right lateral visual pathway was positively associated with DT, whereas strength of connections within primary visual re- gions were negatively associated with DT. These results indicate that there is less integration of visual information during resting state in individuals with higher DT scores. High local connectivity within the primary visual system represents a high degree of segregation. One interpreta- tion of these results could be that creative people are more prone to engage in internally directed cognition (such as mind wandering) in the absence of an external task. Schooler et al. (2011) describe the phenomenon of perceptual decoupling, the capacity to disengage atten- tion from perception, during episodes of mind wander- ing. Perceptual decoupling during resting-state may indicate engagement in more abstract, internally directed attention, which characterizes creative individuals. Internally directed attention has been associated with ex- tended deactivation in occipital and inferior parietal cor- tices (Benedek et al., 2016). In addition, Sepulcre (2014) identifies the lateral occipital cortex and the inferior pari- etal lobule as integrators of sensory information, toward more high-level cognition. One possibility is that partici- pants with reduced functional connectivity to the primary visual system are engaged in more creative, self-generated cognitive processes during resting state. Link-level analyses showed that strength of connec- tions within the primary visual cortex was negatively as- sociated with DT, whereas connectivity from the right lateral and ventral occipital cortex to the precuneus was positively associated with DT. At first glance, these results may appear to be in conflict, as we see projections from the precuneus both positively and negatively associated with DT. This differential connectivity reflects the atten- uation of primary visual areas, with higher connectivity between lateral inferior occipital regions and multimodal integration areas being positively associated with DT. The precuneus is a core hub of the DMN and is thought to play a role in multimodal integration. In a study examin- ing gray matter density and verbal creativity, it was found that increased gray matter density in precuneus was pos- itively associated with DT (Benedek, 2018). Our results suggest that regions of the precuneus may be differentially involved in creative problem solving. Stronger connec- tivity between visual regions and precuneus may reflect tighter coupling between perception and generation sys- tems, whereas decreased connectivity between the visual cortex and semantic regions may reflect weaker commu- nication between perception and object representation along the ventral stream. Differential pathways for the in- tegration of visual information may elucidate the neural mechanisms involved in DT, although further research is needed to support this claim. Previous studies of task-based and resting-state data have described consistent patterns of network interaction between DMN and executive control networks in relation to creative cognition (Christensen, Benedek, Silvia, & Beaty, 2019; Beaty, Kenett, et al., 2018; Beaty et al., 2015). DMN activity is thought to contribute to the generation of candidate ideas, whereas executive control networks exert top–down monitoring to meet external constraints (Beaty et al., 2016). Notably, our results do not reflect this interac- tion between default mode and executive control networks, suggesting that the novel semantic distance measures and graph theory analyses reported here capture variance in cre- ative thinking that is distinct from these network dynamics. The finding of a negative association between WD in the right middle temporal gyrus and DT raises questions regarding the involvement of this region in verbal creativ- ity. Task-based studies of DT have applied dynamic causal modeling to describe unidirectional control from the pFC over the middle temporal gyrus ( Vartanian et al., 2018). Other studies have implicated the middle temporal gyrus in idea generation (Ellamil, Dobson, Beeman, & Christoff, 2012) and semantic integration ( Jung-Beeman, 2005). It has been suggested the right hemisphere is preferentially involved in the processing of distantly related “coarse” se- mantic information (Beeman et al., 1994). The negative association between DT performance and WD of right middle temporal areas demonstrate that, at rest, these re- gions are less functionally connected to the rest of the brain in participants who scored higher on the AUT. Link-level connectivity strength between inferior tempo- ral areas and the precuneus was positively associated with DT, which may reflect more coupling of semantic processing and multimodal-integration areas. In summary, our findings extend past research by identifying neural correlates of individual creative thinking using objective and automated assessments of creativity based on seman- tic distance. 506 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 33, Number 3 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 3 3 4 9 9 1 8 6 2 6 1 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 6 5 8 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Limitations and Future Directions An important consideration in cognitive and neuroscience studies of creativity concerns the operationalization of cre- ativity. Many studies focus on the creative quality of ideas (assessed via human creativity ratings or semantic distance) as the primary dependent outcome, but this outcome alone can only provide a modest window into the cognitive basis of idea generation. Viewed as a high-level cognitive ability, creative thought (or the output from creativity tasks and their assessed quality) likely results from the complex interplay of multiple “lower level” cognitive processes, such as memory retrieval, cognitive control, and attention. We encourage future research to further identify the cognitive processes that give rise to creative ideas. The observed overlap between WD maps generated from human ratings and semantic distance measures showed 51% of voxels common between the two maps. Currently, there is no clear benchmark for determining the magnitude or significance between these maps. Although the cortical distributions appear visually similar (see Figure 3), we cannot explain what is reflected by the nonoverlapping regions. Speculatively, the distinction may be because of features of ideas that humans consider when rating creative responses that are not captured by compu- tational semantic distance (e.g., utility or cleverness of an idea). These unique neural features should be more deeply explored in future work (cf. Vartanian et al., 2020). The present research uses resting-state fMRI data to examine functional networks of the brain at rest. Given the nature of resting-state data, there is a considerable amount noise. In collecting the data, we are not able to control for the par- ticipant’s mood or state of mind during the resting-state scan and there is known to be high variability between scans of the same individual. Although this measure is sub- ject to noise, the size of our sample (n = 175) allows us to be confident that these findings are not simply the result of noise and do provide new insight into individual differ- ences in the creative brain. Future studies may explore the causal relationship be- tween identified regions and DT performance. For exam- ple, transcranial magnetic stimulation to the right middle temporal gyrus before completion of the AUT may pro- vide further clarification into the role of this brain region in the integration of semantically distant concepts (Luft, Zioga, Thompson, Banissy, & Bhattacharya, 2018). In ad- dition, future work in the field may explore cortical gene expression related with patterns of functional connectiv- ity to describe how specific genetic pathways may be re- lated to individual variability in creativity. Conclusions This study applied graph theory techniques to resting-state fMRI data to measure individual differences in functional connectivity associated with DT. Computational semantic models were applied to calculate the semantic distance between objects and responses to obtain an automated measure of DT performance. This automated measure of DT provided results consistent with (but not identical to) those of human ratings, highlighting the potential of semantic distance as a reliable metric of creativity. The find- ing of a negative association between resting-state connec- tivity in the occipital cortex and DT may be explained by perceptual decoupling, reflecting a higher degree of inter- nally directed cognition in participants who scored higher on the AUT. In addition, our findings suggest a positive association between DT and connectivity from the pre- cuneus to right inferior temporal regions. More work is needed to validate these findings and further characterize the complexities of the creative brain. Acknowledgments The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this paper. Reprint requests should be sent to William Orwig, 149 13th St, Suite 5.209, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA 02129, or via e-mail: worwig@mgh.harvard.edu. Author Contributions William Orwig: Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Visualization; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing. Ibai Diez: Formal analysis; Methodology. Patrizia Vannini: Writing – review & editing. Roger Beaty: Data curation; Supervision; Writing – review & editing. Jorge Sepulcre: Supervision; Writing – review & editing. Funding Information This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01AG061811 to J. S.; R01AG061083 to P. V. and J. S.). R. E. B. is supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (DRL-1920653). This research was supported by grant RFP-15-12 to R. E. B., from the Imagination Institute (www.imagination-institute.org), funded by the John Templeton Foundation. REFERENCES Adnan, A., Beaty, R., Silvia, P., Spreng, R. N., & Turner, G. R. (2019). Creative aging: Functional brain networks associated with divergent thinking in older and younger adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 75, 150–158. DOI: https://doi.org /10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.11.004, PMID: 30572185 Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316, 29–52. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12360, PMID: 24502540, PMCID: PMC4039623 Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Kaufman, S. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5, 10964. DOI: https:// Orwig et al. 507 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 3 3 4 9 9 1 8 6 2 6 1 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 6 5 8 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 doi.org/10.1038/srep10964, PMID: 26084037, PMCID: PMC4472024 Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 87–95. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.tics.2015.10.004, PMID: 26553223, PMCID: PMC4724474 Beaty, R. E., Chen, Q., Christensen, A. P., Qiu, J., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). Brain networks of the imaginative mind: Dynamic functional connectivity of default and cognitive control networks relates to openness to experience. Human Brain Mapping, 39, 811–821. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002 /hbm.23884, PMID: 29136310, PMCID: PMC5764809 Beaty, R. E., Christensen, A. P., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2017). Creative constraints: Brain activity and network dynamics underlying semantic interference during idea production. Neuroimage, 148, 189–196. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.012, PMID: 28082106, PMCID: PMC6083214 Beaty, R. E., & Johnson, D. R. (2020). Automating creativity assessment with SemDis: An open platform for computing semantic distance. PsyArXiv Preprints. DOI: https://doi.org /10.31234/osf.io/nwvps Beaty, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Benedek, M., Chen, Q., et al. (2018). Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 115, 1087–1092. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas .1713532115, PMID: 29339474, PMCID: PMC5798342 Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1186–1197. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0428-8, PMID: 24898118 Buckner, R. L., Sepulcre, J., Talukdar, T., Krienen, F. M., Liu, H., Hedden, T., et al. (2009). Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: Mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1860–1873. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI .5062-08.2009, PMID: 19211893, PMCID: PMC2750039 Bullmore, E. T., & Bassett, D. S. (2011). Brain graphs: Graphical models of the human brain connectome. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 113–140. DOI: https://doi.org/10 .1146/annurev-clinpsy-040510-143934, PMID: 21128784 Bullmore, E. T., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 186–198. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2575, PMID: 19190637 Christensen, A. P., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2019). Executive and default network connectivity reflects conceptual interference during creative imagery generation. PsyArXiv Preprints. DOI: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/n438d Diez, I., Ortiz-Terán, L., Williams, B., Jalilianhasanpour, R., Ospina, J. P., Dickerson, B. C., et al. (2019). Corticolimbic fast-tracking: Enhanced multimodal integration in functional neurological disorder. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 90, 929–938. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136 /jnnp-2018-319657, PMID: 30850473, PMCID: PMC6625895 Dumas, D., Organisciak, P., & Doherty, M. (2020). Measuring divergent thinking originality with human raters and text- mining models: A psychometric comparison of methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000319 Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M., & Christoff, K. (2012). Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. Neuroimage, 59, 1783–1794. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.008, PMID: 21854855 Beeman, M., Friedman, R. B., Grafman, J., Perez, E., Diamond, S., Feng, Q., He, L., Yang, W., Zhang, Y., Wu, X., & Qiu, J. (2019). & Lindsay, M. B. (1994). Summation priming and coarse semantic coding in the right hemisphere. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 26–45. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn .1994.6.1.26, PMID: 23962328 Benedek, M. (2018). Internally directed attention in creative cognition. In R. E. Jung & O. Vartanian (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (1st ed., pp. 180–194). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011 Benedek, M., Christensen, A. P., Fink, A., & Beaty, R. E. (2019). Creativity assessment in neuroscience research. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13, 218–226. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000215 Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Beaty, R. E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Brain mechanisms associated with internally directed attention and self-generated thought. Scientific Reports, 6, 22959. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038 /srep22959, PMID: 26960259, PMCID: PMC4785374 Benedek, M., Mühlmann, C., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Assessment of divergent thinking by means of the subjective top-scoring method: Effects of the number of top-ideas and time-on-task on reliability and validity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 341–349. DOI: https://doi.org /10.1037/a0033644, PMID: 24790683, PMCID: PMC4001084 Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, Methodological, 57, 289–300. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111 /j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1196/annals .1440.011, PMID: 18400922 Verbal creativity is correlated with the dynamic reconfiguration of brain networks in the resting state. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 894. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00894, PMID: 31068873, PMCID: PMC6491857 Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2019). The neuroscience of creativity. Neuroforum, 25, 231–240. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515 /nf-2019-0006 Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Hutterer, L., Steiner, E., Benedek, M., Weber, B., et al. (2014). Gray matter density in relation to different facets of verbal creativity. Brain Structure and Function, 219, 1263–1269. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007 /s00429-013-0564-0, PMID: 23636224 Frith, E., Elbich, D. B., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Chen, Q., Kane, M. J., et al. (2020). Intelligence and creativity share a common cognitive and neural basis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. DOI: https://doi.org /10.1037/xge0000958, PMID: 33119355 Gao, Z., Zhang, D., Liang, A., Liang, B., Wang, Z., Cai, Y., et al. (2017). Exploring the associations between intrinsic brain connectivity and creative ability using functional connectivity strength and connectome analysis. Brain Connectivity, 7, 590–601. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2017.0510, PMID: 28950708 Green, A. E. (2016). Creativity, within reason: Semantic distance and dynamic state creativity in relational thinking and reasoning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 28–35. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415618485 Green, A. E., Cohen, M. S., Raab, H. A., Yedibalian, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2015). Frontopolar activity and connectivity support dynamic conscious augmentation of creative state. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 923–934. DOI: https://doi.org /10.1002/hbm.22676, PMID: 25394198, PMCID: PMC6869232 Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. 508 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 33, Number 3 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 3 3 4 9 9 1 8 6 2 6 1 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 6 5 8 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Heinen, D. J. P., & Johnson, D. R. (2018). Semantic distance: An automated measure of creativity that is novel and appropriate. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 12, 144–156. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000125 Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 512–518. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics .2005.09.009, PMID: 16214387 Kenett, Y. N., Betzel, R. F., & Beaty, R. E. (2020). Community structure of the creative brain at rest. Neuroimage, 210, 116578. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020 .116578, PMID: 31982579 Kenett, Y. N., & Faust, M. (2019). A semantic network cartography of the creative mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 271–274. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.007, PMID: 30803872 Li, J., Zhang, D., Liang, A., Liang, B., Wang, Z., Cai, Y., et al. (2017). High transition frequencies of dynamic functional connectivity states in the creative brain. Scientific Reports, 7, 46072. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/srep46072, PMID: 28383052, PMCID: PMC5382673 Liu, Z., Zhang, J., Xie, X., Rolls, E. T., Sun, J., Zhang, K., et al. (2018). Neural and genetic determinants of creativity. Neuroimage, 174, 164–176. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.neuroimage.2018.02.067, PMID: 29518564 Luft, C. D. B., Zioga, I., Thompson, N. M., Banissy, M. J., & Bhattacharya, J. (2018). Right temporal alpha oscillations as a neural mechanism for inhibiting obvious associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 115, E12144–E12152. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas .1811465115, PMID: 30541890, PMCID: PMC6310824 Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2017). Explaining human performance in psycholinguistic tasks with models of semantic similarity based on prediction and counting: A review and empirical validation. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 57–78. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.04.001 Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1037/h0048850, PMID: 14472013 Ortiz-Terán, L., Diez, I., Ortiz, T., Perez, D. L., Aragón, J. I., Costumero, V., et al. (2017). Brain circuit–gene expression relationships and neuroplasticity of multisensory cortices in blind children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 114, 6830–6835. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073 /pnas.1619121114, PMID: 28607055, PMCID: PMC5495230 Pennington, J., Socher, R., & Manning, C. (2014). Glove: Global vectors for word representation. In Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP) (pp. 1532–1543). Doha, Qatar: Association for Computational Linguistics. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.3115/v1/D14-1162 Prabhakaran, R., Green, A. E., & Gray, J. R. (2014). Thin slices of creativity: Using single-word utterances to assess creative cognition. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 641–659. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0401-7, PMID: 24163211, PMCID: PMC4105589 Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 98, 676–682. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073 /pnas.98.2.676, PMID: 11209064, PMCID: PMC14647 Salvi, C., & Bowden, E. M. (2016). Looking for creativity: Where do we look when we look for new ideas? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 161. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016 .00161, PMID: 26913018, PMCID: PMC4753696 Salvi, C., Bricolo, E., Franconeri, S. L., Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2015). Sudden insight is associated with shutting out visual inputs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1814–1819. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0845-0, PMID: 26268431 Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 319–326. DOI: https://doi.org /10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006, PMID: 21684189 Sepulcre, J. (2014). Functional streams and cortical integration in the human brain. Neuroscientist, 20, 499–508. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858414531657, PMID: 24737695 Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., & Goñi, J. (2014). Hubs and pathways. In M. Mesulam & S. Kastner (Eds.), Brain mapping: An encyclopedic reference (Vol. 2, pp. 441–447). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978 -0-12-397025-1.00023-3 Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Yeo, T. B., Liu, H., & Johnson, K. A. (2012). Stepwise connectivity of the modal cortex reveals the multimodal organization of the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 10649–10661. DOI: https://doi.org/10 .1523/JNEUROSCI.0759-12.2012, PMID: 22855814, PMCID: PMC3483645 Shi, L., Sun, J., Xia, Y., Ren, Z., Chen, Q., Wei, D., et al. (2018). Large-scale brain network connectivity underlying creativity in resting-state and task fMRI: Cooperation between default network and frontal-parietal network. Biological Psychology, 135, 102–111. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018 .03.005, PMID: 29548807 Silvia, P. J. (2008). Discernment and creativity: How well can people identify their most creative ideas? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 139–146. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1037/1931-3896.2.3.139 Sporns, O., Chialvo, D. R., Kaiser, M., & Hilgetag, C. C. (2004). Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 418–425. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.008, PMID: 15350243 Sun, J., Liu, Z., Rolls, E. T., Chen, Q., Yao, Y., Yang, W., et al. (2019). Verbal creativity correlates with the temporal variability of brain networks during the resting state. Cerebral Cortex, 29, 1047–1058. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor /bhy010, PMID: 29415253 Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., et al. (2012). The association between resting functional connectivity and creativity. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 2921–2929. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr371, PMID: 22235031 van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 519–534. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010 .03.008, PMID: 20471808 Vartanian, O., Beatty, E. L., Smith, I., Blackler, K., Lam, Q., & Forbes, S. (2018). One-way traffic: The inferior frontal gyrus controls brain activation in the middle temporal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule during divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 118, 68–78. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.024, PMID: 29477840 Vartanian, O., Smith, I., Lam, T. K., King, K., Lam, Q., & Beatty, E. L. (2020). The relationship between methods of scoring the alternate uses task and the neural correlates of divergent thinking: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage, 223, 117325. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.neuroimage.2020.117325, PMID: 32882380 Wei, D., Yang, J., Li, W., Wang, K., Zhang, Q., & Qiu, J. (2014). Increased resting functional connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex in creativity by means of cognitive stimulation. Cortex, 51, 92–102. DOI: https://doi.org /10.1016/j.cortex.2013.09.004, PMID: 24188648 Orwig et al. 509 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 3 3 4 9 9 1 8 6 2 6 1 5 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 6 5 8 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3Creative Connections: Computational Semantic image
Creative Connections: Computational Semantic image
Creative Connections: Computational Semantic image
Creative Connections: Computational Semantic image
Creative Connections: Computational Semantic image

Scarica il pdf