Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in

Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in
Human Time Processing

Nicola Binetti1, Alessandro Tomassini2, Karl Friston3, and Sven Bestmann3,4

Abstrait

■ Timing emerges from a hierarchy of computations ranging
from early encoding of physical duration (time sensation) à
abstract time representations (time perception) suitable for stor-
age and decisional processes. Cependant, the neural basis of the per-
ceptual experience of time remains elusive. To address this, nous
dissociate brain activity uniquely related to lower-level sensory
and higher-order perceptual timing operations, using event-
related fMRI. Participants compared subsecond (500 msec) sinu-
soidal gratings drifting with constant velocity (standard) against
two probe stimuli: (1) control gratings drifting at constant velocity
ou (2) accelerating gratings, which induced illusory shortening of
temps. We tested two probe intervals: a 500-msec duration (Short)
and a longer duration required for an accelerating probe to be per-
ceived as long as the standard (Long—individually determined).

On each trial, participants classified the probe as shorter or longer
than the standard. This allowed for comparison of trials with an
“Objective” (physical) or “Subjective” (perceived) difference in
duration, based on participant classifications. Objective duration
revealed responses in bilateral early extrastriate areas, extending
to higher visual areas in the fusiform gyrus (at more lenient
thresholds). Par contre, Subjective duration was reflected by
distributed responses in a cortical/subcortical areas. This com-
prised the left superior frontal gyrus and the left cerebellum,
and a wider set of common timing areas including the BG, pariétal
cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex. These results suggest two
functionally independent timing stages: early extraction of dura-
tion information in sensory cortices and Subjective experience
of duration in a higher-order cortical–subcortical timing areas. ■

INTRODUCTION

Duration estimation is a fundamental ability for successful
interactions with our environment. Evaluating and com-
paring durations of sensory events is thought to require
the interplay between parietal, premotor, cingulate, et
prefrontal cortices and subcortical regions in the BG,
cerebellum, and thalamus (Merchant & Yarrow, 2016;
Coull, Nazarian, & Vidal, 2008). Although research has
shed light on various processes that enable our sense of
duration, such as the extraction and representation of
duration information, or the mnemonic and decisional
processes required for duration comparison and classifica-
tion, the biological basis of the subjective experience of
time remains scarcely understood (Wittmann & Meissner,
2018; Trojano, Caccavale, De Bellis, & Crisci, 2017; Bueti &
Macaluso, 2011; Wittmann, 2009).

Sensory input conveys durational information, with evi-
dence from neural network modeling (Buonomano,
Bramen, & Khodadadifar, 2009), brain stimulation (Salvioni,
Murray, Kalmbach, & Bueti, 2013), and neuroimaging studies

1UCL Interaction Centre, University College London, 2MRC
Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge,
3Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Queen
Square Institute of Neurology, 4Department of Movement and
Clinical Neurosciences, UCL Queen Square Institute of
Neurologie, University College London

(Bueti, Bahrami, Walsh, & Rees, 2010; Bueti & Macaluso,
2010), suggesting that early striate and extrastriate ( V5/
MT) regions play a key role in temporal encoding and
STM, independent of low-level visual feature processing.

The ability to compare durations of stimuli belonging to
different sensory modalities or to reproduce the duration
of a sensory stimulus with an equivalently timed motor
response implies that duration information can be repre-
sented in a more abstract format. Several models of time
perception propose that time is quantified by means of a
linear accumulation of timing evidence (Ivry & Richardson,
2002; Rosenbaum, 2002; Wing, 2002; Treisman, 1963). Ce
seems compatible with the build-up of activity before an
event, which covaries with stimulus duration, as seen in
nonhuman primate ( Janssen & Shadlen, 2005; Leon &
Shadlen, 2003) and human electrophysiology recordings
(Pfeuty, Ragot, & Pouthas, 2005; Macar & Vidal, 2004;
Pouthas, Garnero, Ferrandez, & Renault, 2000).

En effet, imaging studies reveal multiple regions sensi-
tive to time accumulation when contrasting timing tasks
with equivalent nontiming controls. Par exemple, differ-
ences in visual stimulus duration lead to larger activity in
the anterior portion of the SMA (preSMA) and the ACC
(Pouthas et al., 2005). Bueti and Macaluso (2011) assessed
the relationship between the subjective experience of
time and corresponding neural responses in a more direct
approach for identifying the biological substrates of time

© 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Published under a
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC PAR 4.0) Licence.

Journal des neurosciences cognitives 32:7, pp. 1369–1380
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01557

D
o
w
n
je
o
un
d
e
d

je

je

/

/

/

/
j

F
/

t
t

je
t
.

:
/
/

F
r
o
m
D
o
h
w
t
t
n
p
o
:
un
/
d
/
e
m
d
je
F
r
t
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
je
je
d
v
je
r
e
e
r
c
t
c
.
m
h
un
je
e
r
d
.
toi
c
o
o
m
c
n
/
j
un
o
r
t
c
je
c
n
e
/

un
p
r
d
t
je
3
c
2
je
7
e
1

3
p
6
d
9
F
2
/
0
3
1
2
3
/
5
7
9
/
2
1
3
o
6
c
9
n
_
/
un
1
_
8
0
6
1
1
5
6
5
7
7
9
p
/
d
j
o
b
c
oui
n
g
_
toi
un
e
_
s
0
t
1
o
5
n
5
0
7
7
.
p
S
d
e
F
p
e
b
m
oui
b
e
g
r
toi
2
e
0
s
2
t
3

/
j

/

t

F

.

o
n

0
5

M.
un
oui

2
0
2
1

accumulation (Bueti & Macaluso, 2011). The authors
observed that activity in the right putamen, right mid insula,
and right mid/superior temporal cortex correlated with an
illusory lengthening of time, suggesting a distributed corti-
cal and subcortical time accumulation network. These
findings are consistent with recent models proposing that
timekeeping is distributed and carried out in parallel. Pour
example, the striatal beat frequency model suggests that
striatal spiny neurons act as temporal integrators of corti-
cal and thalamic oscillatory clock signals (Buhusi & Meck,
2005; Matell, Meck, & Nicolelis, 2003) across both duration
estimation and duration reproduction tasks and indepen-
dently of sensory modality.

Duration discrimination further relies on working memory
processes to store and compare duration of nonsimulta-
neous stimuli. Lateral premotor and right inferior frontal
cortices have been linked to the mnemonic components
of comparison/reproduction timing tasks (Pouthas et al.,
2005; Schubotz & von Cramon, 2001; Kawashima et al.,
1999). Although typically observed in timing research,
SMA is more generically linked to working memory, typically
recruited in perceptual and motor tasks (Rao, Mayer, &
Harrington, 2001; Kawashima et al., 1999; Rao et al., 1997).
Early auditory (Franssen, Vandierendonck, & Van Hiel,
2006) and visual (Salvioni et al., 2013) processing areas
are involved in maintaining STM traces of temporal infor-
mation in working memory. Enfin, the comparison of
these memory traces must lead to a categorical decision
of which stimulus lasted longer (Lindbergh & Kieffaber,
2013). Studies have linked the cingulate cortex to these
decision-making stages in duration discrimination tasks
(Pouthas et al., 2005; Rao et al., 2001).

Cependant, there is still a limited understanding of what
subset of timing areas uniquely contribute to our subjec-
tive experience of time (Hayashi, van der Zwaag, Bueti, &
Kanai, 2018; Trojano et al., 2017; Wittmann, Van Wassenhove,
Craig, & Paulus, 2010; Wittmann & Paulus, 2008). Ici, nous
address at which stage—within a timing hierarchy—our
perceptual experience of duration (c'est à dire., a duration “percept”)
emerges. To this end, we contrasted activity related to the
low-level encoding of duration features embedded in a sen-
sory signal (sensitive to differences in time—duration sen-
sation) from activity related to the subjective, higher-order
perception of duration (sensitive to perceived differences
in time—duration perception), which inform categorical
duration judgments.

Participants evaluated the duration of sequential drifting
sinusoidal gratings in a classic standard/probe comparison
task. Participants compared subsecond (500 msec) sinu-
soidal gratings drifting with constant velocity (standard)
against two probe stimuli: (1) control gratings drifting at
constant velocity or (2) accelerating gratings, which in-
duced an illusory shortening of time (Bruno, Ayhan, &
Johnston, 2015; Sasaki, Yamamoto, & Miura, 2013; Binetti,
Lecce, & Doricchi, 2012; Matthieu, 2011). The inclusion of
accelerating stimuli dissociated objective and subjective
duration effects. On each trial, participants classified the

probe as lasting shorter or longer than the standard.
This allowed for identifying brain activity uniquely related
to an objective difference in probe duration (Objectively
Long vs. Objectively Short) or to a subjective difference
in probe duration (Subjectively Short vs. Subjectively Long),
based on participants’ duration classifications. Although
there is no dedicated sensor for time, duration is a feature
that characterizes a physical stimulus. We hypothesized
that visual areas, which are sensitive to features embedded
within a sensory signal, should primarily respond to varia-
tions in objective duration (Salvioni et al., 2013; Bueti
et coll., 2010), c'est, reflecting duration sensation. This ex-
tracted information would then inform a conscious per-
cept of time, c'est, duration perception. Objective and
perceived duration, cependant, do not always agree, as evi-
denced by time distortions that highlight the constructive
nature of timing (Eagleman, 2008). We reasoned that the
subjective experience of duration would engage higher-
order areas and in some measure should be less sensitive
to objective differences in duration.

We found two independent (c'est à dire., sans chevauchement) sets,
one sensitive to objective duration and comprising bilat-
eral early extrastriate areas and one sensitive to subjective
duration, including the left superior frontal gyrus and the
left cerebellum, and to a lesser degree the BG, pariétal
cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex. Direct comparisons
of Subjective and Objective activations confirmed a pat-
tern of activation that was consistent with that observed
in the Subjective contrast and not with the Objective con-
trast, thus further supporting the notion of independent
sets of brain activations. These data suggest two function-
ally independent, hierarchically segregated systems for
sensory and perceptual representations of duration.

MÉTHODES

Participants

Twenty-eight participants were recruited in the study
(19 femmes, age = 27.7 ± 6.3 années, range = 20–48 years).
All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
Informed consent was obtained from all participants
before starting the experiment. Five participants were
excluded from the analysis of functional data because of
poor psychometric fits (with left tail >.25 and right tail
<.75 probability) for Accelerating probe data collected during the structural scan. The functional data of the re- maining 23 participants is reported below (15 women, age = 28.5 ± 6.5 years, range = 20–48 years). The study was approved by the University College London ethics com- mittee and was in agreement with research guidelines and regulations. Experimental protocols conformed to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Apparatus Visual stimuli were back-projected with an LCD projector displaying at 1024 × 760 (60 Hz) onto a translucent screen at 1370 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 32, Number 7 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j f / t t i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j / f . t o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 end of the MRI bore. Participants viewed the screen through a mirror mounted on a 32-channel head coil. Participants wore silicone earplugs and overear headphones through which they received instructions. Stimulus presentation and response collection were implemented on MATLAB 2011a (The MathWorks), running the Cogent toolbox library (www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent.php). Task and Stimuli Participants performed duration comparisons in a visual Standard–Probe binary choice task. Participants were tested on two variants of the task, the first during a 12-min struc- tural scan (Phase 1) and the second during two 22-min functional scan runs (Phase 2). Identifying the Accelerating Drift Timing Bias for Perceptually Consistent Timing Categories (Phase 1) Participants sequentially viewed Standard and Probe dura- tions (Figure 1A), delivered through horizontally drifting sinusoidal carriers set within a stationary Gaussian enve- lope (drifting Gabor patch encompassing approximately 5° of visual angle, with a 0.6 cpd spatial frequency). The central portion of the patch was occupied by a Gaussian blob, which encompassed 0.5° of visual angle, on top of which a fixation cross was overlaid. The Standard stimulus drifted rightward with Constant velocity (0.42 rad dis- placement per draw cycle) for 500 msec. The Probe stim- uli were characterized by Constant or Accelerating drift and variable duration (for animation lasting nFrames equiv- alent to 500 msec or required to match point of subjective equality [PSE], drift was approximated by a fourth-degree polynomial function, y = 976.87 × x4 – 49620 × x3 + 9.1337e+05 × x2 − 6.2571e+06 × x + 1.0829e+07, for x was between 1 and 30 in nFrames steps and y was succes- sively scaled to guarantee that the Probe’s average drift velocity was equal to the Standard drift velocity). Horizontal shifts progressively increased (accelerated) across draw cycles, with an average velocity that matched that of the Constant drift Standard. Previous studies reveal that accel- erating stimuli appear to last shorter than stimuli of equal duration that move at constant velocity (Bruno et al., 2015; Sasaki et al., 2013; Binetti et al., 2012; Matthews, 2011). Probe duration was selected based on randomly perturbed estimates of the PSE, yielded by a QUEST adaptive staircase routine across 200 trials ( Watson & Pelli, 1983). Stimuli were separated by a 500-msec ISI, where only the central fixation cross was displayed. At the end of the trial, partici- pants were required to indicate with a right hand button press which stimulus, the first or second, lasted longer. Standard and Probe stimulus order was counterbalanced across trials. The purpose of this task was that of identifying the PSE of the Accelerating drift stimuli for each participant, D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j t t f / i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j t . f / o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 Figure 1. (A) Time course of events in trial during functional scans. The Standard grating drifted rightward with Constant velocity for 500 msec, whereas the Probe grating had Constant or Accelerating drift, lasting 500 msec or a duration that matched the participant’s PSE for accelerating stimuli. Stimuli were separated by a 500-msec ISI. At the end of the trial, participants indicated which stimulus lasted longer (first or second). Standard and Probe order was counterbalanced across trials. (B) During the structural scan (Phase 1), we identified the participant’s PSE for Accelerating stimuli, that is, duration required for an Accelerating probe to appear equally long to the Constant standard. During the functional scans (Phase 2), participants were presented Constant (C) and Accelerating (A) stimuli lasting 500 msec (S) or longer durations equivalent to the participant’s PSE (L), yielding four stimulus categories: CS, CL, AS and AL. (C) Mean and variability (standard deviation) of the PSE required for Accelerating stimuli to be perceived equally long to the 500-msec Constant drift Standard, as calculated during the structural scan (Phase 1). Error bars depict the SEM. (D) Proportion of “Probe lasted longer” responses for the four stimulus categories presented during the functional scans (Phase 2). Binetti et al. 1371 that is, the duration required for the Accelerating stimulus to be perceived equally long to the Constant drift Standard. This was aimed at generating duration categories that were perceptually consistent across participants. We predicted, based on previous observations (Binetti et al., 2012; Matthews, 2011), that the Accelerating drift would pro- duce a PSE greater than the 500-msec Standard duration, indicating a compression of perceived duration, that is, the Probe would have to last longer than the Standard in order for the two to appear equally long. yielded an estimate of the participant’s PSE for Accelerating drift, that is, the duration required for an Accelerating Probe to be perceived equally long to the Constant drift Standard. A PSE larger than the 500-msec Standard would indicate that accelerating drift induces a compression of time as the Probe has to last longer than the Standard in order for the two to appear equally long. The PSE values for Accelerating drift, calculated for each participant, determined the dura- tion of Long stimuli presented in Phase 2. Behavioral data were analyzed with JASP 0.8.1.1 (https://jasp-stats.org/). Dissecting Activity Sensitive to Objective and Subjective Differences in Duration (Phase 2) fMRI Data Acquisition After completing the structural scan, participants performed a similar task during two functional scan runs. The task varied with respect to the previous one in two respects: (1) Participants now estimated both Accelerating and Constant drift probes, presented in alternating blocks of six trials, and (2) only two Probe durations were tested (Figure 1A, B): a 500-msec duration (Short—equivalent to the standard duration) and the duration required for an Accelerating probe to appear equally long to the standard (>500 msec, Long—the PSE determined on an individual
basis in Phase 1). This yielded four stimulus combinations:
Constant-Short (CS), Accelerating-Short (AS), Constant-
Long (CL), and Accelerating-Long (AL). To maintain atten-
tional set, incidental Constant and Accelerating stimuli at
two flanker durations were included (Short flankers and
Long flankers—the duration required for an Accelerating
probe to generate 10% et 90% of “Probe longer”
réponses, respectivement; not shown in Figure 1). Ce
ensured the participants were engaged by occasionally ex-
posing them to easy trials (in which Standard and Probe
were noticeably different). Probe duration (Short, Long, et
flankers) was randomly selected across trials. Participants
were presented 140 Short probes (70 Constant drift +
70 Accelerating drift), 140 Long probes (70 Constant drift +
70 Accelerating drift), et 56 flankers (28 Short Constant
drift flankers + 28 Long Accelerating drift flankers), for a
total of 336 trials per run. Every 48 trials, participants had
a 12-sec resting period where only the central fixation cross
was displayed (six resting periods per run). Phase 2 était
aimed at identifying brain activity uniquely related to an
Objective difference in probe duration (Long vs. Short
probes) or to a Subjective difference in probe duration,
based on duration classifications (Subjectively Short vs.
Subjectively Long), revealing responses linked to duration
sensation and perception, respectivement.

Behavioral Data Analysis

Randomly perturbed QUEST estimates were binned (seven
linearly spaced time bins ranging between 100 et 1500 msec),
and the proportion of “Probe longer” responses per time bin
was calculated. We fit each participant’s responses with a cumu-
lative Gaussian (Figure 1B). Le 50% point of this function

Whole-brain 3-D gradient-echo EPI (Lutti, Thomas, Hutton,
& Weiskopf, 2013) data were acquired on a Siemens Trio 3T
scanner equipped with a 32-channel head coil for signal
reception. Functional data were acquired over two sessions
(2249 volumes total per participant). Acquisition parameters
were as follows: 3 mm isotropic resolution, echo time =
15.85 et 34.39 msec (multiecho acquisition), repetition
time = 1.1 sec per volume. Each volume was acquired with
an acceleration of 3 in the through-slab direction and a
speed-up factor of 2 in the in-plane direction, et le
images were reconstructed using the GRAPPA algorithm
(Griswold et al., 2002) as implemented on the scanner
console. The first five volumes were discarded to allow
for steady-state magnetization. A flip angle of 15° was used.
Field maps were acquired before the functional runs for
subsequent correction of geometric distortions in EPI data
at high field strength using a multiecho gradient-echo
with 3-mm isotropic resolution and echo times of 10 msec/
12.46 msec (short/long, respectivement). Whole-brain T1-weighted
anatomical images of each participant’s brain were acquired
using an optimized 3-D modified driven equilibrium Fourier
transform imaging sequence (Deichmann, Schwarzbauer, &
Tourneur, 2004) with 1-mm isotropic resolution. The sequence
used an inversion time of 910 msec, echo time = 2.48 msec,
repetition time = 7.92 msec, and flip angle = 16°.

fMRI Data Analysis

Data were analyzed using SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for
Neuroimaging, University College London). Preprocessing
included realignment and unwarping using individual field
maps, coregistration of EPI to individual anatomical images,
spatial normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute
(MNI) espace, and spatial smoothing using an 8-mm FWHM
Gaussian kernel.

We disambiguate Objective and Subjective duration by
contrasting stimuli based on their physical duration (Long—
L vs. Short—S) or based on the classification of duration
as indicated through participant responses (Probe judged
shorter—0 vs. Probe judged longer—1). The general linear
model included 13 event-related regressors modeling
responses to each event in our 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design.
The first eight involved combinations of stimulus type

1372

Journal des neurosciences cognitives

Volume 32, Nombre 7

D
o
w
n
je
o
un
d
e
d

je

je

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/

je
t
.

:
/
/

F
r
o
m
D
o
h
w
t
t
n
p
o
:
un
/
d
/
e
m
d
je
F
r
t
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
je
je
d
v
je
r
e
e
r
c
t
c
.
m
h
un
je
e
r
d
.
toi
c
o
o
m
c
n
/
j
un
o
r
t
c
je
c
n
e
/

un
p
r
d
t
je
3
c
2
je
7
e
1

3
p
6
d
9
F
2
/
0
3
1
2
3
/
5
7
9
/
2
1
3
o
6
c
9
n
_
/
un
1
_
8
0
6
1
1
5
6
5
7
7
9
p
/
d
j
o
b
c
oui
n
g
_
toi
un
e
_
s
0
t
1
o
5
n
5
0
7
7
.
p
S
d
e
F
p
e
b
m
oui
b
e
g
r
toi
2
e
0
s
2
t
3

/
j

t

.

/

F

o
n

0
5

M.
un
oui

2
0
2
1

(Accelerating—A/Constant drift—C), stimulus duration
(Short—S/Long—L), and perceived duration (Perceived
Short—0/Long—1), modeled relative to the onset of the
Probe stimulus. En plus, the onset of short (Regressor 9)
and long (Regressor 10) flanker stimuli, the onset of
(Regressor 11) eye blinks, (Regressor 12) pauses, et le
onset participants’ button presses (Regressor 13) were in-
cluded. All event-related responses were modeled as stick
les fonctions; apart from pauses that were modeled as contin-
uous 12-sec events. In addition to event regressors, a total
de 23 nuisance regressors were included to account for mo-
tion and physiological effects of no interest. A physiological
noise model was constructed using an in-house MATLAB
toolbox (Hutton et al., 2011) to remove variance accounted
for by cardiac and respiratory responses. The model com-
prised 17 physiological regressors in total: 6 for respiratory
phase, 1 for respiratory volume, et 10 for cardiac phase.
Enfin, to discount motion-related artifacts that eluded rigid
body motion correction, the six motion regressors obtained
during realignment were included.

In this paper, we focus on the simple main effects of
Objective and Subjective duration (Short vs. Long) par
testing two contrasts. Objective duration was explored by
contrasting (CL1 + AL1 + CL0 + AL0)–(CS1 + AS1 + CS0 +
AS0). Subjective duration was explored by contrasting stimuli
based on the participant duration classifications, indepen-
dently of their objective duration: Subjective contrast (CS1
+ AS1 + CL1 + AL1)–(CS0 + AS0 + CL0 + AL0). Pour
whole-brain analyses, statistical parametric maps of each
contrast were characterized in terms of significant clusters,
using a cluster-defining threshold of p = .001; le 0.05
family-wise error (FWE)-corrected critical cluster size was
100 voxels. We also report additional findings descriptively
at p = .001, uncorrected for subsequent confirmation.
We also directly contrasted Subjective and Objective dura-
tion to identify activations that significantly differed between
these conditions.

Experimental data and codes used in the study are avail-
able upon direct request, consistent with University
College London’s guidelines and regulations.

RÉSULTATS

Behavioral Data

We computed the PSE for Accelerating drift stimuli. Comme
anticipated, we found that Accelerating drift induced a
substantial compression of perceived time: on average,
Accelerating drift stimuli had to last 806 ± 324 msec to
be perceived equally long to the 500-msec Constant drift
Standard (Figure 1C).

We submitted proportion of “Probe longer” responses to
a repeated-measures ANOVA with Stimulus Type as within-
subject factor (AS vs. CS vs. AL vs. CL; Figure 1D) and par-
ticipant Age (binned, above and below mean) and Sex as
covariables. The analysis revealed a main effect of Stimulus
Type, F(3, 57) = 51.99, p < .001, ηp 2 = .71; we observed no modulatory effect of Age, F(3, 57) = .2, p = .89, ηp 2 = .003, or Sex, F(3, 57) = 1, p = .4, ηp 2 = .01, on Stimulus Type. t-Test comparisons showed that, relative to Constant drift stimuli, Accelerating drift stimuli yielded significantly smaller proportions of “Probe longer” responses, both for Short (AS vs. CS: t(22) = −4.6, p < .0001, d = −.96) and Long (AL vs. CL: t(22) = −4.78, p < .00001, d = −.99) durations. We also tested whether proportions of “Probe longer” responses for AS, CS, AL, and CL stimuli differed from chance-level performance (.5 proportion of “Probe longer responses”). Bonferroni corrected t tests revealed that, despite lasting the same as the Standard, the proportion of “Probe longer” responses for AS stimuli significantly differed from chance level, t(22) = −8.61, p < .00001, d = −1.79, due to an illusory shortening of time induced by acceleration. Unsurprisingly, proportion of “Probe longer” responses for CL stimuli also significantly differed from chance level, t(22) = 14.83, p < .00001, d = 3.09, as these did not accelerate and physically lasted longer than the Standard. Proportion of “Probe longer” responses for AL stimuli showed a borderline significant difference from chance level, t(22) = 2.73, p = .049, d = .57. Because Long durations were selected based on each participant’s PSE for accelerating stimuli observed in Phase 1, AL stimuli were perceived on average similar in duration to the Standard despite their physical difference in duration. Finally, proportion of “Probe longer” responses for CS stimuli, which shared duration and drift profile with the Standard, did not significantly differ from chance level, t(22) = −1.74, p = .38, d = −.36. Imaging Analyses Our general linear model design enabled contrasting stimuli based on differences in Objective duration (Objectively Long = L vs. Objectively Short = S) or based on the classification of duration provided by participant responses on a trial-by-trial basis, Subjective duration (Subjectively Long = 1 vs. Subjectively Short = 0). This design accounts for low-level feature processing, atten- tional, decisional, and motoric task demands, which are equally shared across conditions. We performed two con- trasts to identify brain activity uniquely related to Objective and Subjective differences in duration (Figure 2A). Objective Duration Contrast Reveals Activity Bilateral Early Visual Extrastriate Areas Objective duration was explored by contrasting longer versus shorter stimuli, independently of participant re- sponses (Figure 2B, top). Activated areas were confined to visual regions including right inferior occipital gyrus ( p = .002 FWE cluster-corrected; peak coordinate [36 −88 6], t(1, 22) = 4.96) and the left occipital pole ( p = .003 FWE cluster-corrected; peak coordinate [−22 −96 14], t(1, 22) = 4.82). For exploratory purposes, we also evaluated the Binetti et al. 1373 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j f / t t i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j f . t / o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j t t f / i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j f t . / o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 Figure 2. (A) Objective and Subjective duration contrasts, based on the subjective (columns) or objective stimulus duration (rows). Percentages depict the distribution of Short (S) and Long (L) Control (C) and Accelerating (A) stimuli across Objective/Subjective duration categories. (B) Axial slices depicting activations for the contrast identifying objective duration difference, collapsed across stimuli categories (top). Lower panels show the activity changes within each stimulus category. Activation images thresholded for t scores > 2.8 (all stimuli and Accelerating stimuli) and t scores >
2.3 (Constant stimuli). *p < .001, uncorrected. Objective duration contrast at a more lenient p < .001 thresh- old (uncorrected), which showed additional activity in the left precentral gyrus (peak coordinate [−66 −4 16], t(1, 22) = 4.53) and the right fusiform gyrus (peak coordinate [24 −36 −20], t(1, 22) = 4). We further explored Objective duration for Accelerating and Constant stimuli separately (Figure 2B, bottom). Accelerating drift stimuli ((AL1 + AL0) − (AS1 + AS0)) revealed a cluster of activation in the right inferior occipital gyrus ( p = .005 FWE cluster-corrected; p = .046 voxelwise FWE correction; peak coordinate [−36 −86 2], t(1, 22) = 6.18) and in the left occipital pole ( p < .001 FWE cluster-corrected; peak coordinate [−24 −96 8], t(1, 22) = 5.67). For Constant drift stimuli ((CL1 + CL0) − (CS1+CS0)), no regions reached either FWE cluster-corrected significance ( p < .05) or significance at more lenient statistical thresholds ( p < .001, uncorrected). We explored Objective duration for Constant drift stimuli at a more lenient threshold ( p < .01 uncor- rected), which showed an equivalent pattern of activity to the Accelerating drift stimuli, with a cluster of activation in the left and right occipital gyri. Although these patterns of activity were equivalent for Accelerating and Constant drift stimuli, the former were more effective at revealing differences in time sensation. Subjective Duration Contrast Reveals Activity in the Left Superior Frontal Gyrus and the Left Cerebellum Subjective duration was explored by contrasting stimuli based on a participant’s duration classification, indepen- dently of their physical duration (Figure 3, top). We iden- tified a cluster of activation in the medial segment of the left superior frontal gyrus ( p = .002 FWE cluster- corrected; peak coordinate [−2 28 42], t(1, 22) = 5.61) and on the left lateral lobe of the cerebellum ( p = .041 FWE cluster-corrected; peak coordinate [−46 −60 −38], t(1, 22) = 5.17). In addition, at a more lenient statistical threshold (i.e., p < .001 uncorrected), activations includ- ed the right putamen (peak coordinate [26 −6 8], t(1, 22) = 3.89), the left caudate ([−8 8 4], t(1, 22) = 3.81), the right anterior insula ([32 18 −8], t(1, 22) = 3.93), the left parietal cortex (supramarginal gyrus [−46 −50 56], t(1, 22) = 4.3) and the left occipital fusiform gyrus ([−16 −98 −14], t(1, 22) = 3.81). We explored Subjective duration for each stimulus drift type separately (Constant and Accelerating; Figure 3, bottom). Accelerating drift stimuli ((AS1 + AL1) − (AS0 + AL0)) showed a cluster of activation in the medial segment of the left superior frontal gyrus ( p < .001 FWE cluster-corrected; peak coordinate [−4 26 42], t(1, 22) = 5.69) and marginally significant activations ( p < .001 uncorrected) in the left and right lateral lobes of the cerebellum (left: peak coordinate [−44 −62 −44], t(1, 22) = 5.81; right: peak coordinate [26 −80 −46], t(1, 22) = 5.41), left superior frontal gyrus (peak coordinate [−14 18 62], t(1, 22) = 4.65), left middle frontal gyrus (peak coordinate [−40 56 −4], t(1, 22) = 4.27), right supra- marginal gyrus (peak coordinate [60 −40 38], t(1, 22) = 3.53), right angular gyrus (peak coordinate [54 −52 40], t (1, 22) = 4.19), and left caudate (peak coordinate [−8 8 4], t (1, 22) = 3.8), all areas generally associated with tasks that require integration of somatosensory signals and timing tasks (Jones & Jahanshahi, 2011; Buhusi & Meck, 2005; Nenadic et al., 2003; Rao et al., 2001; Ivry & Keele, 1989). More specifically, the BG and pFC are believed to be involved in magnitude representations used in percep- tual timing tasks (Buhusi & Meck, 2005). Constant drift 1374 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 32, Number 7 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j t t f / i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j f / . t o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 Figure 3. Subjective duration contrast activations, collapsed across stimuli categories (top) or within each stimulus category separately (bottom). Activation images thresholded for t scores > 2.8. *p < .001, uncorrected, for display purposes. stimuli ((CS1 + CL1) − (CS0 + CL0)) revealed only mar- ginally significant activations on the left and right lateral lobe of the cerebellum ( p < .001 uncorrected, left: peak coordinate [−22 −66 −24], t(1, 22) = 5.51; right: peak coordinate [36 −60 −24,] t(1, 22) = 3.86) and the right putamen ( p < .001 uncorrected, peak coordinate [30 −2 10], t(1, 22) = 3.93). Subjective–Objective Duration Direct Comparison Confirms Differences in Frontal–Medial Areas Objective and Subjective activations were directly contrasted to formally assess differences between networks uniquely processing timing sensation and perception, identified by the simple main Objective and Subjective contrasts. Paired- sample t tests revealed a cluster of activity in frontal–medial areas, peaking in dorsal ACC BA 32 ( p < .001 FWE cluster- corrected; peak coordinate [−12 32 24], t(1, 22) = 5.22). This cluster included left superior frontal gyrus, which was the strongest activation observed in the Subjective contrast ( p < .001 FWE cluster-corrected; peak coordinate [−4 26 40], t(1, 22) = 5.22). At a more lenient statistical threshold (i.e., p < .001 uncorrected), we observed a pattern of acti- vations that were consistent with what observed in the Subjective duration contrast, notably including the insula (left: peak coordinate [−32 −20 −8], t(1, 22) = 5.08; right: peak coordinate [32 18 −10], t(1, 22) = 3.89) and the left lobe of the cerebellum (peak coordinate [−26 −86 −42], t(1, 22) = 5.08; Figure 4). DISCUSSION We identified brain activity uniquely sensitive to Objective differences in duration (time sensation) and Subjective differences in duration (time perception) while accounting for low-level feature processing, attentional, decisional, and Binetti et al. 1375 Figure 4. Rendered, semi-transparent view depicting brain regions sensitive to Objective (yellow) and Subjective (blue) duration (t scores > 2.8 and Objective
cluster size > 12 voxels or
Subjective cluster size > 35
voxels).

D
o
w
n
je
o
un
d
e
d

je

je

/

/

/

/
j

t
t

F
/

je
t
.

:
/
/

F
r
o
m
D
o
h
w
t
t
n
p
o
:
un
/
d
/
e
m
d
je
F
r
t
o
p
m
r
c
h
.
s
p
je
je
d
v
je
r
e
e
r
c
t
c
.
m
h
un
je
e
r
d
.
toi
c
o
o
m
c
n
/
j
un
o
r
t
c
je
c
n
e
/

un
p
r
d
t
je
3
c
2
je
7
e
1

3
p
6
d
9
F
2
/
0
3
1
2
3
/
5
7
9
/
2
1
3
o
6
c
9
n
_
/
un
1
_
8
0
6
1
1
5
6
5
7
7
9
p
/
d
j
o
b
c
oui
n
g
_
toi
un
e
_
s
0
t
1
o
5
n
5
0
7
7
.
p
S
d
e
F
p
e
b
m
oui
b
e
g
r
toi
2
e
0
s
2
t
3

/
j

F

t

.

/

o
n

0
5

M.
un
oui

2
0
2
1

motoric task demands, which were balanced across condi-
tion. We found that Objective duration was reflected in
activity increases in early visual areas (left occipital pole and
right inferior occipital gyrus), whereas Subjective duration
was reflected in activity increases across a cortical/subcortical
network comprising the left superior frontal gyrus and the
left lateral cerebellum (Tableau 1). A more lenient threshold
( p < .001, uncorrected) revealed additional activity in- creases in the BG (right putamen, left caudate), the right supramarginal gyrus, and right angular gyrus, all areas typi- cally associated with perceptual timing tasks. Interestingly, there was no overlap between these networks, suggesting a clear dichotomy between activity linked to Objective and Subjective duration (Figure 4). Timing Is Achieved throughout a Distributed Cortical and Subcortical Hierarchy Timing relies on mechanisms that guarantee a quasi-linear correspondence between objective and perceived dura- tion, at least within the millisecond-to-second range. As with all perceptual systems, timing is constructive, where sensory and nonsensory (e.g., contextual, prior experience) information are integrated across processing stages. Indeed, neuroimaging, brain lesion, and neurophysiological evi- dence reveal a highly modular and distributed architecture (Merchant, Harrington, & Meck, 2013). Furthermore, recent studies reveal a chronotopic organization of time (Harvey, Dumoulin, Fracasso, & Paul, 2020; Protopapa et al., 2019). Adaptation studies show duration selectivity in neural popu- lations of the inferior parietal lobule, the posterior temporal cortex, the middle frontal gyrus, the middle cingulate cortex, the caudate, putamen, the inferior temporal gyri (Hayashi et al., 2015), and the SMA (Protopapa et al., 2019). Given the repeated presentation of two durations in our study (500 and ∼800 msec—average PSE), a subset of activations (parietal, cingulate cortex, putamen, and caudate) may additionally reflect adaptation-like effects uncovered in these previous studies. Primate electrophysiology (Shuler & Bear, 2006; Ghose & Maunsell, 2002) and human neuroimaging (Bueti et al., 2010; Bueti & Macaluso, 2010) studies have shown that time information modulates activity in primary visual areas, suggesting an involvement of sensory cortices in memory and encoding stages of time perception (Salvioni et al., 2013). Studies contrasting timing against carefully matched nontiming tasks have identified the cerebellum and BG/striatum as candidate time accumulation structures, whereas frontal and parietal areas are more likely involved with attentional, working memory and decisional aspects of the task (Ferrandez et al., 2003; Nenadic et al., 2003; Belin et al., 2002; Rao et al., 2001). Physiological and imaging studies have narrowed down brain activity that is modu- lated by time and therefore more specifically linked to timekeeping. Direct recordings in macaque parietal cortex show a build-up of response frequency as a function of duration ( Janssen & Shadlen, 2005). Similarly, amplitude of evoked brain responses originating in frontal brain areas (contingent negative variation) covary with elapsed dura- tion (Herbst, Chaumon, Penney, & Busch, 2015; Pfeuty et al., 2005). These findings suggest that areas involved with the accumulation of timing evidence should be sensitive 1376 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 32, Number 7 Table 1. FWE Cluster-corrected Peak Coordinates of Activation for Objective and Subjective Duration Contrasts and Objective/Subjective Duration for Accelerating Stimuli Contrast Objective Objective Subjective Subjective Objective (Accelerating stimuli) Objective (accelerating stimuli) Subjective (accelerating stimuli) Subjective (accelerating stimuli) Location in Stereotaxic Space: MNI Cluster Size pFWE t Value (Peak) Label (Neuromorphometrics) 38 −88 6 −22 −96 14 −2 28 42 −46 −60 −38 36 −86 2 712 622 608 294 512 .002 4.96 4.82 5.61 5.17 .003 .002 .041 .005 Right IOG (inferior occipital gyrus) Left OP (occipital pole) Left MSFG (superior frontal gyrus medial segment) Left cerebellum exterior 6.18 Also pfwe = 0.046 voxelwise Right IOG (inferior occipital gyrus) −24 −96 8 886 p < .001 5.67 Left OP (occipital pole) −44 −62 −44 367 .055 5.81 Left cerebellum exterior −4 26 42 1285 p < .001 5.696 Left MSFG (superior frontal gyrus medial segment) to differences in duration, which is a feature expected from hypothetical time accumulation mechanisms. Objective or Subjective Differences in Duration as Handles of Time Accumulation Mechanisms Based on this evidence, Pouthas and coworkers (2005) inves- tigated what areas within a timing network were specifically modulated by duration, aimed at identifying physiological correlates of time accumulator/s (Pouthas et al., 2005). A timing versus nontiming task contrast identified a timing network comprising the frontal, mesiofrontal (preSMA), parietal cortices, and BG. This was followed by contrasting Long versus Short stimuli, revealing activity sensitive to difference in duration in the preSMA, the ACC, the right inferior frontal gyrus (corresponding to Broca’s area), the lateral premotor cortex bilaterally, and the right caudate nucleus. PreSMA and the caudate nucleus were identified as candidate accumulator mechanisms, consistently with neu- ropsychological (Jahanshahi et al., 2010; Malapani, Deweer, & Gibbon, 2002; Harrington, Haaland, & Hermanowitz, 1998) and pharmacological (Coull, Hwang, Leyton, & Dagher, 2012) evidence linking frontostriatal dopaminergic activity to timing. The authors argued that the ACC activa- tion reflected attentional control related to the response selection component of the task, where longer durations require longer sustaining of attention ( Wu et al., 2017; Peru, Pavesi, & Campello, 2004). As with all constructive processes, our perception of dura- tion is not necessarily veridical. This is clearly evidenced by time distortions induced by endogenous factors such as fluc- tuations of arousal (Binetti, Harrison, Mareschal, & Johnston, 2017) or dopamine levels (Marinho et al., 2018) or through manipulations of stimulus features, such as stimulus number, size, or luminance (Xuan, Zhang, He, & Chen, 2007) or motion dynamics (Binetti et al., 2012; Matthews, 2011; Kanai, Paffen, Hogendoorn, & Verstraten, 2006). Based on this, perceived duration, opposed to objective duration of a stimulus, should provide a more robust handle on timekeeping. Bueti and Macaluso (2011) focused on the relationship between the perceptual experience of time and corresponding neural activity by inducing illusory time dilation via visual motion (faster moving stimuli = time overestimation) of stimuli in a duration reproduction task (Bueti & Macaluso, 2011). Contrasting the timing task versus a nontiming control revealed activity in the puta- men, the mid-insula, the SMA, the mid/superior temporal gyri, the right VI lobule of the cerebellum, and the TPJ. Importantly, the authors identified activity that covaried with perceptual duration estimates (reproduction errors) in the right putamen, the right mid-insula, and the superior temporal cortex, revealing accumulation behavior across various brain structures. However, a limitation of studies relying on timing versus nontiming comparisons is that, despite being carefully matched in difficulty and sensory features, they can introduce discrepancies in attentional, strategic, or decisional demands (Pouthas et al., 2005). Disentangling Activity Uniquely Sensitive to Objective and Subjective Differences in Duration In this study, we combined approaches comparing Ob- jective and Subjective differences in duration, within a single protocol strictly focused on comparisons within Binetti et al. 1377 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j f / t t i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j f t . / o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 timing conditions. Opposed to prior studies, this ap- proach did not rely on a nontemporal control task. Both Objective and Subjective contrasts accounted for processing of low-level visual features of stimuli, attentional, decisional, and motoric task demands, which were equally shared across conditions. Objective duration was associated with activity increases in early visual areas (left occipital pole and right inferior occipital gyrus), corresponding to bilateral early extrastriate area, as previously reported in animal (Onoe et al., 2001) and human imaging studies (Bueti et al., 2010). Importantly, early sensory areas have been associated with the extraction of time features from the sensory signal (Salvioni et al., 2013), and Objective differences in duration did not modu- late activity in areas beyond the visual system that are involved with cognitive (i.e., nonsensory) components of timing. For exploratory purposes, we investigated brain activity at a more lenient threshold ( p < .001, uncorrected). We found activations in the left precentral gyrus, which has been frequently documented in time estimation (Ortuño, Guillén-Grima, López-García, Gómez, & Pla, 2011) and re- production tasks ( Jech, Dušek, Wackermann, & Vymazal, 2005), as well as the right fusiform gyrus, which has been linked to attention and working memory in visual duration discrimination tasks (Pouthas et al., 2000). The fusiform gyrus therefore was the latest visual processing stage whose activity was modulated by objective duration information. We observed no activity in V5, which was expected, given that stimuli of different durations were equated in average velocity. Subjective duration was reflected in activity increases in the medial segment of the left superior frontal gyrus, on the border between the dorsal anterior cingulate and the pFC, and the left cerebellum. The dorsal anterior cingu- late is involved in reward-based decision-making (Bush et al., 2002) and in focusing attention toward task-relevant features ( Weissman, Gopalakrishnan, Hazlett, & Woldorff, 2004), whereas the pFC has been frequently linked to timing performance in neuropsychological (Koch, Oliveri, Carlesimo, & Caltagirone, 2002) and brain stimulation studies (Koch, Oliveri, Torriero, & Caltagirone, 2003) and has been associated with attention to time information (Coull, Vidal, Nazarian, & Macar, 2004). Notably, both the superior frontal gyrus (Murai, Whitaker, & Yotsumoto, 2016; Jones & Jahanshahi, 2011) and the cerebellum (Murai et al., 2016; Lewis & Miall, 2003; Ivry & Keele, 1989) are com- monly identified in standard/probe comparisons (Wiener, Turkeltaub, & Coslett, 2010). At more lenient thresholds ( p < .001, uncorrected), we observed activations in the pa- rietal cortex, a hub for abstract representations of quanti- ties (Bueti & Walsh, 2009), posterior cingulate cortex, the angular gyrus, and the BG (right putamen and left cau- date), all areas associated with perceptual timing tasks (Nenadic et al., 2003; Onoe et al., 2001; Rao et al., 2001; Ivry & Keele, 1989). Notably, we also observed activations in the anterior insula, which has been previously observed in imaging (Bueti & Macaluso, 2011; Wittmann et al., 2010) and brain lesion (Trojano et al., 2017) timing studies. The insula is believed to mediate the subjective awareness of duration through the integration of interoceptive signals (Wittmann & Meissner, 2018; Craig, 2009a, 2009b). Also, a recent meta-analysis on conscious visual awareness revealed a distributed network involving the superior frontal gyrus, the caudate, insula, and the fusiform gyrus (Bisenius, Trapp, Neumann, & Schroeter, 2015), all areas observed in the subjective duration contrast. Importantly, Objective and Subjective contrasts yielded independent, nonoverlap- ping sets of activations, confirmed by direct comparisons of Subjective and Objective activations. This suggests functional independence of sensory and perceptual timing stages, which has also been reported in the frequency domain in an MEG study (Noguchi & Kakigi, 2006). Conclusion We identify within a timing network a subset of brain areas that are uniquely related to Objective differences in dura- tion and Subjective differences in duration, thus disentan- gling time sensation and perception. We argue that early visual extrastriate regions extract duration features from the sensory signal (time sensation), without directly medi- ating a subjective duration experience, whereas superior frontal gyrus and the cerebellum relate to higher-order timekeeping, attentional and decisional processes sub- tending the subjective experience of duration and ensuing behavior (time perception). Importantly, time sensation and perception arise from functionally independent pro- cessing stages in the cortical hierarchy. Acknowledgments N. B. was supported through the Newton International Fellowship (Fellowship Number FN111112) awarded by the British Academy and the Royal Society. S. B. was supported by the European Research Council Starter Grant (ActSelectContext; 260424). The Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome trust (203147/Z/16/Z). K. F. was funded by a Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellowship (Ref: 088130/Z/09/Z). We thank Peter Zeidman for his excellent suggestions with the data analysis and interpretation of findings. Reprint requests should be sent to Nicola Binetti, Gower St., Bloomsbury, WC1E 6BT, University College London, United Kingdom, or via e-mail: nicolabinetti@gmail.com. REFERENCES Belin, P., McAdams, S., Thivard, L., Smith, B., Savel, S., Zilbovicius, M., et al. (2002). The neuroanatomical substrate of sound duration discrimination. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1956–1964. Binetti, N., Harrison, C., Mareschal, I., & Johnston, A. (2017). Pupil response hazard rates predict perceived gaze durations. Scientific Reports, 7, 3969. Binetti, N., Lecce, F., & Doricchi, F. (2012). Time-dilation and time-contraction in an anisochronous and anisometric visual scenery. Journal of Vision, 12. 1378 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 32, Number 7 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j t t f / i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j f / t . o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 Bisenius, S., Trapp, S., Neumann, J., & Schroeter, M. L. (2015). Identifying neural correlates of visual consciousness with ALE meta-analyses. Neuroimage, 122, 177–187. Bruno, A., Ayhan, I., & Johnston, A. (2015). Changes in apparent duration follow shifts in perceptual timing. Journal of Vision, 15, 2. Bueti, D., Bahrami, B., Walsh, V., & Rees, G. (2010). Encoding of temporal probabilities in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4343–4352. Bueti, D., & Macaluso, E. (2010). Auditory temporal expectations modulate activity in visual cortex. Neuroimage, 51, 1168–1183. Bueti, D., & Macaluso, E. (2011). Physiological correlates of subjective time: Evidence for the temporal accumulator hypothesis. Neuroimage, 57, 1251–1263. Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1831–1840. Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 755–765. Buonomano, D. V., Bramen, J., & Khodadadifar, M. (2009). Influence of the interstimulus interval on temporal processing and learning: Testing the state-dependent network model. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1865–1873. Bush, G., Vogt, B. A., Holmes, J., Dale, A. M., Greve, D., Jenike, M. A., et al. (2002). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A role in reward-based decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 99, 523–528. Coull, J. T., Hwang, H. J., Leyton, M., & Dagher, A. (2012). Dopamine precursor depletion impairs timing in healthy volunteers by attenuating activity in putamen and supplementary motor area. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 16704–16715. Coull, J. T., Nazarian, B., & Vidal, F. (2008). Timing, storage, and comparison of stimulus duration engage discrete anatomical components of a perceptual timing network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2185–2197. Coull, J. T., Vidal, F., Nazarian, B., & Macar, F. (2004). Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science, 303, 1506–1508. Craig, A. D. (2009a). Emotional moments across time: A possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1933–1942. Craig, A. D. (2009b). How do you feel—Now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 59–70. Deichmann, R., Schwarzbauer, C., & Turner, R. (2004). Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: Technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T. Neuroimage, 21, 757–767. Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 131–136. Ferrandez, A. M., Hugueville, L., Lehéricy, S., Poline, J. B., Marsault, C., & Pouthas, V. (2003). Basal ganglia and supplementary motor area subtend duration perception: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 19, 1532–1544. Franssen, V., Vandierendonck, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2006). Duration estimation and the phonological loop: Articulatory suppression and irrelevant sounds. Psychological Research, 70, 304–316. Ghose, G. M., & Maunsell, J. H. (2002). Attentional modulation in visual cortex depends on task timing. Nature, 419, 616–620. Griswold, M. A., Jakob, P. M., Heidemann, R. M., Nittka, M., Jellus, V., Wang, J., et al. (2002). Generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisitions (GRAPPA). Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 47, 1202–1210. Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., & Hermanowitz, N. (1998). Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology, 12, 3–12. Harvey, B. M., Dumoulin, S. O., Fracasso, A., & Paul, J. M. (2020). A network of topographic maps in human association cortex hierarchically transforms visual timing-selective responses. Current Biology, 30, 1424–1434. Hayashi, M. J., Ditye, T., Harada, T., Hashiguchi, M., Sadato, N., Carlson, S., et al. (2015). Time adaptation shows duration selectivity in the human parietal cortex. PLoS Biology, 13, e1002262. Hayashi, M. J., van der Zwaag, W., Bueti, D., & Kanai, R. (2018). Representations of time in human frontoparietal cortex. Communications Biology, 1, 233. Herbst, S. K., Chaumon, M., Penney, T. B., & Busch, N. A. (2015). Flicker-induced time dilation does not modulate EEG correlates of temporal encoding. Brain Topography, 28, 559–569. Hutton, C., Josephs, O., Stadler, J., Featherstone, E., Reid, A., Speck, O., et al. (2011). The impact of physiological noise correction on fMRI at 7 T. Neuroimage, 57, 101–112. Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 136–152. Ivry, R. B., & Richardson, T. C. (2002). Temporal control and coordination: The multiple timer model. Brain and Cognition, 48, 117–132. Jahanshahi, M., Jones, C. R., Zijlmans, J., Katzenschlager, R., Lee, L., Quinn, N., et al. (2010). Dopaminergic modulation of striato-frontal connectivity during motor timing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 133, 727–745. Janssen, P., & Shadlen, M. N. (2005). A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 234–241. Jech, R., Dušek, P., Wackermann, J., & Vymazal, J. (2005). Cumulative blood oxygenation-level-dependent signal changes support the ‘time accumulator’ hypothesis. NeuroReport, 16, 1467–1471. Jones, C. R., & Jahanshahi, M. (2011). Dopamine modulates striato-frontal functioning during temporal processing. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 70. Kanai, R., Paffen, C. L., Hogendoorn, H., & Verstraten, F. A. (2006). Time dilation in dynamic visual display. Journal of Vision, 6, 1421–1430. Kawashima, R., Inoue, K., Sugiura, M., Okada, K., Ogawa, A., & Fukuda, H. (1999). A positron emission tomography study of self-paced finger movements at different frequencies. Neuroscience, 92, 107–112. Koch, G., Oliveri, M., Carlesimo, G. A., & Caltagirone, C. (2002). Selective deficit of time perception in a patient with right prefrontal cortex lesion. Neurology, 59, 1658–1658. Koch, G., Oliveri, M., Torriero, S., & Caltagirone, C. (2003). Underestimation of time perception after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology, 60, 1844–1846. Leon, M. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2003). Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron, 38, 317–327. Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003). Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 250–255. Lindbergh, C. A., & Kieffaber, P. D. (2013). The neural correlates of temporal judgments in the duration bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 51, 191–196. Lutti, A., Thomas, D. L., Hutton, C., & Weiskopf, N. (2013). High-resolution functional MRI at 3 T: 3D/2D echo-planar Binetti et al. 1379 D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j f / t t i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j . / f t o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 imaging with optimized physiological noise correction. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 69, 1657–1664. Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2004). Event-related potentials as indices of time processing: A review. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 89–104. Malapani, C., Deweer, B., & Gibbon, J. (2002). Separating storage from retrieval dysfunction of temporal memory in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 311–322. Marinho, V., Oliveira, T., Rocha, K., Ribeiro, J., Magalhães, F., Bento, T., et al. (2018). The dopaminergic system dynamic in the time perception: A review of the evidence. International Journal of Neuroscience, 128, 262–282. Matell, M. S., Meck, W. H., & Nicolelis, M. A. (2003). Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 760–773. Matthews, W. J. (2011). How do changes in speed affect the perception of duration? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1617–1627. Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L., & Meck, W. H. (2013). Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 36, 313–336. Merchant, H., & Yarrow, K. (2016). How the motor system both encodes and influences our sense of time. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 8, 22–27. Murai, Y., Whitaker, D., & Yotsumoto, Y. (2016). The centralized and distributed nature of adaptation-induced misjudgments of time. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 8, 117–123. Nenadic, I., Gaser, C., Volz, H. P., Rammsayer, T., Häger, F., & Sauer, H. (2003). Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: New evidence from fMRI. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 238–246. Noguchi, Y., & Kakigi, R. (2006). Time representations can be made from nontemporal information in the brain: An MEG study. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1797–1808. Onoe, H., Komori, M., Onoe, K., Takechi, H., Tsukada, H., & Watanabe, Y. (2001). Cortical networks recruited for time perception: A monkey positron emission tomography (PET) study. Neuroimage, 13, 37–45. Ortuño, F., Guillén-Grima, F., López-García, P., Gómez, J., & Pla, J. (2011). Functional neural networks of time perception: Challenge and opportunity for schizophrenia research. Schizophrenia Research, 125, 129–135. Peru, A., Pavesi, G., & Campello, M. (2004). Impairment of executive functions in a patient with a focal lesion in the anterior cingulate cortex. Evidence from neuropsychological assessment. Functional Neurology, 19, 107–111. Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2005). Relationship between CNV and timing of an upcoming event. Neuroscience Letters, 382, 106–111. Pouthas, V., Garnero, L., Ferrandez, A. M., & Renault, B. (2000). ERPs and PET analysis of time perception: Spatial and temporal brain mapping during visual discrimination tasks. Human Brain Mapping, 10, 49–60. Pouthas, V., George, N., Poline, J. B., Pfeuty, M., VandeMoorteele, P. F., Hugueville, L., et al. (2005). Neural network involved in time perception: An fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 433–441. Protopapa, F., Hayashi, M. J., Kulashekhar, S., van der Zwaag, W., Battistella, G., Murray, M. M., et al. (2019). Chronotopic maps in human supplementary motor area. PLoS Biology, 17, e3000026. Rao, S. M., Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., Bobholz, J. A., Cox, R. W., & Binder, J. R. (1997). Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 5528–5535. Rao, S. M., Mayer, A. R., & Harrington, D. L. (2001). The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 317–323. Rosenbaum, D. A. (2002). Time, space, and short-term memory. Brain and Cognition, 48, 52–65. Salvioni, P., Murray, M. M., Kalmbach, L., & Bueti, D. (2013). How the visual brain encodes and keeps track of time. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 12423–12429. Sasaki, K., Yamamoto, K., & Miura, K. (2013). The difference in speed sequence influences perceived duration. Perception, 42, 198–207. Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Interval and ordinal properties of sequences are associated with distinct premotor areas. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 210–222. Shuler, M. G., & Bear, M. F. (2006). Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science, 311, 1606–1609. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs, 77, 1–31. Trojano, L., Caccavale, M., De Bellis, F., & Crisci, C. (2017). The brain and the subjective experience of time. A voxel based symptom-lesion mapping study. Behavioural Brain Research, 329, 26–34. Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 113–120. Weissman, D. H., Gopalakrishnan, A., Hazlett, C., & Woldorff, M. (2004). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex resolves conflict from distracting stimuli by boosting attention toward relevant events. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 229–237. Wiener, M., Turkeltaub, P., & Coslett, H. B. (2010). The image of time: A voxel-wise meta-analysis. Neuroimage, 49, 1728–1740. Wing, A. M. (2002). Voluntary timing and brain function: An information processing approach. Brain and Cognition, 48, 7–30. Wittmann, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1955–1967. Wittmann, M., & Meissner, K. (2018). The embodiment of time: How interoception shapes the perception of time. In M. Tsakiris & H. De Preester (Eds.), The interoceptive mind: From homeostasis to awareness (p. 63). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 7–12. Wittmann, M., Van Wassenhove, V., Craig, B., & Paulus, M. P. (2010). The neural substrates of subjective time dilation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 2. Wu, D., Deng, H., Xiao, X., Zuo, Y., Sun, J., & Wang, Z. (2017). Persistent neuronal activity in anterior cingulate cortex correlates with sustained attention in rats regardless of sensory modality. Scientific Reports, 7, 43101. Xuan, B., Zhang, D., He, S., & Chen, X. (2007). Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–5. D o w n l o a d e d l l / / / / j f / t t i t . : / / f r o m D o h w t t n p o : a / d / e m d i f r t o p m r c h . s p i l d v i r e e r c t c . m h a i e r d . u c o o m c n / j a o r t c i c n e / - a p r d t i 3 c 2 l 7 e 1 - 3 p 6 d 9 f 2 / 0 3 1 2 3 / 5 7 9 / 2 1 3 o 6 c 9 n _ / a 1 _ 8 0 6 1 1 5 6 5 7 7 9 p / d j o b c y n g _ u a e _ s 0 t 1 o 5 n 5 0 7 7 . p S d e f p e b m y b e g r u 2 e 0 s 2 t 3 / j f t / . o n 0 5 M a y 2 0 2 1 1380 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 32, Number 7Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in image
Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in image
Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in image
Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in image
Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in image
Uncoupling Sensation and Perception in image

Télécharger le PDF