ARTICLE DE RECHERCHE
Bilingualism Effects on the Cognitive Flexibility
of Autistic Children: Evidence From
Verbal Dual-Task Paradigms
un accès ouvert
journal
Eleni Peristeri1
, Margreet Vogelzang2, and Ianthi Maria Tsimpli2
1Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Larissa, Faculty of Medicine, University of Thessaly, Larissa, Grèce
2Faculty of Modern and Medieval Languages and Linguistics, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, ROYAUME-UNI
Mots clés: autism, bilingualism, cognitive flexibility, dual-task, executive functions, langue
ability
ABSTRAIT
The deficit in cognitive flexibility (c'est à dire., the ability to adapt cognitive behavior to changing
contexts) is one of the most prominent characteristics of autistic individuals. Inflexibility may
manifest in restricted interests and increased susceptibility to the effects of misinformation
either through inefficient inhibition of non-target information or deficient recall of correct
information. Bilingualism has been shown to enhance executive functions in both typically
developing children and autistic children; yet, the effect of bilingualism on cognitive flexibility
in autism remains underexplored. Dans cette étude, we used verbal dual-tasks to compare
cognitive flexibility across 50 monolingual autistic and 50 bilingual autistic children, et
50 monolingual and 50 bilingual typically developing children. The children were also
administered language ability tests and a nonverbal global-local cognitive flexibility task, dans
order to investigate whether performance in the dual-tasks would be modulated by the
children’s language and executive function skills. The bilingual autistic children outperformed
their monolingual autistic peers in the dual-tasks. The strength of the bilingualism effect,
cependant, was modulated by the type of language processing that interfered with the target
information in each dual-task, which suggests that the bilingual autistic children calibrated
their processing resources and efficiently adapted them to the changing demands of the dual-
task only to the extent that the task did not exceed their language abilities. Bilingual autistic
children relied on their executive functions rather than on their language abilities while
performing in the dual-tasks. The overall results show that bilingualism compensates for the
reduced cognitive flexibility in autism.
INTRODUCTION
Prior research shows that autism has an adverse effect on cognition, including cognitive flex-
ibility skills (de Vries & Geurts, 2012; Van Eylen et al., 2011). This impairment has been at-
tributed to executive function (EF) deficits in autism that make performance under conditions
of divided attention cognitively demanding (Garcia-Villamistar & Sala, 2002; Goldberg et al.,
2005; Landa & Goldberg, 2005; see Geurts et al., 2014 for a review). On the other hand, concernant-
cent research (Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, 2019; Iarocci et al., 2017; Peristeri et al., 2020;
Citation: Peristeri, E., Vogelzang, M., &
Tsimpli, je. M.. (2021). Bilingualism
effects on the cognitive flexibility of
autistic children: Evidence from verbal
dual-task paradigms. Neurobiology of
Language, 2(4), 558–585. https://doi.org
/10.1162/nol_a_00055
EST CE QUE JE:
https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00055
Informations complémentaires:
https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00055
Reçu: 18 Février 2021
Accepté: 3 Septembre 2021
Intérêts concurrents: Les auteurs ont
a déclaré qu'aucun intérêt concurrent
exister.
Auteur correspondant:
Eleni Peristeri
eperiste@uth.gr
Éditeur de manipulation:
Steven Dick
droits d'auteur: © 2021
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Publié sous Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International
(CC PAR 4.0) Licence
La presse du MIT
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Cognitive flexibility:
Refers to the ability to adapt
cognitive behavior to changing
contexts.
Autism symptomatology:
Refers to the severity of the symptoms
in social and verbal communication,
and restricted repetitive behavior
domains.
Sharaan et al., 2020) has shown that bilingualism has a positive effect on a number of EFs in
autism, including inhibition, flexible switching, and sustained attention, though other research
found no such advantage (Li et al., 2017). In the current study, we examined the impact of
bilingualism on the performance of autistic children and typically developing (TD) enfants
in three language tasks that measure flexibility, since in all of them, participants were expected
to show flexible behaviors. More specifically, across the three tasks, children had to recall
words and then switch to a new rule (c'est à dire., semantic judgment, syntactic processing, counting
aloud) within the same trials. Besides testing bilingualism effects in cognitive flexibility in autism,
the distinct language demands of the three tasks allowed us to investigate whether bilingualism
effects in flexibility in language vary based on the type of language processing that the children
had to perform when switching to the task’s new rule. En outre, we assessed the extent to
which inter-individual variability in the children’s chronological age, autism symptomatology,
socio-economic status (SES), intelligence (IQ), EF, language ability, and dual-language experi-
ence metrics (for bilingual autistic children and bilingual TD children only) is related to their
cognitive flexibility performance in the three dual-tasks.
Executive Functions in Autism
Deficits in executive functioning, an ability that encompasses inhibition and planning (Ozonoff
et coll., 1991; Pellicano et al., 2006), as well as attention regulation, set shifting, and working
mémoire (Solomon et al., 2017), have been often observed in autistic children and adolescents.
Previous research using experimental paradigms measuring inhibition has routinely demon-
strated that autistic children exhibit more erroneous and slower responses than TD peers in
incongruent (vs. congruent) trials, in which the relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions
do not prompt the same response (Kornblum & Lee, 1995). En fait, response errors and/or in-
creases in response latencies in the face of manipulations of incompatible stimulus activation
have been taken to reflect extra effort needed to inhibit interference from competing stimuli;
during EF tasks, the processing of the irrelevant, distracting stimulus interferes with the target
stimulus and has to be inhibited for a relatively short time interval. It is assumed that this re-
quires higher-order executive processes accompanied by increased cognitive effort (par exemple.,
Posner & Cohen, 1984; Tipper et al., 1994). Interference control impairments have been typ-
ically found in autistic children (Bryson, 1983; Christ et al., 2007; Geurts et al., 2014; Sanderson
& Allen, 2013), lequel, in principle, means that task-unrelated stimuli interfere with task-
relevant information more strongly in autism relative to neurotypical controls. Surtout, tel
difficulties have so far been mainly shown in the non-linguistic, cognitive domain. Par exemple,
autistic children in Peristeri et al.’s (2020) study exhibited higher rates of false hits and slower
response times than age-matched TD children in a 2-back task with digits, in which incoming
digits that did not match the digit in trial n–2 had to be inhibited. Aussi, Adams and Jarrold’s
(2012) study showed that autistic children had poorer performance in the conflict trials of a
flanker task comparable to TD and cognitively disabled children used as the control groups.
Individual differences in emerging EF within the autistic population have provided a means
of understanding heterogeneity in early symptom expression. Poor inhibitory control has been
particularly implicated in the maintenance of inappropriate (c'est à dire., non-adaptive) thoughts and
behaviors distinctive of autism, which has been suggested to underpin core features of restric-
tive and repetitive behaviors, such as insistence on sameness (Tourneur, 1997, 1999). Where
studies have managed to run tests of EF that yield substantial variance, this tends to be asso-
ciated with individual differences in age and IQ, especially for inhibition, interference control,
and set-shifting tasks. Par exemple, in Geurts et al.’s (2014) meta-analysis, chronological age
and IQ were found to moderate autistic individuals’ performance on prepotent response
Neurobiology of Language
559
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
inhibition and interference control, respectivement, while other research (Memari et al., 2013)
has shown that set-shifting skills were rather affected by the children’s gender and intellectual
ability, but not age. High-functioning autistic children in Van Eylen and colleagues’ (2011)
study experienced greater switch costs in set shifting relative to the neurotypical group; yet,
the authors acknowledge that this effect was difficult to measure reliably due to the large het-
erogeneity in the autistic group in terms of both demographic (wide age range: 8 à 18 années
vieux) and clinical characteristics (variance in IQ scores). Heterogeneity in EF in autism has been
verified by Leung and Zakzanis’ (2014) meta-analysis, which has exhibited considerable vari-
ability of effect size in set-shifting performance across autistic populations, further implying that
EF deficits do not constitute a uniform characteristic of the autism spectrum. Besides heteroge-
neous demographic, clinical, and behavioral characteristics, which potentially affect the find-
ings on EF in autism, another challenge to overcome is that the EF measures of interest rarely
stand in isolation from one another, since performance on specific EF components often call on
related sub-processes (Van Eylen et al., 2011). A mixture of overlapping EF sub-processes across
experimental paradigms makes it rather difficult to disentangle heterogeneity in EF in autism.
Cognitive Flexibility in Autism
Cognitive flexibility is a common feature of EF and refers to the ability to adapt cognitive be-
havior in response to changing context demands in order to maximize successful performance
in a particular cognitive task or situation (Deák & Wiseheart, 2015; Ionescu, 2012). Though
various EF skills are often nested in cognitive flexibility, shifting has been treated as being syn-
onymous with this cognitive skill, as both necessitate rapid transitions from one task to another,
and a reconfiguration of processing mode and task sets in response to the changing context
(Vandierendonck et al., 2010). Dual-task paradigms, in which two different tasks have to be per-
formed concurrently or sequentially, have been used to quantify cognitive flexibility (among
other tasks), since these paradigms tap into the individual’s ability to simultaneously consider
multiple conflicting representations in two tasks. The main measure of cognitive flexibility in
dual-task paradigms is the switching cost, which stems from the need to rapidly change from
one rule or task to another when giving a response, et, thus, resolve interference from the pre-
vious task set (Koch et al., 2005). Cependant, the fact that various EF skills are often nested in cog-
nitive flexibility, combined with the high predictability in the switching measures used (de Vries
& Geurts, 2012; Van Eylen et al., 2011), makes it difficult to identify the true effect size of cog-
nitive flexibility deficits in autism. So far, the current literature has not reached a consensus on
what cognitive flexibility exactly means and how to best quantify it.
Cognitive flexibility impairments in autistic individuals have often been associated with
their poor performance in dual-task paradigms. Children with autism in Mostert-Kerckhoffs
et al.’s (2015) study needed considerably more time to inhibit distracting stimuli in the auditory
modality of an attention-shifting task. Fait intéressant, no interference effects emerged in the vi-
sual version of the same shifting task, which was explained in part because autistic children
relied more on visual rather than verbal abilities when completing the dual-task. Aussi, Eigsti
et autres. (2008) found that autism had an adverse effect on performance in a dual-task paradigm,
in which autistic participants along with TD controls were required to tap with their finger
while simultaneously describing an image. Though both groups’ tapping rate was slowed-
down in the dual-task as compared to the baseline single-task condition (c'est à dire., finger tapping),
the autistic group was considerably slower than controls in the dual-task condition only.
En outre, in Remington and Fairnie’s (2017) dual-task study, autistic adults and neurotypi-
cal controls were asked to listen to a story (a conversation about a party), which was unex-
pectedly interrupted by the repetition of a phrase that was irrelevant to the story’s content.
Neurobiology of Language
560
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Though the two experimental groups did not differ in overall auditory comprehension accu-
racy, the percentage of the autistic participants who reported hearing the irrelevant auditory
information was significantly greater than neurotypical controls. The overall evidence implies
that dual-task paradigms can be particularly challenging for autistic individuals and, thus, have
a robust autism-discriminative ability in research involving both children and adults. Surtout,
while research in TD children has demonstrated that verbal abilities predict and mediate per-
formance on tasks that measure cognitive flexibility both cross-sectionally (Low & Simpson,
2012) and longitudinally (Watson et al., 2001), this relationship is less understood in autism.
Though the component neural processes underlying cognitive flexibility deficits in autistic
children remain unclear, atypical activation of the lateral frontoparietal and the insular net-
work has been observed. More specifically, 7- to 14-year-old autistic children in Yerys and
colleagues’ (2015) imagerie par résonance magnétique fonctionnelle (IRMf) study demonstrated
hyper-activation of frontal brain regions during a nonverbal set-shifting paradigm. This is con-
sistent with findings of sensory hyper-responsiveness previously identified in sensory and asso-
ciation cortices (S. UN. Green et al., 2015), suggesting that cortical hyper-excitability is also
present in the frontoparietal circuit critical for cognitive flexibility. According to Yerys et al.
(2015), stronger activation of the frontoparietal network in autistic (vs. TD) children suggests that
it may have been harder for them to decide what to do with the feedback regarding the contin-
uation (ou non) of a behavior, thus requiring additional time to switch their responses.
Surtout, these brain-based markers were not found to be linked to either the reaction times
or the accuracy measure of the set-shifting task. Taylor et al.’s (2012) fMRI study has also inves-
tigated the neural underpinnings of the nonverbal set-shifting abilities of a group of 7- to 14-year-
old autistic children. While age-matched TD children demonstrated typical increases in the
activation of the right insula, a region which has been implicated in inhibitory control
(Ghahremani et al., 2015; Swick et al., 2011), the autistic group exhibited a decrease in this
activation with age. This decrease was mostly evident from 10 years of age for the autistic
enfants. The lack of an interaction between age and right insula activation in Taylor et al.’s
(2012) study demonstrates that this region is not being recruited appropriately by autistic
children during inhibition. The overall results indicate significantly altered brain activation
during set-shifting performance in autistic children when compared to TD children, suggérant
a neurobiological difference underpinning cognitive inflexibility in autism. The current study
focuses on investigating cognitive flexibility in bilingual (vs. monolingual) autistic children
based on behavioral evidence; yet, future comparative research should focus on identifying
potential neurophysiological markers of cognitive flexibility performance in these populations.
The consequences of cognitive flexibility impairments in autism have been mainly studied
in the domain of social interactions and everyday functioning. Deficits in the ability to disen-
gage from one task and adapt to a new one have been found to increase social anxiety, comme
interpersonal relations involve the rapid integration of multiple auditory and visual cues,
and selecting components irrelevant to goal-directed behavior make understanding social
events rather chaotic for autistic individuals (Demetriou et al., 2018; South & Rodgers,
2017). En outre, cognitive flexibility has been associated with autistic traits, since individ-
uals with higher autism-spectrum quotient reflected in their poor social, communication, et
imagination abilities, as well as in high attention to detail and effortful attention-switching
(Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), were found to have poorer performance in attention-shifting tasks
(Albein-Urios et al., 2018; Gökçen et al., 2014; Ridley et al., 2011). Ainsi, it is possible that
autistic individuals with relatively low autistic traits can cope more efficiently with an adaptive
or flexible behavior gap. Besides the link between cognitive flexibility and the level of autistic
traits, there is evidence suggesting that performance in cognitive flexibility is a potential
Neurobiology of Language
561
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
predictor of autism symptomatology in children (Kenworthy et al., 2014). Autism symptom-
atology scores could be related to cognitive flexibility scores, leading to a higher likelihood
of decrease in autism symptomatology for individuals with greater cognitive flexibility. Le
current study aims to investigate the relationship between autism symptomatology and cogni-
tive flexibility in both monolingual and bilingual autistic children.
Bilingualism and Cognitive Flexibility in Autism
Research has shown that bilinguals outperform monolinguals on a range of EF tasks of a non-
verbal nature (par exemple., Bialystok et al., 2006; Paap et al., 2018). All speakers must resolve com-
petition arising within the linguistic system. Such competition requires cognitive control
processes to resolve conflict (Hofweber et al., 2021; Pivneva et al., 2012). Bilingual speakers
face additional demands: They possess a language system optimized to handle two coding
systèmes, and to use one language, they must control or inhibit the non-target one. Le
Adaptive Control Hypothesis (D. W. Vert & Abutalebi, 2013) and the Control Process
Model (D. W. Vert & Li, 2014) have proposed that the use of the two languages within
the same context (the dual-language context) provides strong advantages in EF for bilinguals.
Though not consistently found across studies (see van den Noort et al., 2019 for a review),
bilingualism effects have been mainly found in the cognitive subdomains of inhibitory control,
decision-making, attention, and memory processing (Bialystok et al., 2004), as well as in set-
shifting skills, with the latter being equated with cognitive flexibility (Bialystok & Viswanathan,
2009).
The dynamics of how bilingualism affects EF in autism are still poorly understood. Besides
studies that have indirectly compared monolingual and bilingual autistic children on EF
through parent report measures (Iarocci et al., 2017; Ratto et al., 2020), few studies provide
direct evidence for the way bilingualism modulates autistic children’s EF performance
(Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, 2019; Peristeri et al., 2020; Sharaan et al., 2020). More specifi-
cally, Iarocci et al. (2017) report on the results of an EF parental questionnaire that indexed
school age and adolescent children’s ability to plan, anticipate, and maintain goal-directed
behavior. Though the parents of the bilingual autistic children did not report any detrimental
effect on their children’s cognitive skills, the bilingual autistic children experiencing EF deficits
in the clinical range were significantly fewer relative to their peers having no second language
exposure. Aussi, parents of bilingual autistic children in Ratto et al.’s (2020) study reported
fewer cognitive flexibility and impulsivity deficits for their children as compared to the self-
reports of parents of monolingual autistic children. In the same study, bilingual autistic chil-
drens’ reduced cognitive flexibility problems were strongly associated with their scores on the
restricted/repetitive patterns of behaviors and interests subscale measured through the Social
Responsiveness Scale (Constantino & Gruber, 2012).
To the best of our knowledge, three studies provide direct evidence for the relation between
bilingualism and cognitive flexibility in autistic children. Sharaan et al. (2020) did not find
bilingualism effects in the performance of autistic children in nonverbal tasks tapping into flex-
ible switching and interference control; yet, the bilingual autistic children outperformed their
monolingual peers and even TD peers (both monolinguals and bilinguals) on a sustained at-
tention task. Aussi, bilingual autistic children in Peristeri et al.’s (2020) study outperformed their
monolingual peers (both TD and autistic) on a 2-back task with digits by making significantly
fewer false hits. Surtout, Gonzalez-Barrero and Nadig (2019) were the first to show that bi-
lingualism improves the set-shifting abilities of school-age autistic children relative to a group
of age-matched monolingual autistic children.
Neurobiology of Language
562
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
One should mention, though, that the advantages reported for these studies refer to one
outcome variable of the EF measure used. Par exemple, while Gonzalez-Barrero and Nadig
(2019) found a positive effect of bilingualism on autistic children’s set-shifting skills, the op-
posite effect was found for working memory in daily life. De la même manière, Sharaan et al. (2020) trouvé
that bilingualism positively affected autistic children’s sustained attention, but not their flexible
switching and interference control skills. En tant que tel, it remains unclear whether a bilingual ad-
vantage may generalize to autistic populations and which factors modulate its strength. To the
extent that the few studies that have investigated the effect of bilingualism in cognitive flexi-
bility in autism show conflicting evidence (c'est à dire., bilingualism either boosts cognitive flexibility
or has no detrimental effects), further research is needed to make progress in understanding the
interface between bilingualism and cognitive flexibility in autistic individuals. Another issue to
consider is that previous studies have mainly employed nonverbal/visual tasks. One potential
confound with such nonverbal measures is that they rely on visuospatial components of pro-
cessing which are relatively intact in autism (Campbell et al., 2017; Kunda & Goel, 2011).
Although there are good reasons to employ visuospatial set-shifting tasks as measures of cog-
nitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children, it would be beneficial to explore the interplay be-
tween bilingualism and cognitive flexibility in language when separate tasks that require
independent language processing are performed in a concurrent fashion.
The Current Study
As previously discussed, research indicates cognitive flexibility difficulties for autistic individ-
uals, while bilingualism may either carry advantages to this aspect of cognition (González-
Barrero & Nadig, 2019; Peristeri et al., 2020) or confer no benefits at all (Iarocci et al.,
2017). These results need to be interpreted with caution given that bilingualism effects have
been investigated indirectly through parent report measures (Iarocci et al., 2017), tandis que le
sample sizes were rather small (10 monolingual and 10 bilingual autistic children compared
à 10 monolingual and 10 bilinguals TD peers in Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, 2019). Plus loin
research with larger sample sizes is necessary to verify whether bilingualism can mitigate
flexibility difficulties in autistic children.
The current study aimed to investigate bilingualism effects in autistic children’s performance
in three verbal dual-tasks that tapped into cognitive flexibility skills. In the first task, namely the
listening span paradigm (Chrysochoou, 2006; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980), children listened
to sentences, verified their semantic plausibility (truth/falsity), and then had to recall the individ-
ual words at the end of each sentence set. In the syntactic interference word recall task, enfants
were asked to listen to sets of sentences of varying degrees of structural complexity (c'est à dire., active
transitives, passives, sujet- and object-relatives), select the image that best matched the sen-
tence out of multiple-choice visual arrays, and recall separate words that were presented after
each sentence (Ivanova & Hallowell, 2014; Peristeri & Tsimpli, 2010). In the third task, namely
the proactive interference task, children were presented with four sequential lists of words. Items
in the first three lists were members of the same semantic category (fruit), while the words in the
fourth list were members of a different semantic category (sports). After the presentation of each
list and following a delay period during which the participant was instructed to count forward
from a given digit for 10 s, the child was asked to recall the words of each list. En tant que tel, the first
and the second dual-task tested the extent to which semantic and syntactic processing as sec-
ondary tasks, respectivement, would interfere with word recall, which was the primary task of both
dual paradigms; the third task, namely, the proactive interference task, tested the children’s sus-
ceptibility to interference of previously presented items belonging to the same semantic cate-
gory, while counting aloud was manipulated in the task as an interfering foil.
Neurobiology of Language
563
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
The study’s research questions and hypotheses regarding autistic children and TD children
are the following:
Question 1. What is the effect of bilingualism on autistic and TD children’s performance in the
three dual-task paradigms? Are bilingual autistic and TD children more able to flexibly switch
between the tasks in each dual paradigm as compared to their monolingual autistic and TD
peers? Are bilingualism boosts in flexibility modulated by the different language demands
across the three dual-tasks?
Hypothesis 1. Based on previous research showing that bilingualism improves EF in autism
(Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, 2019; Iarocci et al., 2017; Peristeri et al., 2020; Ratto et al.,
2020; Sharaan et al., 2020), we hypothesized that the bilingual autistic and TD children would
perform better at the dual-tasks as compared to their monolingual peers. This effect was ex-
pected to manifest in autistic and TD bilingual children’s higher scores in word recall, lequel
was the primary task of all three dual-task paradigms.
Regarding the extent to which the groups’ performances in the three dual-tasks would be
modulated by the language demands in each task, we hypothesized that the bilingual children
would be more flexible than their monolingual peers as a function of the language complexity
in each dual-task. More specifically, the syntactic interference word recall task was expected to
be more difficult than the listening span and the proactive interference task because of the syn-
tactic processing of sentences with different levels of complexity that was expected to require a
high working memory load. Syntactic impairments have often been reported for individuals on
the autism spectrum (Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Tager-Flusberg, 2016) in contrast to
semantic abilities, which have been claimed to be relatively intact (Eigsti et al., 2011; Tager-
Flusberg et al., 1990). On the other hand, both the listening span and the proactive interference
task include semantic processing, namely, sentence recall and recall of items belonging to the
same semantic category, respectivement. While semantic interference is involved in both tasks, le
level of interference in the proactive interference task was expected to be higher than the lis-
tening span task, since the target responses (c'est à dire., the words to-be-recalled) in the proactive inter-
ference task are the same with the units that need to be processed (c'est à dire., the lists of words
overlapping in terms of semantic categorical similarity). On the other hand, in the listening span
task, it is sentences that need to be processed and judged, but the target is word recall.
Question 2. To what extent is the potential cognitive flexibility boost in bilingual autistic chil-
dren modulated by individual differences?
Hypothesis 2. Based on previous findings that autistic traits, autism symptomatology, and ste-
reotyped and rigid behaviors affect cognitive flexibility (Albein-Urios et al., 2018; Gökçen
et coll., 2014; Kenworthy et al., 2014; Ridley et al., 2011), we expected autistic children’s per-
formance in the dual-tasks to be modulated by individual variation in age, SES, IQ, autism
symptomatology (for the autistic children only), EF, language ability, and dual-language expe-
rience (for bilingual autistic and TD children only). We expected these features to affect chil-
dren’s performance in the dual-tasks.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
The study included 200 children in total; 50 monolingual autistic children (ASDmono), 50 bi-
lingual autistic children (ASDbi), 50 TD monolingual children (TDmono), et 50 TD bilingual
enfants (TDbi), ranging in age from 7 à 12 années. The autistic children were recruited from
schools and from public and private diagnostic centers in Greece. They had received a diagnosis
Neurobiology of Language
564
je
D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d
F
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
je
r
e
c
t
.
m
je
t
.
e
d
toi
n
o
/
je
/
je
un
r
t
je
c
e
–
p
d
F
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
je
F
b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
of autism from a licensed child psychiatrist or a developmental pediatrician according to the
standard diagnostic criteria (DSM-V; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). We confirmed
the diagnosis of children with autism using the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (ADI-R;
Rutter et al., 2003). ADI-R total scores, as well as scores from the reciprocal social, verbal com-
munication, and restricted and repetitive behaviors domains were used in the study as indicators
of autism symptomatology (Coutanche et al., 2011; Hus & Lord, 2014; Meilleur & Fombonne,
2009; Uddin et al., 2011). At the group level, the autistic children had a mean full scale IQ score
within the normal range (c'est à dire., full IQ > 70; voir, par exemple., Bal et al., 2021; Botting & Conti-Ramsden,
2003; Koyama et al., 2007; Siegel et al., 1996) as measured through the Greek version of the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ( WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991; adapted to and standard-
ized in Greek by Georgas et al., 2003); yet, 8 autistic children had an average verbal IQ (>70)
and a below average performance IQ score (<70), while 4 autistic children exhibited the
opposite pattern.
Typically developing children were recruited from public schools in Greece. The parents
who provided written consent for the children’s participation in the study completed a ques-
tionnaire that included demographic information. The questionnaire also asked parents to re-
port any family history of learning disabilities and their perception of any areas of difficulty for
their child. Only children whose parents did not report any history of language difficulties and
cognitive deficits participated in the study. This profile was also confirmed by the teachers’
written reports on the children’s academic achievements. Typically developing children were
also administered the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., 1992). Both
TDmono and TDbi children performed at a comparable level and within the normal range,
as expected for their age.
Details of participant demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1. Descriptives
were calculated using SummarySE (part of the Rmisc package; Hope, 2013) within the statis-
tical analysis software R (version 3.6.2; R Core Team, 2019), and reported as means and stan-
dard deviations. T tests were performed to examine whether the groups differed in age, SES,
autism symptomatology (only for the autistic groups), IQ, home language history (only for the
bilingual groups), and current language use (only for the bilingual groups). In addition, t tests
were used to compare the groups’ performance on two EF and two language ability measures.
All t tests were performed as unpaired two sample Welch t tests and are reported with un-
corrected p values, as their aim was to flag any potential between-group differences.
Figure 1 illustrates the density of individual scores per group on the total autism symptom-
atology score of the ADI-R.
The TD versus autism and monolingual versus bilingual groups did not differ significantly in
age and SES (which was measured through the mother’s education, in years). No IQ difference
was found either between ASDmono and ASDbi or between TDmono and TDbi groups based
on t tests. The two autistic groups did, however, differ in their ADI-R scores, specifically on their
reciprocal social scores (with bilingual children scoring lower than monolingual children with
autism; t = 5.36; df = 69; p < 0.001), and consequently on the total ADI-R score (t = 4.74; df =
81; p < 0.001). This difference can be seen in more detail in Figure 1. Note that for the other two
ADI-R measures, namely, verbal communication and restricted and repetitive behaviors, there
were no significant differences between the scores of the two autistic groups.
Monolingual children spoke Greek. Bilingual children, with and without autism, came from
mixed marriages, and they were exposed to both languages from the time of birth. Thus, they
were simultaneous bilinguals. The overwhelming majority of the children were Albanian-
Greek speakers (11 bilingual children were Russian-Greek or Bulgarian-Greek speakers) and
Neurobiology of Language
565
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
N
e
u
r
o
b
o
o
g
y
i
l
o
f
L
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
5
6
6
N
Gender (M/F)
Age range
SES range
IQ measures
VIQ (max. score: 160) range
PIQ (max. score: 160) range
IQ (max. score: 160) range
Raven (max. score: 36) range
Executive function measures
loc-to-glo range
glo-to-loc range
Language ability measures
Table 1. Demographic information.
TDmono
50
38/12
TDbi
50
40/10
ASDmono
50
40/10
ASDbi
50
38/12
9.68 (0.94) 7.2–10.9
9.84 (1.11) 6.7–11.1
9.94 (1.46) 6.7–11.8
9.66 (1.61) 7.0–12.0
9.92 (2.53) 3–14
9.76 (2.29) 6–14
9.84 (2.19) 6–12
9.04 (3.10) 3–14
–
–
–
–
–
–
89.30 (16.94) 50–142
90.32 (13.14) 70–109
88.58 (16.19) 66–133
90.96 (18.76) 49–127
88.06 (16.16) 63–135
89.12 (14.14) 64–117
26.2 (2.34) 20–30
26.6 (2.34) 16–31
–
–
3.66 (12.11) −13–60
7.72 (6.99) −7–20
48.68 (25.87) 14–85
8.70 (12.94) −16–29
24.70 (32.29) −4–94
19.19 (16.46) 3–114
11.24 (13.68) −8–57
26.12 (20.24) 0–63
C
o
g
n
i
t
i
v
e
f
l
e
x
i
b
i
l
i
t
y
i
n
b
i
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
a
u
t
i
s
t
i
c
c
h
i
l
d
r
e
n
Expressive vocabulary (max. score: 50) range
39.78 (6.33) 21–47
34.72 (5.34) 24–45
37.94 (6.28) 15–47
34.12 (6.74) 20–45
Sentence repetition (max. score: 96) range
65.92 (17.40) 32–92
63.61 (19.71)a 26–90
63.36 (18.89) 29–95
59.40 (20.97) 10–89
Bilingualism measures
Home language history (%) range
Current language use (%) range
ASD symptomatology (ADI-R)
SOC (cutoff score: 10) range
VC (cutoff score: 8) range
RRB (cutoff score: 3) range
ADI-R total range
42.80 (21.22)a 10–74
45.39 (12.82)a 29–67
38.76 (11.86) 20–62
49.04 (9.19) 30–67
21.02 (4.16) 14–29
17.54 (1.94) 14–23
12.10 (1.63) 9–15
12.16 (1.27) 9–14
4.08 (0.97) 2–6
3.92 (9.66) 2–6
37.20 (4.76) 29–46
33.46 (2.92) 26–42
Note. aN = 49. The demographic information is presented as Mean (SD), and includes: gender, age, socioeconomic status (SES), IQ measures ( VIQ = verbal IQ; PIQ = performance IQ; IQ =
general IQ for the autistic children; Raven scores for the TD children), executive function measures (glo-to-loc = costs of switching from a global to a local task; loc-to-glo = costs of
switching from a local to a global task), language ability measures, bilingualism measures (i.e., home language history and current language use in L1 [Albanian/ Russian/Bulgarian]),
and autism symptomatology measures (results from the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised [ADI-R]; SOC = reciprocal social, VC = verbal communication, RRB = restricted and repetitive
behaviors). TDmono = typically developing monolingual children; TDbi = typically developing bilingual children; ASDmono = monolingual autistic children; ASDbi = bilingual autistic
children.
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Figure 1. Distribution of the ADI-R total scores for the monolingual (ASDmono) and the bilingual
(ASDbi) autistic groups.
they were all dominant in Greek, which was the language used in school and often in the
home context. The children’s bilingual experience was based on a comprehensive parental
language history questionnaire, which asked about the home language history (i.e., the child’s
exposure to each language from birth up to the age of four) and current language use (i.e.,
literacy and language preference in everyday life with family members or friends) of the child
(Andreou, 2015). Following Andreou’s (2015) scoring of the questionnaire data, we awarded
points for input in each language. Points were accumulated based on the number of people
interacting with the child at different stages of development. For example, for home language,
one or the other language was given 1 point, depending on whether a certain family member
interacted with the child (father, mother, siblings, grandparents, etc.) in Greek or
Albanian/Russian/Bulgarian, respectively. If a person interacted with the child in both lan-
guages, the point was divided between the two languages (0.5 points each). This score was
normalized (in percentage) for the total number of people interacting with the child (in one
language or another). Table 1 presents the home language history and current language use
percentages for the Albanian/Russian/Bulgarian language. Implicitly, the remaining percent-
ages correspond to the Greek language. The two bilingual groups did not differ significantly
in home language history and current language use based on t tests.
The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Greek Ministry of
Education, and parental consent was required for participation in the study.
Procedure
Tests were presented in a fixed order, and scored according to standardized criteria. All the
children completed two language ability tasks (expressive vocabulary, sentence repetition), an
EF task (global-local cognitive flexibility), and three dual-task paradigms (listening span, syn-
tactic interference word recall, proactive interference) during three sessions in a quiet room at
the children’s home or school setting. All assessments were administered in Greek.
Expressive vocabulary task (Vogindroukas et al., 2009; adaptation from Renfrew, 1997)
The children’s expressive vocabulary in Greek was assessed through an expressive vocabulary
test, which has been standardized for 3- to 10-year-old Greek-speaking monolingual children.
It includes 50 black-and-white pictures of common objects that each child was asked to name
individually. Each correct answer earns one point, with a maximum score of 50. The test was
terminated when the participant failed to respond correctly to five consecutive trials.
Neurobiology of Language
567
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Sentence repetition task
This task was developed within the Cost Action IS0804, Language Impairment in a Multilingual
Society; Linguistic Patterns and the Road to Assessment (Chondrogianni et al., 2013).
It includes 32 Greek sentences of varying degrees of syntactic complexity, such as
Stimuli.
coordination and subordination. Subordinate clauses include adverbial, complement, and rel-
ative clauses. Examples of a complement and a relative clause of the task are illustrated in (1)
and (2) below.
Procedure. Children sat in front of a computer screen and listened to the sentences via head-
phones. All sentences were recorded by a female native speaker of Greek and had normal
intonation. An attempt to repeat a sentence was considered to be correct if it was a verbatim
repetition of the target sentence. Children performed 5 practice trials and then the 32 exper-
imental sentences.
Scoring. Children’s performance on the sentence repetition task was scored for overall accu-
racy (Marinis & Armon-Lotem, 2015; Semel et al., 2003). More specifically, they were
awarded a score of 3 if they made no errors while repeating the sentence, a score of 2 if they
made one error, a score of 1 if they made two errors, and zero points if they made three or
more errors.
(1)
(2)
i nosokomes ipan oti i ptisi tu jiatru ehi kaθisterisi.
The nurses.NOM.FEM.PL said.PERF.INDIC.3P.PL that the flight.NOM.FEM.SG. of the
doctor.GEN.MASC.SG has.IMPERF.INDIC.3P.SG. delay.ACC.FEM.SG
“The nurses said that the flight of the doctor is delayed.”
i kaθaristria klotsise tin nosokoma pu vjike apo to (cid:1)rafio.
The cleaning-lady.NOM.FEM.SG kicked.PERF.INDIC.3P.SG the nurse.ACC.FEM.SG. that
got.PERF.INDIC.3P.SG out of the office.ACC.NEUT.SG
“The cleaning-lady kicked the nurse that got out of the office.”
Global-local cognitive flexibility task
To assess the children’s ability to shift their attention to different levels of compound shape
stimuli, we utilized a global-local cognitive flexibility task. The specific task was run on a com-
puter with E-Prime (Schneider et al., 2002) and was based on a Navon-type letter task (Navon,
1977) that required participants to shift attention between global and local levels of a visual
stimulus. The particular task was chosen among other EF paradigms, since it is a task in which
participants are expected to show flexible behaviors (Ionescu, 2012). It can thus be treated as a
nonverbal measure of cognitive flexibility.
Stimuli. Four different shapes were used, namely, circles, Xs, triangles, and rectangles. Large
shapes (global level) were composed of small shapes (local level). To present two conditions,
large shapes were constructed from either identical small shapes (congruent condition; e.g., a
big triangle made up of smaller triangles) or different shapes (incongruent condition; e.g., a big
triangle made up of small Xs; see Figure 2).
Procedure. The task of the children was to identify the number of lines required to form each
shape (i.e., one line for circles, two for Xs, three for triangles, and four for rectangles) by
Neurobiology of Language
568
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Figure 2. Example stimuli from the global-local cognitive flexibility task. The two shapes on the
left are congruent trials, while the two shapes on the right are incongruent trials. The scale from
local to global was 1:10. (Figure 2 is from Peristeri et al., 2021).
pressing one of the four numbers (1, 2, 3, or 4) on the keyboard. The task comprised three build-
ing blocks, namely, the Global-No Shift, in which participants were asked to continuously re-
spond to the global shape, the Local-No Shift, in which participants were asked to continuously
respond to the local shape, and the Shift block, in which participants had to interchangeably
respond to the global and local shape (global, local, global, local, etc.). Each target stimulus was
presented on the screen until the participant responded. Children performed 8 practice trials
and then 144 experimental trials. After completion of each block of 48 trials, each child was
allowed a 5-minute break before completing the next block of the task.
Scoring. For the purposes of the present study, we used data from both the Shift and the No
Shift blocks to measure shifting costs. More specifically, we calculated local-to-global cogni-
tive flexibility cost in the accuracy measure by subtracting accuracy scores in the local-to-
global trials of the Shift block from accuracy in the global trials of the Global-No Shift block.
Conversely, global-to-local cognitive flexibility cost in accuracy was calculated by subtracting
accuracy scores in the global-to-local trials of the Shift block from accuracy in the local trials
of the Local-No Shift block. The global-to-local and local-to-global shifting costs reflect global-
to-local and local-to-global interference, respectively.
Dual-task paradigms: Listening span task
The listening span task was adapted from Chrysochoou (2006).
Stimuli. The task included sentences arranged in separate blocks, beginning with single sen-
tences and ending with six-sentence blocks in ascending order. Each subsequent block in-
creased by one sentence. All of the sentences included four to six words. All of the
sentence-final words were mono- or bisyllabic, high frequency, phonologically simple nouns
and adjectives. Approximately equal numbers of nouns and adjectives appeared in
sentence-final position across the sentences. Half of the sentences were semantically plau-
sible (e.g., The sky is pink.), while the rest were nonsensical (e.g., Houses can laugh.).
Table 2 presents the experimental items in the single- and the two-sentence blocks of the
listening span task.
Procedure. Children listened to the prerecorded sentences, verified their semantic plausibil-
ity (truth/falsity), and were then asked to recall the individual words at the end of each sen-
tence set. More specifically, the child was told that they will hear a female voice uttering
sentences and then would need to do two things. First, respond “Yes” or “No” as to whether
the event that the sentence describes could happen in real life and then recall as many of
the sentence-final words at the end of each sentence block as they could remember. Serial
order recall was not required. To pass on to the next sentence block, the child had to suc-
ceed in four-word recall trials. Responses were recorded by the examiner on an answer
sheet.
Neurobiology of Language
569
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Table 2.
Stimuli in the single- and the two-sentence blocks of the listening span task.
Level
1
Sentences
Scissors cut paper.
Goldfish have hair.
Lemons are sour.
Spiders make webs.
Clocks eat apples.
Giants are short.
2
Oranges live in the water.
Roses smell sweet.
Chairs give birth to eggs.
Bananas have teeth.
We wear shoes on our feet.
Apples grow on trees.
Humans have ears.
Oranges play the piano.
Fingers are on our hands.
Sheep have wings.
Cats work at schools.
Pigs have spun tails.
True/False
True
Words to-be-recalled
paper
False
True
True
False
False
False
True
False
False
True
True
True
False
True
False
False
True
hair
sour
webs
apples
short
water
sweet
eggs
teeth
feet
trees
ears
piano
hands
wings
schools
tails
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
Note. The task was administered in Greek.
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Scoring. According to the standard scoring procedure (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980), a score
of 6 was awarded if the child made four correct successive final-word recall attempts, a score of
5 if one error is made, and a score of 4 if two errors are made independently of the child's
accuracy performance in semantic plausibility judgment. If three errors were made, testing
ceased. Nevertheless, to effectively capture interference from semantic plausibility judgment
in word recall, we have only included in the word recall measure the items of the trials in which
the child’s semantic plausibility judgment was correct. The outcome measures were: the final-
word recall score, the number of the sentence-blocks that the child succeeded in passing on to,
and the semantic judgment accuracy score. The maximum final-word recall score was 36,
while the maximum score for the sentence-blocks was 6 points. Internal consistency reliability
coefficients for the composite score ranged from 0.89 to 0.94 (Median = 0.93).
Dual-task paradigms: Syntactic interference word recall task
Stimuli. The task included four blocks of sentences, each including one active transitive, one
passive, one subject-relative, and one object-relative sentence. There were 16 sentences in
total. All the sentences included animate agents and patients and as such, they were seman-
tically reversible. Two- and three-syllable, high frequency nouns and verbs were used in the
sentences. Each sentence was accompanied by a multiple-choice image array. Each array
Neurobiology of Language
570
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Figure 3.
subject-relative; TR = active transitive; PASS = passive; OR = object-relative.
Example stimuli of a sentence-picture matching and a word recall block of the syntactic interference word recall task. SR =
consisted of three pictures: one target and two foils. Each sentence block included four to-be-
remembered words, which were two- or three-syllable nouns from different semantic catego-
ries; they did not rhyme with each other and were unrelated to the sentences. As such, each
experimental block included four picture-matching trials and four words to-be-recalled. The
position of the sentences in each block and the location of the target in each image array were
counterbalanced across the four blocks. An example of a block of the syntactic interference
word recall task is presented in Figure 3.
Procedure. The task was run on PowerPoint (Microsoft Corporation). Children were asked to
listen to the blocks of sentences, select the picture that best matched the meaning of each
sentence, and recall the words that popped up between the sentence-picture matching trials.
More specifically, along with the auditory presentation of each sentence, the multiple-choice
image array was presented. The child was asked to point to the picture that best matched the
meaning of the sentence. After each sentence-picture matching trial, a word was auditorily
presented and displayed on the center of the screen. The child was asked to remember the
word so that they could recall it later on. After the fourth word was auditorily presented
and displayed on the computer screen, a blank screen appeared and the child was asked to
recall aloud as many of the words as they could remember. Responses were recorded by the
examiner on an answer sheet.
Scoring. The dependent variables were: the word recall score and the sentence-picture
matching accuracy score (Peristeri & Tsimpli, 2010). The child was awarded 1 point for each
correct sentence-picture matching trial and 1 point for each successful word recall trial. The
maximum score for both sentence comprehension and word recall was 16 points each.
Dual-task paradigms: Proactive interference task
The proactive interference task of the present study was modelled after Bialystok and Feng’s
(2009) study.
Stimuli. There were four lists of words containing seven words each. The words in the first
three lists belonged to the same semantic category (i.e., fruit), while the words in the
final/fourth list belonged to sports.
Procedure. Experimental stimuli were displayed on a monitor controlled by E-prime software
(Schneider et al., 2002). Children first viewed a red light warning signal (“Get ready for the first
List”) against a white background for 2 s, after which the words from the first list appeared one
at a time. The words across the four lists appeared in black small letters against a white back-
ground. Following Hamilton and Martin’s (2007) paradigm, each of the seven words was pre-
sented for 1,000 ms, separated by a 100 ms inter-stimulus interval. Auditory files of the word
stimuli were recorded as WAV files and each word was auditorily presented through computer
Neurobiology of Language
571
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
speakers as soon as it was displayed on the screen. Immediately after the final word in each list
disappeared, a random number between 19 and 22 appeared on the screen and the partici-
pant was orally instructed to count forward from the specific number for 10 s. After the child
counted for 10 s, a blank screen appeared and an oral instruction cued them to orally recall
the words from the list. Children had 20 s to recall the words in any order and were encour-
aged to keep working to recall throughout the entire period. Responses were recorded by the
examiner on an answer sheet. At the end of the recall period, a screen appeared for 2 s, which
instructed the children to stop recalling. The same sequence, beginning with the red warning
signal, was repeated for lists 2, 3, and 4.
Scoring. The mean number of words that had been accurately recalled per list was recorded
on an answer sheet and counted for each child. Proactive interference effects were subse-
quently calculated for lists 2 and 3 by subtracting the number of words recalled on list 1 from
the number of words recalled on lists 2 and 3.
Analyses
The experimental tasks were coded for analysis. All analyses were performed within the sta-
tistical analysis software R (version 3.6.2; R Core Team, 2019). First, we present the results of
the Welch t tests, which were used to compare the groups’ performance on the two language
ability measures (i.e., expressive vocabulary and sentence repetition), and the global-local
cognitive flexibility task. Linear regression analyses were then carried out to examine the in-
fluence of bilingualism, autism, age, SES, autism symptomatology/ADI-R totals (only for the
autistic groups), IQ, home language history (only for the bilingual groups), current language
use (only for the bilingual groups), EF (global-local cognitive flexibility and local-global cog-
nitive flexibility), and language ability (expressive vocabulary and sentence repetition) as in-
dependent variables on word recall as the dependent variable in each task. Independent
variables that did not reach significance were retained in the models so that the output of
the different models is maximally comparable. Uncorrected p values are reported throughout
the article and in the Supporting Information (located at https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00055).
In addition, only for the syntactic interference task, overall sentence comprehension was
examined as a dependent variable to determine the groups’ syntactic abilities. Separate
models were developed for each of the dependent variables. Treatment coding was used
for the groups (bilingualism and autism); all other dependent and independent variables were
numeric. Separate models were performed for the TD and the autistic groups in order to get a
full overview of the factors influencing the performance of the different groups. This could be
seen as an alternative to testing for interactions, which, with so many factors, would quickly
become difficult to interpret. In addition, testing the groups in pairs eliminates the need for the
post hoc testing of any main effects of group (e.g., through follow-up pairwise t tests). Thus, the
choice of running models separately for the TD and the autistic children was made to increase
the interpretability of the analyses as well as to remove the need for follow-up, post hoc com-
parisons. The full model output can be found in the Supporting Information.
To correct for multiple comparisons, the false discovery rate controlling procedure
(Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) was used. More specifically, we corrected for the fact that mul-
tiple tests were used to investigate the two main variables of interest, namely bilingualism and
autism. In total, 10 different tests examined the effects of bilingualism and 10 different tests
examined the effects of autism. The false discovery rate controlling procedure corrects the sig-
nificance thresholds of those tests from the typical (cid:3) = 0.05 threshold to a lower threshold of (cid:3) =
Neurobiology of Language
572
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
0.05i/n, with i being the comparison number based on the ordered p values and n being the
total number of comparisons for a variable of interest (10 in this case; for more details, see
Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Cases in which the significance after corrections for multiple
comparisons differs from the significance before corrections will be mentioned explicitly in
the text.
Finally, the standardized effect sizes of the significant effects of bilingualism in autism for
each of the dual-task paradigms were calculated using Cohen’s d (with effect sizes of d < 0.5
being small; d < 0.8 being medium; otherwise being large; Cohen, 1992).
RESULTS
Expressive Vocabulary and Sentence Repetition Tasks
Both groups of bilingual children showed lower expressive vocabulary scores than their mono-
lingual peers (TD groups: t = 4.32; df = 95; p < 0.001, autistic groups: t = 2.93; df = 98; p <
0.01). No significant differences in the sentence repetition scores were found between the
groups.
Global-Local Cognitive Flexibility Task
On the EF measure, local-to-global cognitive flexibility costs did not differ significantly be-
tween the two TD groups, nor between the bilingual TD group and the bilingual autistic group.
However, significant differences were found between the two autistic groups, with monolin-
gual children with autism showing increased cognitive flexibility costs (t = 4.30; df = 86; p <
0.001), and between the two monolingual groups, again with monolingual autistic children
showing increased cognitive flexibility costs (t = 2.71; df = 66; p < 0.01). Thus, monolingual
autistic children seemed to show increased costs when switching from the local to the global
level, whereas bilingual autistic children performed similarly to their TD peers. Interestingly,
cognitive flexibility costs for switching in the other direction, namely from the global to the
local level, showed the opposite effects: monolingual autistic children showed decreased costs
compared to bilingual autistic children (t = 9.77; df = 72; p < 0.001) and monolingual TD
children (t = 11.14; df = 70; p < 0.001).
Dual-Task Paradigms
The results for the different dual-task paradigms are listed in Table 3. For each task, all mea-
sures are reported for completeness of information. However, only the word recall perfor-
mance will be analyzed for all dual-task paradigms, as this is the primary task where
interference from the secondary tasks is likely to occur. After discussing the general patterns
in the results, namely, the effects of bilingualism and autism, we will additionally examine the
groups’ syntactic abilities in the syntactic interference task more closely.
Effects of Bilingualism
The first set of regression analyses examined whether the interference effect in the dual-task
paradigms is attenuated by bilingualism in autistic children and TD children. To this end, the
two TD groups (monolinguals and bilinguals) were analyzed separately from the two autistic
groups (monolinguals and bilinguals). The full model results can be found in the Supporting
Information.
Neurobiology of Language
573
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Table 3.
Results for the three different dual-task paradigms.
TDmono
TDbi
ASDmono
ASDbi
Listening span task
Word recall
Block level
Semantic judgment
6.28 (2.78)
1.22 (0.76)
9.86 (3.45)
7.64 (3.27)
1.66 (0.80)
11.00 (3.94)
Syntactic interference word recall task
Word recall
12.18 (2.00)
12.48 (1.61)
Recall after transitives
Recall after passives
Recall after subject relatives
Recall after object relatives
3.60 (0.67)
2.66 (0.98)
3.00 (0.90)
2.92 (0.88)
3.74 (0.49)
2.70 (1.09)
2.98 (0.87)
3.06 (0.87)
Sentence comprehension
12.12 (2.29)
10.20 (2.36)
Proactive Interference task
Score list 1
Score list 2
Score list 3
Score list 4
Proactive interference list 2
Proactive interference list 3
5.18 (0.66)
4.06 (1.22)
3.80 (1.21)
5.86 (0.61)
−1.12 (1.08)
−1.38 (1.12)
5.48 (0.79)
4.26 (1.37)
4.16 (1.33)
6.12 (0.80)
−1.22 (1.27)
−1.32 (1.24)
5.26 (2.26)
1.34 (1.14)
8.48 (4.73)
8.60 (2.78)
2.28 (1.07)
2.22 (0.97)
2.08 (0.97)
2.02 (1.04)
9.28 (2.75)
4.58 (1.11)
2.26 (1.10)
1.72 (0.73)
5.10 (1.05)
−2.32 (1.28)
−2.86 (1.29)
8.36 (3.78)
1.86 (0.97)
11.58 (4.73)
10.60 (3.52)
2.96 (1.05)
2.24 (1.22)
2.76 (1.35)
2.64 (0.85)
9.52 (2.71)
4.78 (1.18)
4.08 (1.37)
3.74 (1.69)
5.74 (1.01)
−0.70 (1.28)
−1.04 (1.71)
Note. The results are presented as Mean (SD). TDmono = typically developing monolingual children; TDbi = typically developing bilingual children;
ASDmono = monolingual autistic children; ASDbi = bilingual autistic children.
On word recall in the listening span task, the results from the TD groups show no significant
difference between the performance of TD bilingual children and TD monolingual children,
although a trend of TD bilingual children performing better than TD monolingual children was
observed ( p = 0.06). Only sentence repetition significantly correlated with performance on
word recall for the TD children ( p < 0.01). On the same task, the results from the autistic
groups show that bilingual autistic children outperformed monolingual autistic children ( p <
0.001; effect size d = 1.03 (large), CI [0.61, 1.45]) indicating that bilingualism can attenuate
language interference effects in a word meaning-based task in autistic children. The results
from the autistic groups further show positive effects of EF (both global-local and local-
global switching costs; p < 0.01).
On word recall in the syntactic interference word recall task, no significant effect of bilin-
gualism was found for the TD children, although a trend was observed ( p = 0.07). In contrast,
significant effects of age ( p < 0.05) and sentence repetition were found ( p < 0.05). For the
autistic children, the effect of bilingualism did reach significance, with bilingual autistic chil-
dren outperforming their monolingual peers ( p < 0.05; effect size d = 0.63 (medium), CI [0.22,
1.04]). In addition, an effect of expressive vocabulary score was found, indicating that children
Neurobiology of Language
574
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
who performed better on this measure of general language ability scored better on word recall
in the syntactic interference word recall task ( p < 0.01).
Finally, on the proactive interference task, we analyzed the proactive interference on lists 2
and 3 compared to list 1. For the TD children, no significant effects of bilingualism were found
on list 2 or list 3. On the proactive interference on list 2, only a positive effect of expressive
vocabulary score was found ( p < 0.05). On the proactive interference on list 3, no significant
effects of any of the variables of interest were found. For the autistic children, a positive effect
of bilingualism was found for list 2 ( p < 0.05; effect size d = 1.26 (large), CI [0.83, 1.70]),
indicating that bilingual autistic children performed better than their monolingual peers.
Notably, the monolingual autistic children scored much lower on list 2 than the other three
groups. On list 3, the monolingual children still scored lower, but the influence of bilingualism
on proactive interference on list 3 did not reach significance in the regression model. Instead,
an effect of EF (global-to-local switching costs) was found ( p < 0.05)
Overall, effects of bilingualism were found for the autistic children on all three different
dual-task paradigms. Note that the standardized effect sizes differed between the three differ-
ent tasks: The sizes of the effects of bilingualism were large in the listening span and in the
proactive interference task, whereas the effect size was medium in the syntactic interference
word recall task. If we assume that the study has a sufficient sample size and sufficient power
to draw conclusions from the observed effect sizes, we can conclude that the effect of bilin-
gualism in autism in the syntactic interference task is smaller relative to the other two dual-task
paradigms. Reasons for why this could be the case are discussed in the Discussion section. For
the TD children, there were marginal effects of bilingualism on the listening span task and the
syntactic interference task, but not on the proactive interference task.
Effects of Autism
The second set of regression analyses examined the extent to which the performance of the TD
bilingual children and bilingual autistic children in the dual-tasks is affected by home lan-
guage history and current language use metrics. In addition, we compared dual-task perfor-
mance effects in monolingual children with and without autism. To this end, the two
monolingual groups (TD and autism) were analyzed separately from the two bilingual groups
(TD and autism). The full model results can be found in the Supporting Information.
On word recall in the listening span task, the results from the monolingual groups show that
TD children significantly outperformed autistic children ( p < 0.01). In addition, EF skills sig-
nificantly correlated with performance on word recall ( p < 0.05); more specifically, local-
global switching costs were found to be positively correlated ( p < 0.05) with word recall.
The results from the bilingual groups show, in contrast, no effect of autism, indicating that
autistic children performed similarly to TD children. Effects of language ability (sentence rep-
etition; p < 0.01) were found, with higher scores on sentence repetition correlating with better
scores on word recall. Notably, no significant effects of home language history and current
language use were found.
On word recall in the syntactic interference task, a significant difference was found be-
tween TD monolingual children and monolingual autistic children ( p < 0.05), with the latter
group underperforming. In addition, the analyses of the monolingual groups showed a nega-
tive effect of SES ( p < 0.01) and a positive effect of sentence repetition score ( p < 0.01). The
results from the bilingual groups also showed a significant negative effect of autism (i.e., TD
bilinguals outperformed those with autism; p < 0.01). In addition, a positive relation between
Neurobiology of Language
575
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
expressive vocabulary and word recall in the syntactic interference task was found ( p < 0.05).
No other variables had a significant effect on bilingual children’s performance.
For the proactive interference task, we again analyzed the proactive interference on lists 2
and 3 compared to list 1. For the monolingual children, no significant effects of autism were
found on list 2 or list 3 (although a trend was found for list 2, p = 0.06; for list 3, p = 0.10). On
the proactive interference on list 2, no significant effects of any of the variables of interest were
found. On the proactive interference on list 3, only a negative effect of local-to-global switch-
ing costs was found ( p < 0.01). For the bilingual groups, the effect of autism on proactive
interference on list 2 just reached significance ( p < 0.05), implying that interference may
be lower for bilingual autistic children compared to TD bilingual children. However, this effect
loses significance when correcting for multiple comparisons. In addition, it should be noted
that the bilingual autistic children overall scored lower on the recall task than TD bilingual
children, but their performance decreased less when interference occurred. Other effects
found for proactive interference on list 2 were local-to-global switching costs ( p < 0.05)
and expressive vocabulary ( p < 0.05). For proactive interference on list 3, no significant effects
of autism were found; only an effect of local-to-global switching costs ( p < 0.05) was found.
Syntactic Abilities in Monolingual and Bilingual Children With and Without Autism
A final set of regression analyses examined the groups’ syntactic abilities as displayed in the
syntactic interference task more closely. The full model results can be found in the Supporting
Information. On overall sentence comprehension scores, TD monolinguals outperformed TD
bilinguals ( p < 0.05), but autistic bilinguals outperformed autistic monolinguals ( p < 0.05). In
addition, in the autistic groups there were effects of age (negative, p < 0.05), EF (global-to-local
switching costs, negative, p < 0.001), and language abilities (expressive vocabulary, positive,
p < 0.001; sentence repetition, positive, p < 0.001). The TD groups did not show effects of EF
or language abilities.
When comparing the two monolingual groups, TD monolingual children outperformed
monolingual autistic children on sentence comprehension ( p < 0.001). Again, a negative cor-
relation with age was found ( p < 0.05), as well as a positive correlation with sentence repe-
tition ( p < 0.01). Finally, in the bilingual groups no effect of autism was found. The bilingual
groups only showed a significant correlation between expressive vocabulary ( p < 0.01) and
sentence comprehension in the syntactic interference task.
DISCUSSION
The current study investigated bilingualism effects in the dual-task performance of autistic chil-
dren and age- and SES-matched TD children. Our design hinges on a key feature of the cog-
nitive profile in autism known as rigidity, which prevents individuals on the autism spectrum
from efficiently regulating their behavior and allocating attention to more than one input
stream in dual-task conditions (Geurts et al., 2009; Poljac & Bekkering, 2012; Schmitz
et al., 2006; Yerys et al., 2015). Reduced cognitive flexibility often makes autistic individuals
persevere on discrete and narrow information, struggling with transitions or not seeing novel
relationships in multitasking situations (Bos et al., 2019; D’Cruz et al., 2013). Conversely, dual
language exposure has been linked to advantages in nonverbal EF, especially in sustained at-
tention (Sharaan et al., 2020), set shifting (Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, 2019), and inhibition
(Peristeri et al., 2020). Experimental evidence of the effects of bilingualism on the cognitive
flexibility of autistic children remains limited. This study aimed to explore whether bilingual-
ism would compensate for reduced cognitive flexibility in autism with a focus on language,
Neurobiology of Language
576
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
and more specifically, on dual-task conditions in which language processing in the form of
semantic judgment, sentence parsing, and digit counting (as a secondary task) interfered with
word recall (as the primary task). As such, the degree of interference across the dual-task par-
adigms varied based on the type of the language processing that needed to be performed dur-
ing the secondary, interfering task. The results revealed that bilingual autistic children
significantly outperformed their monolingual peers in the primary task (i.e., word recall) across
all three dual-tasks, which suggests that bilingualism can mitigate cognitive flexibility deficits
in autism. Crucially, autistic children’s performance in the dual-tasks was found to be modu-
lated mainly by EF skills, which implies that autistic children relied more on EF rather than
their language skills to cope with the dual-task demands.
Our first research question was whether bilingualism would affect TD and autistic children’s
word recall performance in the dual-task paradigms. Bilingual autistic children scored higher
than their monolingual peers in all three dual-task paradigms (i.e., the listening span, the syn-
tactic interference word recall, and the proactive interference task), while their TD peers showed
no significant effects of bilingualism in any of the three dual-task paradigms. These findings sug-
gest that bilingualism enabled autistic children to cope better than their monolingual peers with
multitasking and the demands on distinct control processes that needed to be implemented,
such as adaptive task control, interference detection and inhibition, and attention.
More specifically, in the listening span task, the bilingual autistic group outperformed the
monolingual autistic group, while better word recall scores were found in the children
experiencing higher costs of switching from global-to-local and local-to-global trials.
Interestingly, monolingual autistic children exhibited a more local-level processing style in
the global-local cognitive flexibility task in comparison to their bilingual peers. This effect
was not due to outliers; in fact, there were multiple monolingual autistic children that showed
very high local-to-global switching costs, and the highest costs (as well as the lowest ones)
were within 2 standard deviations of the group’s mean. It is noteworthy that the same mono-
lingual autistic children, who showed high local-to-global switching costs, exhibited lower
costs when shifting from the global to the local level compared to their bilingual peers (see
t test comparisons of the children’s scores on the global-local cognitive flexibility task in the
Results section). This finding is in line with previous research (e.g., Happé & Frith, 2006; Muth
et al., 2014) showing that autistic individuals demonstrate a unique detail-focused processing
style at the expense of the larger picture. This discrepancy between the autistic groups’ perfor-
mance in the global-local cognitive flexibility task is not necessarily an issue from a statistical
point of view; in fact, to control for this difference, we have included the global-to-local and
local-to-global measures in all the statistical models of the study. Furthermore, in the listening
span task, there were no significant word recall differences between bilingual children with and
without autism. In the syntactic interference word recall task, monolingual autistic children re-
called significantly fewer words than both TD monolingual and autistic bilingual children.
In the proactive interference task, the expected decline in word recall between lists 1 and 3
was found across all the experimental groups. Children showed an increase in competition
between the items to-be-recalled and the reactivated non-target items as the experiment prog-
ressed, with the decrease in word recall performance being the highest on list 3. Differences in
the proactive interference build-up effect were most explicitly shown on list 2 of the task, in
which monolingual autistic children scored lower, and thus, showed a sharper memory de-
cline than bilingual autistic children. Whether the interference effect exhibited by the mono-
lingual autistic group in the proactive interference task reflects the children’s inability to
efficiently inhibit memory traces of previously presented items that persisted in memory or
to a rapid decay of lexical representations cannot be conclusively determined. Christ et al.
Neurobiology of Language
577
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
(2011) have also examined proactive interference effects in a group of autistic individuals
ranging in age from 8 to 18 years; though the autistic participants scored lower than their
TD peers on list 2 (compared to list 1) of the task, the difference was not statistically significant.
However, the proactive interference task in Christ et al.’s (2011) study included 4-item lists (vs.
our paradigm that included 7-item lists), while the lexical items were presented in written form
along with their corresponding images, which may have boosted autistic children’s memory
retrieval capacity (Mostert-Kerckhoffs et al., 2015). The overall evidence from the dual-task par-
adigms of the present study demonstrates that bilingualism compensated for reduced cognitive
flexibility in autism. The effect of bilingualism on autistic children’s cognitive flexibility profile is
consistent with previous studies that have also found that bilingualism attenuates autism-related
deficits in core cognitive areas, including theory of mind (Andreou et al., 2020), language
adaptation to specific contexts (Yu, 2016), and narrative ability (Peristeri et al., 2020).
Our prediction about the way performance in the dual-task paradigms would be modulated
by the language demands in each task was verified. Bilingualism effects for the autistic chil-
dren were stronger in the listening span task as compared to the syntactic interference word
recall paradigm. According to our initial prediction, semantic judgment in the listening span
task was hypothesized to be a cognitively non-taxing secondary task which allowed bilingual
autistic children to be more tuned into the primary task and thus recall more words than the
monolingual autistic group, which scored significantly lower than TD controls. Monolingual
autistic children’s poor word recall capacity in the listening span task seems to be associated
with difficulties in resolving interference from the secondary, semantic plausibility judgment
task. In this measure, accuracy scores of the monolingual autistic group were the lowest of all
groups. It is therefore likely that semantic plausibility judgment had a greater interfering effect
relative to the rest of the dual-task paradigms, which made it harder for the monolingual
autistic children to retrieve the target final words of the sentences. Their low performance
may be associated with the fact that sentences in the semantic judgment task required lex-
ical semantics and, most importantly, world knowledge reasoning. More specifically, judging
the plausibility of the sentences tapped into either broad world knowledge information about
habitual events (e.g., Spiders make webs) or the borderline meaning of vague predicates
such as relative adjectives (e.g., Giants are short; Roses smell sweet) that require a mental
context for their interpretation. According to recent findings (Farmer et al., 2017; Howard
et al., 2017; Saldaña & Frith, 2007), the indeterminacy of vague linguistic expressions whose
meaning draws on context, and the integration of world knowledge in language comprehen-
sion can be problematic for autistic individuals, and this may explain the low performance
of monolingual autistic children in semantic judgment.
Similarly, the bilingualism effect in autism was stronger in the proactive interference task
relative to the syntactic interference word recall task. Specifically, in the proactive interference
task, interference from non-target words sharing semantic categorical similarity may have pe-
nalized monolingual autistic children’s word retrieval capacities. Similarity-based interference
has been widely acknowledged as a key factor that contributes to processing difficulty in lan-
guage production and comprehension (Fukumura et al., 2011; Lewis & Vasishth, 2005;
McElree, 2000), and may have also modulated the strength of the bilingualism effect on au-
tistic children’s word recall performance in the current study. On the other hand, in the syn-
tactic interference task, bilingual autistic children scored higher in word recall than their
monolingual peers, with better word recall scores being reported for younger children
experiencing less global-to-local interference, as well as children with better sentence repeti-
tion and expressive vocabulary skills. However, the effect size of the bilingualism effect in
autism in the syntactic interference task was smaller relative to the other two dual-task
Neurobiology of Language
578
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
paradigms. The regression analyses of the syntactic interference data show that bilingual au-
tistic children’s lower scores in sentence repetition and expressive vocabulary as compared to
the monolingual autistic group may have contributed to processing difficulty with sentence
parsing, subsequently freeing up fewer cognitive resources for the retention in memory and
the retrieval of the lexical items in the word recall component of the dual-task.
The study’s second research question focused on the role of individual differences on
monolingual and bilingual autistic children’s performance in the dual-task paradigms with a
particular emphasis on EF, age, SES, language ability, IQ, and autism symptomatology, as well
as home language history and current language use for the bilingual children only. In the lis-
tening span task, EF effects were found to be mostly relevant to the autistic rather than TD
children. More specifically, for the combined autistic groups, global-to-local and local-to-
global interference affected their performance in the word recall component of the task, while
TD children (monolinguals and bilinguals combined) seemed to rely more on their sentence
repetition skills. As already mentioned, the listening span test found no significant behavioral
differences between bilingual children with and without autism, whose performance was cor-
related with sentence repetition. EF effects seemed to be least influential in the syntactic inter-
ference task. Specifically, bilingual autistic children’s higher word recall performance than
monolingual autistic children’s was associated with expressive vocabulary, while autistic chil-
dren’s lower word recall performance than TD children’s was found to be associated with
autistic children’s lower performance in sentence repetition and expressive vocabulary skills.
Furthermore, in the proactive interference task, word recall on list 2 for the bilingual autistic
group was higher for the children experiencing stronger local-to-global interference.
None of the regression analyses showed significant effects of autism symptomatology or IQ
score for the autistic children. Therefore, it is unlikely that the differences between monolin-
gual and bilingual autistic children in the dual-task paradigms are due to a difference in autism
symptomatology. Though bilingual autistic children scored lower than their monolingual peers
on autism symptomatology, and more specifically in reciprocal social skills, this difference
was not reflected in the groups’ performance in the dual-task paradigms. Of course, this
may be due to difficulty in finely quantifying autism symptomatology with the ADI-R tool;
symptoms of autistic individuals are measured on a relatively coarse scale, which raises the
possibility that this scale could not fully capture nuanced individual differences in autism-
related symptomatology. Furthermore, there were no effects of age and SES on autistic chil-
dren’s performance, and no separate influence of home language history or current language
use for the bilingual children. However, it could be the case that these demographic measures
were captured by other variables included in the models, such as EF and language abilities,
and therefore failed to reach significance as a separate factor.
The extent to which bilingual autistic children’s cognitive flexibility skills map on brain
functions remains unknown, as the underlying spatial and temporal dynamics of cognitive
flexibility have not been studied yet in bilingual autistic populations. Studies that have ex-
plored the neural correlates of set-shifting performance in TD bilingual children converge to
show that bilinguals outperform monolinguals, and that this flexibility advantage likely reflects
enhanced left prefrontal and inferior frontal functions (Garbin et al., 2010; Moriguchi &
Lertladaluck, 2020; Stocco et al., 2014), which have been reported to be recruited for lan-
guage control in bilinguals (Calabria et al., 2012; Jasinska & Petitto, 2013). The specific find-
ings imply that enhanced cognitive flexibility in TD bilingual children is supported by
language-specific mechanisms, though there are studies that support the idea that bilingual
language control is a subdomain of general executive control (Abutalebi et al., 2008, 2011;
Craik & Bialystok, 2006; Garbin et al., 2010). The dissociation of the brain areas involved in
Neurobiology of Language
579
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
set shifting across TD monolinguals and bilinguals, coupled with the atypical brain activation
patterns in monolingual autistic children (Taylor et al., 2012; Yerys et al., 2015), makes accu-
rate characterization of the brain network underlying cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic
children all the more challenging. Activation of left prefrontal and inferior frontal brain re-
gions, which have been shown to be correlated with language control in bilinguals, may pro-
vide a mechanism for improved cognitive flexibility performance in bilingual autistic children
as compared to monolingual autistic children. Neuroimaging can provide a means for under-
standing neurobiological differences between monolingual and bilingual autistic populations.
Future directions of our research include attempts at investigating cortical brain volumes in
monolingual and bilingual autistic children, and correlating these measures with behavioral
outcomes in nonverbal and verbal cognitive flexibility tasks.
In the current study, we have compared monolingual and bilingual children with and with-
out autism in terms of their performance in verbal dual-task paradigms, while also controlling
for the children’s EF, autism symptomatology, IQ, SES, and dual-language experience.
Bilingual autistic children outperformed their monolingual peers in all dual-task paradigms;
yet, the bilingualism effect was stronger in the paradigms in which the secondary, interfering
task drew on lexical semantics rather than syntactic information. Bilingual autistic children’s
performance in the dual-task paradigms was mostly driven by their EF rather than their lan-
guage ability. The overall evidence shows that bilingualism, in the form of enhanced language
experience, compensates for reduced cognitive flexibility in language. This particular finding
is of clinical importance, since cognitive flexibility permeates nearly every aspect of our lives
and its disruption in autism results in profound life-altering deficits. An alternative explanation
might be one of cognitive preservation, whereby bilingual autistic children may possess greater
cognitive capacities in the face of increased demands, which could have helped them preserve
both tasks in memory, allowing for word retention to occur for more time than it otherwise would
be able to do (see Bak et al., 2014). However, studies of aging in autism as well as longitudinal
studies are needed to further explore this possibility.
Although the results of the current study provide evidence for a positive effect of bilingual-
ism on autistic children’s cognitive flexibility skills, our findings should be replicated by
follow-up studies, given the following limitations. First, despite IQ scores not affecting autistic
children’s performance in the dual-task paradigms, no direct comparisons were run between
TD and autistic groups on IQ, which raises the possibility that the dual-task performance dif-
ferences between groups might have been affected by IQ differences. Furthermore, the ADI-R
tool may have lacked the nuance of specific inflexible behaviors, including rigidity, persever-
ative thought, and insistence on sameness found in autistic individuals, thus, failing to corre-
late with the dual-task outcomes of the current study. Other ratings of rigidity in autistic
individuals’ daily life may likely be better predictors of the nuances involved in cognitive flex-
ibility measures. The way that bilingualism effects on cognitive flexibility skills may transfer or
generalize to the social competence of autistic individuals is an important question that needs
to be explored in future research.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to the children and the children’s parents for their unfailing commitment and
interest in our study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Eleni Peristeri: Conceptualization: Supporting; Data curation: Lead; Funding acquisition:
Lead; Investigation: Lead; Methodology: Lead; Project administration: Equal; Resources:
Neurobiology of Language
580
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Equal; Writing – original draft: Lead; Writing – review & editing: Equal. Margreet Vogelzang:
Formal analysis: Lead; Software: Supporting; Visualization: Lead; Writing – original draft: Equal;
Writing – review & editing: Equal. Ianthi Maria Tsimpli: Conceptualization: Lead; Methodology:
Equal; Supervision: Lead; Writing – review & editing: Lead.
REFERENCES
Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J. M., Seghier, M., Zimine, I., Lee-Jahnke, H.,
Lazeyras, F., & Khateb, A. (2008). Language control and lexical
competition in bilinguals: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral
Cortex, 18(7), 1496–1505. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor
/bhm182, PubMed: 17947346
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M.,
Scifo, P., Keim, R., & Costa, A. (2011). Bilingualism tunes the an-
terior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex,
22(9), 2076–2086. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/ bhr287,
PubMed: 22038906
Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inhibition in autism: Children
with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not
prepotent responses. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 42(6), 1052–1063. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803
-011-1345-3, PubMed: 21830170
Albein-Urios, N., Youssef, G. J., Kirkovski, M., & Enticott, P. G.
(2018). Autism spectrum traits linked with reduced perfor-
mance on self-report behavioural measures of cognitive flexi-
bility. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(7),
2506–2515. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3503-3,
PubMed: 29468575
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (5th ed.). American
Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books
.9780890425596
Andreou, M. (2015). The effects of bilingualism on verbal and non
verbal cognition: The micro- and macro-structure of narratives in
the weak and the dominant language of the bilingual child
[Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Aristotle University of
Thessaloniki, Greece.
Andreou, M., Tsimpli, I. M., Durrleman, S., & Peristeri, E. (2020).
Theory of Mind, executive functions, and syntax in bilingual chil-
dren with autism spectrum disorder. Languages, 5(4), Article 67.
https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040067
Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014).
Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of
Neurology, 75(6), 959–963. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24158,
PubMed: 24890334
Bal, V. H., Wilkinson, E., & Fok, M. (2021). Cognitive profiles of chil-
dren with autism spectrum disorder with parent-reported extra-
ordinary talents and personal strengths. Autism. June 2021 [Epub
ahead of print]. https://doi.org/10.1177/13623613211020618,
PubMed: 34088226
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., &
Clubley, E. (2001). The autism spectrum quotient (AQ):
Evidence from Asperger syndrome/ high functioning autism,
males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17. https://doi
.org/10.1023/A:1005653411471, PubMed: 11439754
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discov-
ery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological),
57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995
.tb02031.x
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004).
Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the
Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. https://doi
.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290, PubMed: 15222822
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in
a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 32(6), 1341–1354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278
-7393.32.6.1341, PubMed: 17087588
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and execu-
tive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolin-
gual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language,
109(2–3), 93–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.001,
PubMed: 18834625
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive
control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures.
Cognition, 112(3), 494–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition
.2009.06.014, PubMed: 19615674
Bos, D. J., Silver, B. M., Barnes, E. D., Ajodan, E. L., Silverman,
M. R., Clark-Whitney, E., Tarpey, T., & Jones, R. M. (2019).
Adolescent-specific motivation deficits in autism versus typical
development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
50(1), 364–372. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04258-9,
PubMed: 31625010
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic
difficulties, and specific language impairment: Can we distinguish
them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine &
Child Neurology, 45(8), 515–524. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469
-8749.2003.tb00951.x, PubMed: 12882530
Bryson, S. E. (1983). Interference effects in autistic children:
Evidence for the comprehension of single stimuli. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 92(2), 250–254. https://doi.org/10.1037
/0021-843X.92.2.250, PubMed: 6863739
Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Branzi, F. M., & Costa, A. (2012).
Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and
executive control: Evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in
Psychology, 2, Article 399. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011
.00399, PubMed: 22275905
Campbell, K., Carpenter, K. L. H., & Hashemi, J. (2017) Computer
vision analysis captures atypical attention in toddlers with au-
tism. Autism, 23(3), 619–628. https://doi.org/10.1177
/1362361318766247, PubMed: 29595333
Chondrogianni, V., Andreou, M., Nerantzini, M., Varlokosta, S., &
Tsimpli, I. M. (2013). The Greek sentence repetition task. COST
Action IS0804.
Christ, S. E., Holt, D. D., White, D. A., & Green, L. (2007).
Inhibitory control in children with autism spectrum disorder.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6),
1155–1165. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0259-y,
PubMed: 17066307
Christ, S. E., Kester, L. E., Bodner, K. E., & Miles, J. H. (2011).
Evidence for selective inhibitory impairment in individuals with
autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 25(6), 690–701.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024256, PubMed: 21728431
Neurobiology of Language
581
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Chrysochoou, E. (2006). Working memory contributions to young
children’s listening comprehension [Unpublished doctoral disser-
tation]. Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
155–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155,
PubMed: 19565683
Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social responsiveness
scale—second edition (SRS-2). Western Psychological Services.
Coutanche, M. N., Thompson-Schill, S. L., & Schultz, R. T. (2011).
Multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data predicts clinical symp-
tom severity. NeuroImage, 57(1), 113–123. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.016, PubMed: 21513803
Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2006). Planning and task manage-
ment in older adults: Cooking breakfast. Memory & Cognition,
34(6), 1236–1249. https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03193268,
PubMed: 17225505
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in
working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning &
Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022
-5371(80)90312-6
D’Cruz, A. M., Ragozzino, M. E., Mosconi, M. W., Shrestha, S.,
Cook, E. H., & Sweeney, J. A. (2013). Reduced behavioral flexi-
bility in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(2),
152–160. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031721, PubMed: 23527643
Deák, G. O., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Cognitive flexibility in young
children: General or task-specific capacity? Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 31–53. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.003, PubMed: 26026421
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song,
Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., Hickie, I., & Guastella, A. J. (2018). Autism
spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function.
Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5), 1198–1204. https://doi.org/10
.1038/mp.2017.75, PubMed: 28439105
de Vries, M., & Geurts, H. M. (2012). Cognitive flexibility in ASD;
task switching with emotional faces. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2558–2568. https://doi.org
/10.1007/s10803-012-1512-1, PubMed: 22456815
Eigsti, I.-M., Marchena, A. D., Schuh, J. M., & Kelley, E. (2011).
Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: A develop-
mental review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(2),
681–691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.001
Eigsti, I.-M., Markoff, K., Helt, M., Rosenthal, M., Troyb, E., & Fein,
D. (2008, May 15–17). Dual task performance in children and
adults with autism [Paper presentation]. 7th Annual International
Meeting for Autism Research (IMFAR), London, United Kingdom.
Farmer, G. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Skylark, W. J. (2017). People
with autism spectrum conditions make more consistent deci-
sions. Psychological Science, 28(8), 1067–1076. https://doi.org
/10.1177/0956797617694867, PubMed: 28635378
Fukumura, K., van Gompel, R., Harley, H., & Pickering, M. J.
(2011). How does similarity-based interference affect the choice
of referring expression? Journal of Memory and Language, 65(3),
331–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.06.001
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez-
Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Ávila, C.
(2010). Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the im-
pact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53(4),
1272–1278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078,
PubMed: 20558314
Garcia-Villamistar, D., & Sala, S. D. (2002). Dual-task performance
in adults with autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 7(1), 63–74.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800143000140, PubMed:
16571527
Georgas, J., Paraskevopoulos, I. N., Besevegis, E., Giannitsas, N.,
& Mylonas, K. (2003). Ε(cid:4)(cid:4)(cid:5)(cid:6)(cid:7)(cid:8)ό WISC-III: Wechsler (cid:8)(cid:4)ί(cid:9)(cid:3)(cid:8)ες
(cid:6)ο(cid:5)μο(cid:10)ύ(cid:6)(cid:5)ς (cid:1)(cid:7)(cid:3) π(cid:3)(cid:7)(cid:11)(cid:7)ά [Greek WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence
scale for children]. Ellinika Grammata.
Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B. A., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox
of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
13(2), 74–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006,
PubMed: 19138551
Geurts, H. M., van den Bergh, S. F. W. M., & Ruzzano, L.
(2014). Prepotent response inhibition and interference control
in autism spectrum disorders: Two meta-analyses. Autism
Research, 7(4), 407–420. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1369,
PubMed: 24596300
Ghahremani, A., Rastogi, A., & Lam, S. (2015). The role of right an-
terior insula and salience processing in inhibitory control. Journal
of Neuroscience, 35(8), 3291–3292. https://doi.org/10.1523
/JNEUROSCI.5239-14.2015, PubMed: 25716829
Gökçen, E., Petrides, K. V., Hudry, K., Frederickson, N., & Smillie,
L. D. (2014). Sub-threshold autism traits: The role of trait emo-
tional intelligence and cognitive flexibility. British Journal of
Psychology, 105(2), 187–199. https://doi.org/10.1111/ bjop
.12033, PubMed: 24754807
Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M.,
Astor, B. C., Denckla, M. B., & Landa, R. J. (2005). Subtle exec-
utive impairments in children with autism and children with
ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3),
279–293. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-3291-4, PubMed:
16119469
Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. S. (2019). Can bilingualism
mitigate set-shifting difficulties in children with autism spectrum
disorders? Child Development, 90(4), 1043–1060. https://doi.org
/10.1111/cdev.12979, PubMed: 29111575
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilin-
guals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911
.2013.796377, PubMed: 25077013
Green, D. W., & Li, W. (2014). A control process model of
code-switching. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(4),
499–511. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.882515
Green, S. A., Hernandez, L., Tottenham, N., Krasileva, K.,
Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2015). Neurobiology of sen-
sory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders.
JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), 778–786. https://doi.org/10.1001
/jamapsychiatry.2015.0737, PubMed: 26061819
Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2007). Proactive interference in a
semantic short-term memory deficit: Role of semantic and pho-
nological relatedness. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of
the Nervous System and Behavior, 43(1), 112–123. https://doi
.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70449-0, PubMed: 17334211
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-
focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. https://doi
.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0, PubMed: 16450045
Hofweber, J., Marinis, T., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2021). How different
code-switching types modulate bilinguals’ executive functions:
A dual control mode perspective. Bilingualism: Language and
C o g n i t i o n , 2 3 ( 4 ) , 9 0 9 – 9 2 5 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 7
/S1366728919000804
Hope, R. M. (2013). Rmisc: Ryan Miscellaneous [Software]. https://
CRAN.R-project.org/package=Rmisc
Howard, P. L., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2017). Investigating
the use of world knowledge during on-line comprehension in
adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
Neurobiology of Language
582
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Developmental Disorders, 47(7), 2039–2053. https://doi.org/10
.1007/s10803-017-3129-x, PubMed: 28432451
Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The autism diagnostic observation
schedule, module 4: Revised algorithm and standardized severity
scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(8),
1996–2012. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2080-3,
PubMed: 24590409
Iarocci, G., Hutchison, S. M., & O’Toole, G. (2017). Second lan-
guage exposure, functional communication, and executive func-
tion in children with and without autism spectrum disorder
(ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(6),
1818–1829. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3103-7,
PubMed: 28342166
Ionescu, T. (2012). Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility.
New Ideas in Psychology, 30(2), 190–200. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.11.001
Ivanova, M. V., & Hallowell, B. (2014). A new modified listening
span task to enhance validity of working memory assessment for
people with and without aphasia. Journal of Communication
Disorders, 52, 78–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.06
.001, PubMed: 24986153
Jasinska, K. K., & Petitto, L. A. (2013). How age of bilingual expo-
sure can change the neural systems for language in the develop-
ing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation
of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 87–101. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.005, PubMed: 23974273
Kenworthy, L., Anthony, L. G., Naiman, D. Q., Cannon, L., Wills,
M. C., Luong-Tran, C., Werner, M. A., Alexander, K. C., Strang, J.,
Bal, E., Sokoloff, J. L., & Wallace, G. L. (2014). Randomized con-
trolled effectiveness trial of executive function intervention for
children on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 55(4), 374–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp
.12161, PubMed: 24256459
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of
language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic sub-
groups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 287–308.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000058, PubMed:
16703115
Koch, I., Prinz, W., & Allport, A. (2005). Involuntary retrieval in
alphabet-arithmetic tasks: Task-mixing and task-switching costs.
Psychological Research, 69(4), 252–261. https://doi.org/10.1007
/s00426-004-0180-y, PubMed: 15750868
Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility
with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and
do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4),
855–875. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.4.855,
PubMed: 7643052
Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Osada, H., Takeda, T., & Kurita, H.
(2007). Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome
and high-f unctioning autism. Psychiatry and Clinical
Neurosciences, 61(1), 99–104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440
-1819.2007.01617.x, PubMed: 17239046
Kunda, M., & Goel, A. K. (2011). Thinking in pictures as a cognitive
account of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 41(9), 1157–1177. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803
-010-1137-1, PubMed: 21103918
Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, and ex-
ecutive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of
performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
35(5), 557–573. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1,
PubMed: 16211332
Leung, R. C., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2014). Brief report: Cognitive
flexibility in autism spectrum disorders: A quantitative review.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(10),
2628–2645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2136-4,
PubMed: 24862496
Lewis, R. L., & Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of
sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive
S c i e n c e , 2 9 ( 3 ) , 3 7 5 – 4 1 9 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 2 0 7
/s15516709cog0000_25, PubMed: 21702779
Li, H., Oi, M., Gondo, K., & Matsui, T. (2017). How does being
bilingual influence children with autism in the aspect of execu-
tive functions and social and communication competence?
Journal of Brain Science, 47, 21–49.
Low, J., & Simpson, S. (2012). Effects of labeling on preschoolers’
explicit false belief performance: Outcomes of cognitive flexibil-
ity or inhibitory control? Child Development, 83(3), 1072–1084.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01738.x, PubMed:
22364371
Marinis, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2015). Sentence repetition. In S.
Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing multilingual
children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment
(pp. 95–122). Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/10.21832
/9781783093137-007
McElree, B. (2000). Sentence comprehension is mediated by
content-addressable memory. Journal of Psycholinguistic
R e s e a r c h , 2 9 ( 2 ) , 1 1 1 – 1 2 3 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 2 3
/A:1005184709695, PubMed: 10709178
Meilleur, A. A. S., & Fombonne, E. (2009). Regression of language
and non-language skills in pervasive developmental disorders.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 53(2), 115–124.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01134.x, PubMed:
19054269
Memari, A. H., Ziaee, V., Shayestehfar, M., Ghanouni, P.,
Mansournia, M. A., & Moshayedi, P. (2013). Cognitive flexibility
impairments in children with autism spectrum disorders: Links to
age, gender and child outcomes. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 34(10), 3218–3225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd
.2013.06.033, PubMed: 23886763
Moriguchi, Y., & Lertladaluck, K. (2020). Bilingual effects on cog-
nitive shifting and prefrontal activations in young children.
International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(4), 729–739. https://doi
.org/10.1177/1367006919880274
Mostert-Kerckhoffs, M. A. L., Staal, W. G., Houben, R. H., & de
Jonge, M. (2015). Stop and change: Inhibition and flexibility
skills are related to repetitive behavior in children and young
adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3148–3158. https://doi.org
/10.1007/s10803-015-2473-y, PubMed: 26043846
Muth, A., Hönekopp, J., & Falter, C. M. (2014). Visuo-spatial per-
formance in autism: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 44(12), 3245–3263. https://doi.org
/10.1007/s10803-014-2188-5, PubMed: 25022252
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global
features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3),
353–383. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive
function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals:
Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology
& Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081–1105. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x, PubMed:
1787138
Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Mason, L., Alvarado, K., & Zimiga,
B. (2018). Bilingual advantages in inhibition or selective
Neurobiology of Language
583
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
attention: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article
1409. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01409, PubMed:
30158887
Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006).
Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism
spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its relation-
ship to theory of mind and executive control. Development
and Psychopathology, 18(1), 77–98. https://doi.org/10.1017
/S0954579406060056, PubMed: 16478553
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Andreou, M., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2020). The
impact of bilingualism on the narrative ability and the executive
functions of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Communication Disorders, 85, Article 105999. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.105999, PubMed: 32413648
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Vogelzang, M., Tsimpli, I. M., &
Durrleman, S. (2021). The cognitive benefits of bilingualism in
autism spectrum disorder: Is theory of mind boosted and by
which underlying factors? Autism Research, 14(8), 1695–1709.
https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2542, PubMed: 34008896
Peristeri, E., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2010). An interference method to in-
vestigate working memory in Greek-speaking patients with
Broca’s aphasia. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 23,
213–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.034
Pivneva, I., Palmer, C., & Titone, D. (2012). Inhibitory control and
L2 proficiency modulate bilingual language production:
Evidence from spontaneous monologue and dialogue speech.
Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 57. https://doi.org/10.3389
/fpsyg.2012.00057, PubMed: 22438846
Poljac, E., & Bekkering, H. (2012). A review of intentional and
cognitive control in autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article
436. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00436, PubMed:
23112781
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting.
In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and
Performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–556).
Lawrence Erlbaum.
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical
computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna,
Austria. https://www.R-project.org/
Ratto, A. B., Potvin, D., Pallathra, A. A., Saldana, L., & Kenworthy,
L. (2020). Parents report fewer executive functioning problems
and repetitive behaviors in young dual-language speakers with
autism. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and
Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 26(7),
917–933. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2020.1733512,
PubMed: 32157943
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1992). Manual for Raven’s
progressive matrices. Oxford Psychologists Press.
Remington, A., & Fairnie, J. (2017). A sound advantage: Increased
auditory capacity in autism. Cognition, 166, 459–465. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.002, PubMed: 28476357
Renfrew, C. (1997). Word finding vocabulary test (The Renfrew
Language Scales). Winslow Press.
Ridley, N. J., Homewood, J., & Walters, J. (2011). Cerebellar dys-
function, cognitive flexibility and autistic traits in a non-clinical
sample. Autism, 15, 728–745. https://doi.org/10.1177
/1362361310395956, PubMed: 21690210
Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). ADI-R: The Autism
diagnostic interview—revised. Western Psychological Services.
Saldaña, D., & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make bridg-
ing inferences from world knowledge? Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 96(4), 310–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.jecp.2006.11.002, PubMed: 17196973
Sanderson, C., & Allen, M. L. (2013). The specificity of inhibitory
impairments in autism and their relation to ADHD-type symp-
toms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(5),
1065–1079. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1650-5,
PubMed: 23054200
Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, S., & Murphy,
D. G. (2006). Neural correlates of executive function in autistic
spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 59(1), 7–16. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.007, PubMed: 16140278
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime
user’s guide. Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of
language fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4). The
Psychological Corporation.
Sharaan, S., Fletcher-Watson, S., & MacPherson, S. E. (2020). The
impact of bilingualism on the executive functions of autistic chil-
dren: A study of English–Arabic children. Autism Research, 14(3),
533–544. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2439, PubMed: 33241665
Siegel, D. J., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1996). Wechsler IQ
profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 26(4), 389–406. https://doi.org/10
.1007/BF02172825, PubMed: 8863091
Solomon, M., Hogeveen, J., Libero, L., & Nordahl, C. W. (2017). An
altered scaffold for information processing: Cognitive control de-
velopment in adolescents with autism. Biological Psychiatry:
Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(6), 464–475.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.002, PubMed: 28924621
South, M., & Rodgers, J. (2017). Sensory, emotional and cognitive
contributions to anxiety in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 11, Article 20. https://doi.org/10.3389
/fnhum.2017.00020, PubMed: 28174531
Stocco, A., Yamasaki, B., Natalenko, R., & Prat, C. S. (2014).
Bilingual brain training: A neurobiological framework of how bi-
lingual experience improves executive function. International
Journal of Bilingualism, 18(1), 67–92. https://doi.org/10.1177
/1367006912456617
Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, U. (2011). Are the neural corre-
lates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-
analysis of two response inhibition tasks. NeuroImage, 56(3),
1655–1665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.070,
PubMed: 21376819
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2016). Risk factors associated with language
in autism spectrum disorder: Clues to underlying mechanisms.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR,
59(1), 143–154. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-15
-0146, PubMed: 26502110
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T.,
Anderson, M., & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study
of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 1–21.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02206853, PubMed: 2139024
Taylor, M. J., Donner, E. J., & Pang, E. W. (2012). fMRI and MEG in
the study of typical and atypical cognitive Development. Clinical
Neurophysiology, 42(1–2), 19–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neucli.2011.08.002, PubMed: 22200338
Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerreat, L. M., & Burak, A. L. (1994).
Object-based and environment-based inhibition of return of visual
attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception & Performance, 20(3), 478–499. https://doi.org/10
.1037/0096-1523.20.3.478, PubMed: 8027711
Turner, M. A. (1997). Towards an executive dysfunction account
of repetitive behavior in autism. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an
executive disorder (pp. 57–100). Oxford University Press.
Neurobiology of Language
584
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
.
/
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Cognitive flexibility in bilingual autistic children
Turner, M. A. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behavior in autism: A
review of psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 40(6), 839–849. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469
-7610.00502, PubMed: 10509879
Uddin, L. Q., Menon, V., Young, C. B., Ryali, S., Chen, T.,
Khouzam, A., Minshew, N. J., & Hardan, A. Y. (2011).
Multivariate searchlight classification of structural magnetic res-
onance imaging in children and adolescents with autism.
Biological Psychiatry, 70(9), 833–841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.biopsych.2011.07.014, PubMed: 21890111
van den Noort, M., Struys, E., Bosch, P., Jaswetz, L., Perriard, B.,
Yeo, S., Barisch, P., Vermeire, K., Lee, S. H., & Lim, S. (2019).
Does the bilingual advantage in cognitive control exist and if
so, what are its modulating factors? A systematic review.
Behavioral Sciences, 9(3), Article 27. https://doi.org/10.3390
/bs9030027
Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task
switching: Interplay of reconfiguration and interference control.
Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 601–626. https://doi.org/10.1037
/a0019791, PubMed: 20565170
Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., &
Noens, I. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disor-
der: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1390–1401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.rasd.2011.01.025
Vogindroukas, I., Protopapas, A., & Sideridis, G. (2009). Experiment
on the expressive vocabulary (Greek version of Renfrew Word
Finding Vocabulary Test). Glafki.
Watson, A., Painter, K., & Bornstein, M. (2001). Longitudinal rela-
tions between 2-year-olds’ language and 4-year-olds’ theory of
mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(4), 449–457.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0204_5
Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for
children—third edition: Manual [Australian adaptation].
Psychological Corporation.
Yerys, B. E., Antezana, L., Weinblatt, R., Jankowski, K. F., Strang, J.,
Vaidya C. J., Schultz, R. T., Gaillard, W. D., & Kenworthy, L.
(2015). Neural correlates of set-shifting in children with autism.
Autism Research, 8(4), 386–397. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur
.1454, PubMed: 25599972
Yu, B. (2016). Bilingualism as conceptualized and bilingualism
as lived: A critical examination of the monolingual socializa-
tion of a child with autism in a bilingual family. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 424–435.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2625-0, PubMed:
26519327
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
e
d
u
n
o
/
l
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
/
/
/
2
4
5
5
8
1
9
7
9
6
8
4
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
5
5
p
d
/
.
l
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Neurobiology of Language
585