ARTICLE DE RECHERCHE

ARTICLE DE RECHERCHE

Combining Concepts Across Categorical
Domains: A Linking Role of the Precuneus

Giuseppe Rabini

, Silvia Ubaldi

, and Scott Fairhall

Centre for Mind/ Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Trento, Italy

un accès ouvert

journal

Mots clés: IRMf, combinatorial semantics, langue, phrases, precuneus, category selectivity

ABSTRAIT

The human capacity for semantic knowledge entails not only the representation of single
concepts but also the capacity to combine these concepts into the increasingly complex ideas
that underlie human thought. This process involves not only the combination of concepts
from within the same semantic category but also frequently the conceptual combination
across semantic domains. In this fMRI study (N = 24) we investigate the cortical mechanisms
underlying our ability to combine concepts across different semantic domains. Using five
different semantic domains (Personnes, Places, Food, Objects, and Animals), we present
sentences depicting concepts drawn from a single semantic domain as well as sentences that
combine concepts from two of these domains. Contrasting single-category and combined-
category sentences reveals that the precuneus is more active when concepts from different
domains have to be combined. En même temps, we observe that distributed category
selectivity representations persist when higher-order meaning involves the combination of
categories and that this category-selective response is captured by the combination of the
single categories composing the sentence. Collectively, these results suggest that the precuneus
plays a role in the combination of concepts across different semantic domains, potentiellement
functioning to link together category-selective representations distributed across the cortex.

Citation: Rabini, G., Ubaldi, S., &
Fairhall, S. (2021). Combining concepts
across categorical domains: A linking
role of the precuneus. Neurobiology of
Language, 2(3), 354–371. https://doi.org
/10.1162/nol_a_00039

EST CE QUE JE:
https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00039

Informations complémentaires:
https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00039

Reçu: 13 Décembre 2020
Accepté: 8 Avril 2021

Intérêts concurrents: Les auteurs ont
a déclaré qu'aucun intérêt concurrent
exister.

INTRODUCTION

Auteur correspondant:
Scott Fairhall
scott.fairhall@unitn.it

Éditeur de manipulation:
Yanchao Bi

droits d'auteur: © 2021
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Publié sous Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International
(CC PAR 4.0) Licence

La presse du MIT

The human capacity for semantic knowledge involves not only the representation of single
concepts but also the capacity to combine these concepts into the increasingly complex ideas
that underlie human thought. A wealth of research on single concepts has shown that the hu-
man brain implements semantic representation over a complex system involving regions that
are sensitive to specific semantic classes of objects, such as people, food, or places, in addition
to regions that are generally more active for semantically richer stimuli, regardless of category.
Accordingly, the functioning of the semantic system is reflected in a dynamic interplay be-
tween domain-specific and domain-general representations (Binder, 2016; Binder et al.,
2009; Binder & Desai, 2011; Chen et al., 2017; Kiefer & Pulvermüller, 2012; Martine, 2016;
Patterson et al., 2007). The distributed representation of semantic knowledge in the brain po-
tentially indicates a fundamental organisational principle, whereby basic object-related
knowledge extends to complex, multi-faceted units of meaning.

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

e
d
toi
n
o

/

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

2
3
3
5
4
1
9
2
8
8
9
3
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
3
9
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Combining concepts across categorical domains

The combination of concepts into higher-order representations not only involves the linking
of concepts from within the same semantic domain but also frequently entails the flexible
association of concepts spanning different domains. Par exemple, reading about a boy playing
with his dog in the garden requires the system to link concepts from distinct conceptual domains
(c'est à dire., people, animals, and places) to build a distinct and coherent representation. À ce jour, comment-
jamais, it is still unclear how category-selective brain regions interact when concepts from different
domains have to be combined into higher-order semantic units. En particulier, there remain two
open questions: (1) Do specific brain regions coordinate information contained in category-
selective cortical regions? Et (2) do complex ideas that combine category information from
multiple domains continue to utilise those domain-selective representations of single concepts,
or do derived multi-categorical concepts rely more on domain-general semantic mechanisms?

The general semantic processing of semantically richer stimuli recruits a left-lateralised cor-
tical network encompassing several heteromodal associative regions (the angular gyrus (AG),
lateral temporal cortex, ventral temporal cortex, dorso-medial and ventro-medial prefrontal cor-
tex, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and the precuneus) (Binder et al., 2009). Although modality-
specific activations emerge during modality-specific conceptual processing—for example,
perceptual/motor-related concepts activate the respective perceptual/motor brain areas (voir
Binder & Desai, 2011; Borghesani & Piazza, 2017; Kiefer & Pulvermüller, 2012)—the general
semantic network appears to be clearly distinguished from primary sensory and motor cortices
(Binder et al., 2009). While a strong embodied view of cognition states that conceptual knowl-
edge emerges exclusively from sensory and action/motor experience and is therefore grounded
and represented in the related cortical regions (Barsalou, 2010; Gallese, 2005; see also Mahon &
Caramazza, 2008, for a critical perspective), a softer version (“embodied abstraction”; Binder &
Desai, 2011) gives an alternative view. Spécifiquement, it states that different levels of abstraction starting
from sensory, moteur, and emotional experiences model our conceptual representation. Higher-level
concepts can be abstracted from the primary sensory meaning, and different levels of abstraction
can be selectively activated depending on several factors, such as context and task demand. Dans
this view, heteromodal cortices might be involved in the representation of more abstract—high-
level—concepts, which are not necessarily and directly linked to sensorimotor experience.

The position has found complementary support in neurocomputational models of neural
semantic representation (Chen et al., 2017) and in recent theoretical proposals (Lambon
Ralph et al., 2016). In the controlled semantic cognition framework, the representational sub-
system has been described as “the hub-and-spoke model” (Patterson et al., 2007; Rogers et al.,
2004). This model predicts that concepts emerge from both verbal and nonverbal experience
and that modality-selective cortices, distributed across the whole brain, represent this specific
information (“the spokes”). En outre, a unique transmodal core region (“the hub”), identi-
fied in the ventro-lateral anterior temporal lobe (ATL) (see also Visser et al., 2010), would be
engaged in modality-invariant representations, so that this region can represent object-
concepts in multimodal, more abstract, global units. Recent advances further elaborated on
this proposal by advocating a graded specialisation within the ATL, based on the different con-
nectivity patterns of its subparts (Lambon Ralph et al., 2016; Rice et al., 2015).

The presence of distributed cortical representations of semantic information, linked to the
notion of “spokes,” as previously defined, grounds its evidence in longstanding research
focusing on object–concept representation in the brain and how the semantic category to which
the object belongs can affect its cortical representation. There is now compelling evidence that
conceptual knowledge can be selectively impaired following focal brain lesions (Capitani
et coll., 2003; Caramazza & Mahon, 2003; Miceli et al., 2000; Warrington & Shallice, 1984)
and that the neural responses of discrete brain regions are more sensitive to specific semantic

Neurobiology of Language

355

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

e
d
toi
n
o

/

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

2
3
3
5
4
1
9
2
8
8
9
3
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
3
9
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Combining concepts across categorical domains

catégories (Caramazza & Shelton, 1998; Kuhnke et al., 2020; Mahon et al., 2009; Mahon &
Caramazza, 2011; Martine, 2016; Noppeney et al., 2006). This cortical category selectivity, à
least for people and places, has been shown to persist, both considering specific, unique en-
tities (people: “Leonardo DiCaprio”; lieux: “The colosseum”; Fairhall et al., 2014) and gen-
eral semantic knowledge (“kind of”—people: “lawyer”; lieux: “courthouse”; Fairhall &
Caramazza, 2013un), thus demonstrating an additional category-specific sensitivity for concepts
abstracted from their principal sensorimotor counterparts. Like object category, accessed con-
tent also selectively recruits specialised conceptual representations. When atypical informa-
tion is accessed about people or food, such as the geographical provenance of the item,
regions classically associated with place selectivity are recruited (Fairhall, 2020).

Semantic category sensitivity in the human brain has also been highlighted through data-
driven approaches, by assessing brain responses to naturally spoken narrative stories (Deniz
et coll., 2019; Huth et al., 2016), further mapping single word-related semantic selectivity on the
whole brain surface (see also Pereira et al., 2018).

While crucial for the understanding of conceptual knowledge, single concepts do not
capture the complexities of the semantic contents we manage in everyday life (Frankland &
Vert, 2020). The capacity to flexibly combine multiple concepts from distinct categories
into unitary representations is a fundamental feature of human semantic cognition. The present
study focuses on the cortical mechanism underlying the formation of higher-order meaning
that necessitates the combination of concepts belonging to different semantic domains, spe-
cifically those concepts that are frequently represented across distinct category-selective brain
régions. To this end, we implemented an event-related fMRI paradigm presenting written sen-
tences regarding a single semantic category (c'est à dire., Personnes, Places, Food, Objects, and Animals)
or sentences encompassing two distinct conceptual domains (par exemple., People and Food). Our ob-
jective was to answer two main questions: (1) Are brain regions differentially activated when
information from different semantic categories has to be combined? (2) Do higher-order se-
mantic representations that combine concepts across category continue to rely on category-
specific representations, or do these more derived combinatorial semantic meanings rely more
heavily on general semantic representations?

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Twenty-eight native Italian speakers were recruited for the study. Three participants were ex-
cluded due to head motion exceeding 2 mm during scanning. Another participant did not
perform the entire protocol and therefore was excluded. Ainsi, the final sample consisted of
24 participants (12 males, mean age 24.9 années). Before entering the scanner, participants
underwent a medical interview with a neurologist, and all of them reported no history of neu-
rological or psychiatric disease. Participants gave informed consent and were compensated
for participation (15 A/ hour). The study was conducted in line with the declaration of
Helsinki (1964, amended in 2013) and was approved by the Ethical Committee of the
University of Trento.

Experimental Design

Stimuli

The stimuli set was composed of 288 Italian written sentences formed by a subject, a verb, et
a complement. Sentences were of three types. (je) Single-category sentences (mean number of

Neurobiology of Language

356

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

e
d
toi
n
o

/

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

2
3
3
5
4
1
9
2
8
8
9
3
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
3
9
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Combining concepts across categorical domains

words: 5.79 (0.67)): sentences in which both the subject and the complement belong to the
same semantic category [Personnes, Places, Food, Objects, Animals]. (ii) Combined-category sen-
tences (mean number of words: 5.59 (0.60)): sentences in which the subject and the comple-
ment belong to different semantic categories. Il y avait 10 combinations of the five main
conceptual categories [Personnes & Places, Personnes & Food, Personnes & Objects, Personnes &
Animals, Places & Food, Places & Objects, Places & Animals, Food & Objects, Food &
Animals, Objects & Animals]. (iii) Bizarre sentences (mean number of words: 5.6 (0.68)): gram-
matically correct sentences but with an anomalous semantic meaning (par exemple., “The window was
inside the tomato”). Representative sentences (and the English translation) for each sentence
type are presented in Table 1 (the full list can be found in Supplementary Table S3; supporting
information can be found online at https://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/suppl/10.1162/nol_a
_00039). Il y avait au total 16 sentences for each of the single- and combined-category
conditions, et 48 sentences for the bizarre condition.

To assess the imageability of different sentences, 10 participants (who did not take part in
the main experiment) rated the imageability of each sentence on a 5-point Likert scale.
Globally, the sentences were perceived as highly imaginable (mean = 4.17). Imageability rat-
ings were averaged within each condition and statistical analysis performed across partici-
pants. A paired sample t test between the average ratings for single-category (4.105) et
combined-category sentences (4.197), revealed a subtle difference (0.092, t(9) = 2.02, p =
0.0371). Comparatively, variation across sentence types was more pronounced over condi-
tions within the 5 single-category conditions (range: 3.969–4.375) and within the 10
combined-category conditions (range: 3.769–4.456).

Sentences were matched on the proportion of action to state verbs in the single (71%) et
combined category sentences (80%, z = 1.6, p = 0.12; normal approximation to the binomial).
We additionally considered the sociality of verbs. Verbal phrases were presented in isolation
and labelled by two raters (S.U. and G.R.) according to the criteria: “likely to relate to an in-
teraction between two individuals.” Social verbs occurred more frequently in single-category
phrases (15%) compared to combined-category sentences (5.6%). This was primarily driven
by an over-representation of social verbs in the person–person condition, where they were
present in 12 out of 16 phrases.

fMRI experimental task

The fMRI session was divided into six experimental runs. In each run, there were 8 trials for
each single- and combined-category sentence (5 single and 10 combined), 24 trials with bi-
zarre sentences, et un supplémentaire 24 fixation-cross null-events. In an event-related paradigm,
sentences were pseudo-randomized across runs and randomly interleaved with fixation cross
events (sentences were repeated three times across the experiment). Using MATLAB (www
.mathworks.com) and Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (psychtoolbox.org), each sentence
was presented in black font against a gray background and presented consecutively in three
fragments (sujet, verb, complement). Each trial lasted 2.5 s. Each sentence-fragment was
presented consecutively on the centre of the screen for 400 ms. After the 1.2 s of stimulus
presentation, a black fixation cross appeared in the centre of the screen for the remainder
du procès. The participant’s task was to indicate via button-press if the sentence was seman-
tically meaningful (index finger) or was “bizarre” (right middle). Reaction times (RTs) were cal-
culated from the onset of the last sentence fragment, and responses faster than 400 ms or
slower than 1,700 ms were excluded. RT data for one participant was unavailable due to mea-
surement error.

Neurobiology of Language

357

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

e
d
toi
n
o

/

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

2
3
3
5
4
1
9
2
8
8
9
3
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
3
9
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Combining concepts across categorical domains

Tableau 1.

Representative stimuli

Single

Sentence category
Personnes

Representative sentence

I poliziotti arrestano i ladri

Places

Food

The cops arrest the thieves

Le fabbriche sono fuori dalle cittá

Factories are outside cities

I bigné sono ripieni di cioccolata

Cream puffs are filled with chocolate

Objects

Le lampade illuminano i tavoli

Animals

I cani rincorrono le lepri

Lamps illuminate tables

Dogs chase hares

Combined

Personnes & Places

Gli studenti si trovano all’universitá

Personnes & Food

Le mamme cuociono le crostate

Students are at the university

Mothers bake pies

Personnes & Objects

I camerieri sistemano le forchette

Personnes & Animals

I cacciatori sparano ai cervi

Waiters arrange forks

Hunters shoot deer

Places & Food

Le pere crescono nel frutteto

Pears grow in the orchard

Places & Objects

Le forbici si vendono al supermercato

Scissors are sold at the supermarket

Places & Animals

I delfini saltano nel mare

Dolphins jump in the sea

Food & Objects

I caffé si bevono nelle tazzine

Coffees are drunk in cups

Food & Animals

I gatti bevono sempre il latte

Objects & Animals

I topi evitano le trappole

Cats always drink milk

Mice avoid traps

La finestra era dentro al pomodoro

Window was inside the tomato

Bizarre

Neurobiology of Language

358

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

e
d
toi
n
o

/

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

/

/

/

2
3
3
5
4
1
9
2
8
8
9
3
n
o
_
un
_
0
0
0
3
9
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Combining concepts across categorical domains

Post-scanner test

After the fMRI session, participants were again presented the meaningful sentences they had
read in the fMRI session. In the task, a part of the sentence (subject/complement) was missing,
and participants were instructed to complete the missing part.

MRI Scanning Parameters

00

Functional and structural data were collected with a Prisma 3T scanner (Siemens AG,
Erlangen, Allemagne) at the Centre for Mind/ Brain Sciences (CIMeC) of the University of
Trento. Participants lay in the scanner and viewed the visual stimuli through a mirror system
connecté à un 42
, MR-compatible Nordic NeuroLab LCD monitor positioned at the back of
the magnet bore. Data collection was performed using a 64-channel head coil. Functional
images were acquired using echo planar imaging (EPI) T2*-weighted scans. Acquisition
parameters were: repetition time (TR) de 2 s, an echo time (TE) de 28 ms, a flip angle of 75°,
a field of view (FoV) de 100 mm, and a matrix size of 100 × 100. Total functional acquisition
consisted of 1,266 volumes for the six experimental runs, each of 78 tranches axiales (lequel
covered the whole brain) with a thickness of 2 mm and a gap of 2 mm, AC/PC aligned.
High-resolution (1 × 1 × 1 × mm) T1-weighted MPRAGE sequences were also collected (sagittal
slice orientation, centric phase encoding, image matrix = 288 × 288, FoV = 288 mm, 208 slices
with 1-mm thickness, TR = 2,290, LE = 2.74, inversion time (TI) = 950 ms, 12° flip angle).

fMRI Data Analysis

Data were analysed and preprocessed with SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Le
first four volumes of each run were dummy scans. All images were corrected for head move-
ment. Functional images were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) T1
espace, resampled to a voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm and spatially smoothed with 6-mm
FWHM kernel. Subject-specific parameter estimates (β weights) for each of the 16 conditions
(see Experimental Design section for details) were derived through a general linear model
(GLM) and a more lenient implicit mask for inclusion in the GLM (0.1 instead of the SPM de-
fault of 0.8). The control condition with a fixation cross formed the implicit baseline. The six
head-motion parameters were included as additional regressors of no interest.

Region of Interest Selection

Region of interest (ROI) analysis was performed within category-selective ROIs defined using
an Omnibus ANOVA to highlight cortical regions showing a differential response across
categories for the single category sentences only. ROIs were defined as the intersection
between a sphere of 5-mm radius around the group peak coordinates, and the activation map
for the Omnibus ANOVA thresholded at p < 0.001. The location of ROIs is indicated in Supplementary Table S1. Voxel: A three-dimensional pixel produced by volumetric imaging techniques such as fMRI. Region of interest (ROI) analysis: As an alternative to investigating experimental voxels over the tens of thousands of voxels in the brain, ROI analysis is a more acute and focused measure that designates a specific region to investigate and in which to perform analysis. Additional Voxel-Wise Multivariate Pattern Analysis Multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA): An information-based measure that, as applied to brain imaging techniques, tests whether the spatial pattern of brain activation reliably distinguishes between two cognitive states. A supplementary voxel-wise multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) was performed within a pre- cuneus ROI showing a greater response to combined-category than single-category sentences. In this analysis, the constituent single-category sentences were used to predict patterns pro- duced by combined-category sentences. Specifically, correlation based MVPA analysis was performed between pairs of combined category sentences (e.g., (A) people and food sentences versus (B) place and object sentences) using the summed pattern of responses of the relevant Neurobiology of Language 359 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains single category sentences (e.g., (C) people+food single category sentences and (D) place+ object single-category sentences). To assess whether category information present for single- category sentences persisted in the combined-category sentences, correlations between unlike sentence types (A & D; B & C) were subtracted from like sentence types (A & C; B & D). This process was repeated 45 times for each of the possible pairwise combinations of the ten combined-category conditions. One-sample t tests were performed on the resulting values to allow inference. RESULTS Behavioural Results Reaction times on the meaningfulness judgment did not differ between single-category (mean = 716 ms, SD = 120) and combined-category (mean = 723 ms, SD = 120) sentences (t < 1). A repeated-measure ANOVA revealed that RTs differed among sentence-category for single- category sentences (F(4, 92) = 17.2, p < 0.001), and combined-category sentences (F(5.3, 121) = 10.5, p < 0.001; Greenhouse-Geisser corrected). In this way, RT-related effects will not influence comparisons between single-category and combined-category sentences but may influence category-selective effects. This will be further discussed in the relevant sections. Task compliance was high, with meaningful sentences being judged meaningful 90.5% of the time and bizarre sentences identified as bizarre 84.2% (16.51) of the time. In the post- scanner test, participants were able to provide the missing sentence fragment with a high de- gree of accuracy (mean = 68.56%, SD = 15), further indicating a high level of engagement in the scanner task. Combination of Concepts Across Semantic Domains Our first goal was to determine which brain regions may coordinate the combination of con- cepts across different semantic domains. To identify brain regions showing an increased fMRI response when conceptual domains are combined, we compared sentences presenting a com- bination of conceptual categories (e.g., Places & Animals: “The dog is in the kitchen”) to sen- tences involving a single conceptual domain (e.g., Animals: “The cat is next to the dog”). The weighted contrast [Combined-Category sentences > Single-Category sentences] iden-
tified a significant cluster in the precuneus (Extent 1,214 voxels, p < 0.001fwe-cluster, peak = [−4 −50 28], Figure 1), indicating that the precuneus plays a role in the combination of con- cepts across categorical domains. To exclude the possibility that a single category was driving this effect, we repeated the analysis iteratively, excluding one category from both the single- category and combined-category sentences. The increased response in the precuneus for sen- tences that combine information across categories persisted when removing animals (Extent: 1,189 voxels, p < 0.001fwe-cluster, peak = [2 −54 18]), people (Extent: 210 voxels, p = 0.002fwe-cluster, peak = [−6 −48 28]), places (Extent: 415 voxels, p < 0.001fwe-cluster, peak = [−12 −56 20]), food (Extent: 219 voxels, p = 0.002fwe-cluster, peak = [10 −54 40]) and objects (Extent: 1,525 voxels, p < 0.001fwe-cluster, peak = [−4 −68 32]). This iterative leave-one-category-out process was also used to assess the potential role of imageability on activation within the precuneus by recalculating single-/combined-category differences in imageability based only on the included conditions. On iterations where image- ability was balanced between single-category and combined-category conditions, when re- moving Places (difference = 0.02, t < 1) and Objects (difference = 0.04, t < 1), the greater Neurobiology of Language 360 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains Figure 1. Selective activation related to combinations of semantic concepts compared to concepts related to a single semantic category. The whole-brain univariate response for the contrast [Combined-Category sentences > Single-Category sentences] is shown in the figure. A significant
cluster emerged in the precuneus (Extent 1,214 voxels, p < 0.001fwe-cluster, peak = [−4 −50 28]). response in the precuneus for combined-category sentences is seen to persist, indicating that imageability does not drive this effect. We performed a second analysis to assess which brain regions are modulated by sentence imageability over our 15 stimulus types. Using a weighted contrast, we identified voxels where the response amplitude was predicted by the average im- ageability rating of the 15 experimental conditions. While we observed positive evidence for response modulation by imageability in left ATL (−54x −8y −14z; extent: 148 voxels; p = 0.017, cluster-corrected) and vmPFC (−4x 58y −10z; extent: 171 voxels; p = 0.008, cluster- corrected), evidence was not present in the precuneus. Collectively, these results suggest that, in the present paradigm, imageability-related processes are not driving the increased fMRI re- sponse in the precuneus for combined-category compared to single-category sentences. Like nouns, the sociality of verbs may selectively influence cortical activation and is known to in- crease the response in the precuneus (Lin et al., 2019). As social verbs are more prevalent in the single-category condition (see Materials and Methods), such factors cannot account for the increased response for combined-category sentences. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d / . l Category Selective Semantic Representations To characterise category-selective semantic responses, we contrasted each single category with the remaining four single categories (e.g., People > [Places, Food, Objects, Animals],
voir la figure 2). We observed strong category-selective fMRI responses for People in ventro-
medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), precuneus, and bilateral ATL; for Places in bilateral
para-hippocampal place area (APP), bilateral tranverse occipital sulcus (TOS), bilateral retro-
splenial complex (RSC), left middle temporal gyrus (pMTG), left anterior superior temporal
gyrus (aSTG), and left dorsal superior frontal gyrus (dSFG); for Food in the bilateral orbito-
frontal cortex (OFC), left IFG, left preMotor cortex, left and right posterior inferior temporal
gyrus (pITG), left amygdala, left ventro-temporal cortex ( VTC), and right pITG; for Objects in
the left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG); for Animals in the precuneus, right superior frontal gyrus
(SFG), left dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), left inferior parietal sulcus (IPS), and right
temporo-parietal junction (TPJ). The results of the category-selective contrasts are reported in
Tableau 2.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

The Role of Category-Selective Representations in Combined-Category Sentences

Having ascertained the presence of strong category-selective representation for single-category
phrases, we asked whether these regions make a comparable contribution when concepts
are combined across semantic domains. To this end, we assessed whether the activation

Neurobiology of Language

361

Combining concepts across categorical domains

FWE cluster-corrected:
A family-wise error correction to
overcome multiple comparisons
issues in fMRI using Random Field
Theory. This approach considers the
probability of observing a contiguous
cluster of significant voxels,
correcting for multiple comparisons
over the whole brain volume.

Chiffre 2. Category-sensitive activations associated with single-category (Personnes, red; Places, light
blue; Food, vert; Objects, blue; Animals, yellow) phrases. Significant clusters (voxel-wise p < 0.001, uncorrected, FWE cluster-corrected p < 0.05) are shown on the same brain surface map (standard MNI152, MRIcroGL software, https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricrogl). Transparency has been applied to highlight the whole identified clusters. We reported significant clusters for People in the vmPFC, precuneus, and bilateral ATL; for Places in bilateral PPA, bilateral TOS, bi- lateral RSC, left MTG, left aSTG, and left dSFG; for Food in the bilateral OFC, left IFG, left preMotor, left pITG, left amygdala, left VTC, and right pMTG; for Objects in the left MTG; and for Animals in the precuneus, right SFG, left dlPFC, left IPS, and right TPJ. patterns across this network produced by single-category sentences predict the neural re- sponse of the related combined-category sentences, or in other words, whether the response evoked by a sentence combining two categorical representations can be reconstructed from the individual contribution of the two categories. ROIs were defined via an Omnibus ANOVA as isolated brain regions within which activity varies across single-category sentences without introducing bias towards a particular object category. Resulting ROIs were consistent with category-selective regions identified in the pre- ceding section (see Figure S1 and Table S2 for network visualisation and ROI data). As noted earlier, behavioural difference existed between the categories, which may partially account for the effects observed here. To ensure that category-selectivity effects were not due to RT confounds, the persistence of category effects after controlling for RT differences was assessed. Specifically, within ROIs described in the next section, for each subject, the beta responses for each category were regressed against the mean RT for each single-category con- dition. Then, the category-selective contrasts were recomputed on the residuals of this regression (now with the linear effects of RT removed). Category-selective responses persisted in all regions ( p < 0.001) with the exception of left IPS, left lateral preMotor, vmPFC, and right lateral PFC. As we cannot be sure of the veracity of the category-selective nature of the responses in these re- gions, while we report them in Figure 2 and Figure S1, and Table 2 and Table S2, they have not been included in subsequent analyses. To construct the estimate of the combined-category sentences, within each ROI we took the response of single-category sentences (Figure 3A) and averaged them to form a prediction of the amplitude of the regional response for each combined-category sentence (Figure 3B). In this way, the regional response to sentences involving a person and place (Figure 3B, column 1) Neurobiology of Language 362 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains Category People Places Food Objects Animals Table 2. Category-sensitive brain regions Cluster Peak Region Left ATL Precuneus Right ATL vmPFC Left PPA Right RSC Left RSC Left TOS Right PPA Right TOS Left pMTG Left aSTG Left dSFG Left OFC Left pITG Left IFG Right OFC Left preMotor Left VTC Left Amygdala Right pMTG Left pMTG Precuneus Right TPJ Left IPS Right SFG Left dlPFC p(FWE-cor) <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.036 0.002 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.014 <0.001 0.011 0.018 0.014 0.026 <0.001 <0.001 0.001 0.014 0.005 Extent 699 873 297 276 2597 1315 617 872 283 126 222 420 940 495 154 331 161 146 155 136 p(FEW-cor) <0.001 <0.001 0.001 0.002 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.047 0.095 0.141 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.010 0.012 0.623 0.742 0.685 1685 <0.001 705 233 155 185 0.020 0.0.381 0.0386 0.0901 t 9.01 7.48 5.87 5.59 15.16 11.15 11.09 12.59 11.69 8.30 5.04 4.86 4.26 8.47 7.75 7.20 6.80 5.39 5.34 4.28 4.18 4.23 7.36 5.24 4.47 4.47 4.03 p(unc) <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 Note. Significant clusters are reported separately for each semantic category ( p < 0.05, cluster-corrected). x, y, z (MNI) −54, −10, −14 −4, −50, 28 58, −6, −14 −4, 58, −12 −30, −40, −12 10, −54, 10 −10, −54, 8 −38, −84, 32 28, −36, −16 40, −84, 34 −60, −58, −4 −52, −6, −18 −20, 22, 46 −22, 38, −16 −50, −52, −18 −42, 36, 10 22, 38, −18 −44, 6, 24 −36, −28, −24 −20, −2, −24 52, −48, −16 −54, −58, −2 4, −58, 30 48, −62, 20 −26, −56, 38 28, 30, 56 −38, 42, 38 Neurobiology of Language 363 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains Figure 3. Consistency of regional response from single-category to combined-category sentences. Average beta values are shown for each selected ROI and sentence type. Values obtained from the single-category sentences (A) were averaged to create a prediction vector for the response to the sentence that combined those categories (B) for comparison against the veridical response to the combined-category sen- tences (C). is predicted by the combination of the regional response of sentences about people and the regional response of sentences about places (Figure 3A, columns 1 and 2). Overall, the response to the 10 combined-category sentences was closely predicted by their constituents. This is evident in the high congruence between the patterns evident in Figure 3B and Figure 3C. While the combined response to single-category sentences did not predict the response to combined-category sentences in dmPFC (r = −0.008, p = 0.98), pMTG (r = 0.50, p = 0.14), or left TOS (r = 0.59, p = 0.07), the combined response to single-category sentences did predict combined-category sentences in the remaining 13 re- gions. Prediction ability was high, with a mean r value across regions of 0.81 (min, 0.70, max, 0.92, all p values < 0.05). Thus, the combination of single-category sentences explained, on average, 66.0% of the observed response to combined-category sentences in these regions. Importantly, RT differences in the single-category sentences did not predict response pat- terns in the combined-category sentences. Average RTs from the single categories used to form the predicted response (i.e., Figure 3B) were correlated with the RTs from the 10 combined- category sentences. Correlation was non-significant (r = −0.067, p = 0.86), indicating that this analysis is not contaminated by RT confounds. To reconcile whole brain analysis showing a greater response for combined-category than single-category sentences in the precuneus and ROI analysis showing a category-selective effect in the precuneus, we performed two supplementary analyses. Firstly, we assessed the relationship between single-category and combined-category sentences in the category- selective precuneus ROI. The response in the category-selective precuneus ROI for combined-category sentences is super-additive. Specifically, the response for combined- category sentences is greater than the summed response to the composite single-category sen- tences (t(23) = 2.38, p = 0.026). This demonstrates that the category-selective defined Neurobiology of Language 364 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains precuneus ROI exhibited a greater response for combined- than single-category sentences, consistent with the whole brain contrast (c.f. Figure 1). Next, within the precuneus cluster identified in the Combined-Category > Single-Category
whole-brain analysis, we performed a correlation-based MVPA across voxels (see Materials
and Methods). Spécifiquement, the multivoxel pattern for each combined-category condition
(par exemple., People and Places) was reconstructed by combining the single-category sentences of
its constituent categories (par exemple., Single-Category People + Single-Category Places). Alors, recon-
structed patterns were used to distinguish between pairs of combined-category conditions
(par exemple., People and Places vs. Food and Places). The combination of single-category sentences
could accurately predict the combined-category sentences in 36 out of 45 pairwise combina-
tion ( p < 0.05). Of the 9 cases where prediction failed, these were equally likely to occur when one of the two categories was present in both elements of the pair (6/30) as it was to occur when no categories were shared across the pair (3/15). These results indicate both the presence of category-sensitive neurons within this ROI and the consistency of category- sensitive patterns from single to combined sentences. DISCUSSION Semantic knowledge involves not just the representation of single concepts but also the com- bination of these singular concepts into complex ideas. The construction of these higher-order units of meaning often requires the combination of concepts arising from different conceptual domains, which are differentially represented across the cortex. In this work, we asked two related questions: (1) Do specific brain regions play a particular role in combining concepts from different domains? (2) Do units of meaning that combine object categories continue to show a decentralised cortical representation, or are they represented in more centralised domain-general semantic regions? To address these questions, we presented participants with meaningful sentences comprising concepts referring to a single semantic category (e.g., People: “The doctors treat the patients”) or different semantic categories (e.g., People and Places: “The employees go to the office”). We found that the precuneus showed an increased response when meaning had to be constructed across distinct semantic domains and that the distributed representation of conceptual contents across category-selective regions persists when multidomain, higher-order meaning is constructed. Precuneus Activity Increases When Concepts Are Combined Across Domains The precuneus responded more strongly when sentences involved the combination of concep- tual domains compared to when sentences involved a single semantic category. This activa- tion remained significant when individual categories were iteratively removed from the contrast, indicating that the response is not driven by a specific category-selective response (Binder et al., 1999; Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013a). Furthermore, using the same univariate contrast, we did not observe any cortical modulation in other “language” regions associated with linguistic processing or complexity (Fedorenko, 2014; Santi & Grodzinsky, 2010), indi- cating that our strategy of matching sentence structure between single-category and combined-category items was successful and did not strongly influence brain regions associ- ated with linguistic demand. Taken together, these findings suggest that the precuneus is a component of the neuronal circuitry involved in the flexible construction of unitary meaning originating from distinct conceptual domains. Neurobiology of Language 365 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains Contemporary research has shown that the precuneus plays a dominant role in the general semantic system, being one of the most widely reported regions responding to semantically richer stimuli (Binder et al., 2009 for a review). The precuneus is a key element of the default mode network, an interconnected set of regions involved in internalised cognitive processes (Buckner et al., 2008; Buckner & DiNicola, 2019; Raichle et al., 2001). In this framework, the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex have been described as a central hub of the network, acting as a convergence zone of distinct functional subsystems (Buckner et al., 2008; Xu et al., 2016). Concurrently, research has suggested that the precuneus plays a pivotal role in the core network enabling episodic memory retrieval and prospective imagining (Schacter et al., 2007; Schacter et al., 2012). It is interesting to note that within episodic memory retrieval the pre- cuneus appears to play a linking role, flexibly binding together disparate conceptual informa- tion into meaningful units, a functional role that is notably consistent with the linking of concepts across domains to form transitory, higher-order semantic representations (for a related discussion, see Frankland & Greene, 2020). These observations are not incompatible with previously proposed models, such as the “convergence zone hypothesis” (Damasio et al., 2004), the “distributed-plus-hub view” (Patterson et al., 2007), or the classical “hub-and-spoke” model (Rogers et al., 2004). According to the latter theories, a single hub in the ATL supports transmodal conceptual representation (Lambon Ralph et al., 2016). In this view, the function of the ATL is to link modality-specific information into a unitary representation related to a stable, singular, conceptual representation. The precuneus may play an analogous role, forming transitory links between discrete concepts from different domains into a higher-order unit of semantic meaning. This interpretation is based on the assumption that concepts from two domains are com- bined when presented within these simple sentences, which is consistent with the automatic processioning of language (Kutas & Federmeier, 2011). However, it is possible that the precu- neus response may reflect the presence of two domains not their combination. To exclude this possibility, it would be necessary to formulate a condition where concepts from different domains are present but are not integrated, which may not be possible due to the mind’s tendency to impose sense by linking even inexplicably connected concepts (e.g., “aardvark,” “cannon”). A potential indicator that concepts are being combined across domains is the superadditive nature of the precuneus response evident in the independently defined category-selective ROI—the response to combined sentences is greater than that of the summed response of the constituent single-category sentences. Such non-linearity of response has long been seen as a marker for integration in the multisensory literature (Holmes & Spence, 2005) and suggests some interaction beyond the representation of the two concepts in isola- tion in the present study. The activation of the precuneus, together with the right AG, has been previously reported during the combination of noun–noun pairs (Graves et al., 2010). Converging findings from healthy adults and patients with neurodegenerative diseases (Price et al., 2015; Price et al., 2016) also support the role of the left AG in the integration of different concepts units (adjective–noun pairs) into meaningful combinations. While the general aim of these studies is consistent with the present work, here we adopted a paradigm with richer sentence stimuli (subjective, verb, complement) that was grounded in the combination of domain-specific concepts embedded in coherent meanings at the sentence level. Thus, while the AG may play a role in combining words to form more specific meaning (“lake house” or “red ball”), the precuneus is a potential mechanism by which concepts from different domains are genera- tively combined into higher-order meaning. Neurobiology of Language 366 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains Category-Selective Conceptual Representations for Single-Category Sentences While often reported for image processing in ventral and dorsal streams, category-selective responses are less common for word stimuli (Bi et al., 2016). Here, using sentences depicting specific semantic categories, we observe robust category-selective responses for People, Places, Food and Animals. Consistent with previous research, person-selective representations were seen in the precu- neus and vmPFC, (Fairhall et al., 2014; Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013a; Leibenluft et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2016) as well as lateral ATL (Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013a; Grabowski et al., 2001; Tempini et al., 1998; Wang et al., 2016). Likewise, selective activation of bilateral PPA, TOS, and RSC for places is highly consistent not only with selectivity during the percep- tion of places and scenes (Dilks et al., 2011; Epstein et al., 2007), but also in word-meaning related to places (Bi et al., 2016; Binder et al., 2009; Fairhall et al., 2014; Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013a), as well as spatially relevant geographic information about non-place ob- jects such as food or people (Fairhall, 2020). Food-selective responses were found in bilateral OFC, consistent with previous literature investigating neural responses to food-related pictures (García-García et al., 2013; Killgore et al., 2003; Simmons et al., 2005), potentially reflecting the role of this region in processing the reward value (Mainen & Kepecs, 2009). Selective re- sponses in VTC and amygdala have been previously reported in response to food pictures in relation to different motivational contexts (pre-/post-meal) in children and adolescents (Holsen et al., 2005). We did not observe a significant response in the left insula, which is frequently reported with studies using pictures (van der Laan et al., 2011, for a meta analysis). A food- selective response in the insula has been reported for word stimuli when participants access taste knowledge but the generalisation of this response to non-taste-related conceptual infor- mation is subtle and persists only at the voxel-level pattern (Fairhall, 2020). Animals selectively activated the precuneus, consistent with previous studies presenting participants with spoken names of animals (Binder et al., 1999). Responses in left IPS and right TPJ have been similarly reported by eliciting mental pictures of animals through spoken words (Lambert et al., 2002). We saw category selectivity for objects (here as human-made concrete items including manip- ulable objects) restricted to the pMTG, a region previously known to exhibit tool selectivity for word stimuli (Noppeney et al., 2006; Peelen et al., 2013). Combined-Category Representations Continue to Rely on Category-Selective Representations Previous work investigating the combination of words has emphasised centralised, default mode, semantic systems (Graves et al., 2010; Palliera et al., 2011; for a discussion, see Frankland & Greene, 2020), elements of which are known to contain representations of dif- ferent categories of objects (Bruffaerts et al., 2013; Devereux et al., 2013; Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013b; Liuzzi et al., 2020). To address whether the combination of concepts across domains continues to rely on distributed category-selective regions or is rather centra- lised into domain-general semantic systems, we compared the representation based upon single-category sentences to those of combined-category sentences across domain-sensitive ROIs. Specifically, we used the response evoked by the single-category sentences (e.g., People or Food) to predict the response to the relevant combined-category sentence (e.g., People and Food). The high consistency between the observed regional pattern of combined- category sentences and that predicted by combining the patterns of the relevant single-category sentences indicates that category-sensitive regions respond similarly when single-domain- specific information is processed and when a combination of domain-specific concepts are processed. Thus, both during the formation of complex ideas from single categories Neurobiology of Language 367 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains and in the combination of concepts across domains, distributed category-selective semantic representations continue to play a role in concept representation. This underscores the impor- tance of distributed category-selective semantic representations both during the formation of complex ideas from single categories and in the combination of concepts across domains. Conclusions Domain-specific concepts are a fundamental building block of our semantic cognition. At the same time, our cognitive system is constantly faced with the challenge of binding together distinct, category-selective semantic information in order to create the higher-order unitary meanings that allow flexible knowledge manipulation. In this work, we provide partial insight into how the human brain combines concepts into complex ideas. Our results suggest that the precuneus plays an important role in this regard, acting on diverse domain-specific semantic concepts across their respective neural representations and thus representing an important functional node of the human semantic system. Concurrently, the present findings showing highly comparable responses in category-sensitive regions, when both single and multiple domain-specific concepts are processed, indicates the persistence of decentralised represen- tations of conceptual knowledge when derived information combining concepts from multiple categories are formed. Collectively, these results show the importance of category-selective representations in the formation of higher-order semantic representations and the potential role of the precuneus in binding these together. FUNDING INFORMATION Scott Fairhall, H2020 European Research Council (StG), Award ID: 640594. AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS Silvia Ubaldi*: Investigation: Lead; Project administration: Lead; Data curation: Equal; Formal analysis: Equal; Project administration: Lead. Giuseppe Rabini*: Data curation: Equal; Formal analysis: Equal; Writing – original draft: Equal; Writing – review & editing: Equal. Scott L. Fairhall: Conceptualization: Lead; Formal analysis: Equal; Writing – original draft: Equal; Writing – review & editing: Equal; Supervision: Lead; Funding acquisition: Lead. [*These authors contributed equally to the research.] REFERENCES Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716–724. https://doi .org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x, PubMed: 25164052 Bi, Y., Wang, X., & Caramazza, A. (2016). Object domain and mo- dality in the ventral visual pathway. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 282–290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.02 .002, PubMed: 26944219 Binder, J. R. (2016). In defense of abstract conceptual representa- tions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1096–1108. https:// doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0909-1, PubMed: 27294428 Binder, J. R., & Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 527–536. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001, PubMed: 22001867 Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta- analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055, PubMed: 19329570 Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Rao, S. M., & Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 80–93. https://doi.org/10.1162 /089892999563265, PubMed: 9950716 Borghesani, V., & Piazza, M. (2017). The neuro-cognitive representa- tions of symbols: The case of concrete words. Neuropsychologia, 105, 4–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06 .026, PubMed: 28648571 Bruffaerts, R., Dupont, P., Peeters, R., De Deyne, S., Storms, G., & Vandenberghe, R. (2013). Similarity of fMRI activity patterns in left perirhinal cortex reflects semantic similarity between words. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 18597–18607. https://doi.org/10 .1523/JNEUROSCI.1548-13.2013, PubMed: 24259581 Neurobiology of Language 368 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L., (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 11(24), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011, PubMed: 18400922 Buckner, R. L., & DiNicola, L. M. (2019). The brain’s default net- work: Updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 593–608. https://doi.org/10 .1038/s41583-019-0212-7, PubMed: 31492945 Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A. (2003). What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A crit- ical review of the clinical evidence. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3), 213–261. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290244000266, PubMed: 20957571 Caramazza, A., & Mahon, B. Z. (2003). The organization of con- ceptual knowledge: The evidence from category-specific seman- tic deficits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 354–361. https://doi .org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00159-1, PubMed: 12907231 Caramazza, A., & Shelton, J. R. (1998). Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate-inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1162 /089892998563752, PubMed: 9526080 Chen, L., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). A unified model of human semantic knowledge and its disorders. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016 -0039, PubMed: 28480333 Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Grabowski, T., Adolphs, R., & Damasio, A. (2004). Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition, 92, 179–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition .2002.07.001, PubMed: 15037130 Deniz, F., Nunez-Elizalde, A. O., Huth, A. G., & Gallant, J. L. (2019). The representation of semantic information across human cerebral cortex during listening versus reading is invariant to stimulus modality. Journal of Neuroscience, 39, 7722–7736. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.0675-19.2019, PubMed: 31427396 Devereux, B. J., Clarke, A., Marouchos, A., & Tyler, L. K. (2013). Representational similarity analysis reveals commonalities and differences in the semantic processing of words and objects. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 18906–18916. https://doi.org/10 .1523/JNEUROSCI.3809-13.2013, PubMed: 24285896 Dilks, D. D., Julian, J. B., Kubilius, J., Spelke, E. S., & Kanwisher, N. (2011). Mirror-image sensitivity and invariance in object and scene processing pathways. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 11305–11312. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.1935-11 .2011, PubMed: 21813690 Epstein, R. A., Parker, W. E., & Feiler, A. M. (2007). Where am I now? Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices in place recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 6141–6149. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.0799-07.2007, PubMed: 17553986 Fairhall, S. L. (2020). Cross recruitment of domain-selective cortical representations enables flexible semantic knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(15), 3096–3103. https://doi.org/10.1523 /JNEUROSCI.2224-19.2020, PubMed: 32152199 Fairhall, S. L., Anzellotti, S., Ubaldi, S., & Caramazza, A. (2014). Person- and place-selective neural substrates for entity-specific semantic access. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 1687–1696. https://doi .org/10.1093/cercor/bht039, PubMed: 23425892 Fairhall, S. L., & Caramazza, A. (2013a). Category-selective neural substrates for person- and place-related concepts. Cortex, 49, 2748–2757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.010, PubMed: 23831433 Fairhall, S. L., & Caramazza, A. (2013b). Brain regions that repre- sent amodal conceptual knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(25), 10552–10558. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI .0051-13.2013, PubMed: 23785167 Fedorenko, E. (2014). The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 335. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00335, PubMed: 24803909 Frankland, S. M., & Greene, J. D. (2020). Concepts and composi- tionality: In search of the brain’s language of thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 273–303. https://doi.org/10.1146 /annurev-psych-122216-011829, PubMed: 31550985 Gallese, V. (2005). Embodied simulation: From neurons to phe- nomenal experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4, 23–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-005-4737-z García-García, I., Narberhaus, A., Marqués-Iturria, I., Garolera, M., Ra(cid:1)doi, A., Segura, B., Pueyo, R., Ariza, M., & Jurado, M. A. (2013). Neural responses to visual food cues: Insights from func- tional magnetic resonance imaging. European Eating Disorders Review, 21(2), 89–98. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2216, PubMed: 23348964 Grabowski, T. J., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Ponto, L. L. B., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). A role for left temporal pole in the retrieval of words for unique entities. Human Brain Mapping, 13(4), 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1002/ hbm.1033, PubMed: 11410949 Graves, W. W., Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Conant, L. L., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2010). Neural correlates of implicit and ex- plicit combinatorial semantic processing. Neuroimage, 53(2), 638–646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.055, PubMed: 20600969 Holmes, N. P., & Spence, C. (2005). Multisensory integration: Space, time and superadditivity. Current Biololgy, 15(18), R762–R764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.058, PubMed: 16169476 Holsen, L. M., Zarcone, J. R., Thompson, T. I., Brooks, W. M., Anderson, M. F., Ahluwalia, J. S., Nollen, N. L., & Savage, C. R. (2005). Neural mechanisms underlying food motivation in chil- dren and adolescents. Neuroimage, 7(3), 669–676. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.043, PubMed: 15993629 Huth, A. G., De Heer, W. A., Griffiths, T. L., Theunissen, F. E., & Jack, L. (2016). Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature, 532, 453–458. https://doi .org/10.1038/nature17637, PubMed: 27121839 Kiefer, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex, 48, 805–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .cortex.2011.04.006, PubMed: 21621764 Killgore, W. D. S., Young, A. D., Femia, L. A., Bogorodzki, P., Rogowska, J., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2003). Cortical and lim- bic activation during viewing of high- versus low-calorie foods. Neuroimage, 19, 1381–1394. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053 -8119(03)00191-5, PubMed: 12948696 Kuhnke, P., Kiefer, M., & Hartwigsen, G. (2020). Task-dependent recruitment of modality-specific and multimodal regions during conceptual processing. Cerebral Cortex, 30, 3938–3959. https:// doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa010, PubMed: 32219378 Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123, PubMed: 20809790 Lambert, S., Sampaio, E., Scheiber, C., & Mauss, Y. (2002). Neural substrates of animal mental imagery: Calcarine sulcus and dorsal Neurobiology of Language 369 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains pathway involvement—An fMRI study. Brain Research, 924(2), 176–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03232-2, PubMed: 11750903 Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2016). The neural and computational bases of semantic cogni- tion. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 42–55. https://doi.org/10 .1038/nrn.2016.150, PubMed: 27881854 Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M. I., Harrison, T., & Haxby, J. V. (2004). Mothers’ neural activation in response to pictures of their chil- dren and other children. Biological Psychiatry, 56(4), 225–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.017, PubMed: 15312809 Lin, N., Xu, Y., Wang, X., Yang, H., Du, M., Hua, H., & Li, X. (2019). Coin, telephone, and handcuffs: Neural correlates of so- cial knowledge of inanimate objects. Neuropsychologia, 133, 107187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019 .107187, PubMed: 31499047 Liuzzi, A. G., Aglinskas, A., & Fairhall, S. L. (2020). General and feature-based semantic representations in the semantic network. Scientific Reports, 10, 8931. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020 -65906-0, PubMed: 32488152 Mahon, B. Z., Anzellotti, S., Schwarzbach, J., Zampini, M., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Category-specific organization in the human brain does not require visual experience. Neuron, 63, 397–405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.012, PubMed: 19679078 Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the em- bodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology Paris, 102(1–3), 59–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.004, PubMed: 18448316 Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2011). What drives the organiza- tion of object knowledge in the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.01 .004, PubMed: 21317022 Mainen, Z. F., & Kepecs, A. (2009). Neural representation of behav- ioral outcomes in the orbitofrontal cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(1), 84–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb .2009.03.010, PubMed: 19427193 Martin, A. (2016). GRAPES—Grounding representations in action, perception, and emotion systems: How object properties and categories are represented in the human brain. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 979–990. https://doi.org/10.3758 /s13423-015-0842-3, PubMed: 25968087 Miceli, G., Capasso, R., Daniele, A., Esposito, T., Magarelli, M., & Tomaiuolo, F. (2000). Selective deficit for people’s names follow- ing left temporal damage: An impairment of domain-specific conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 489–516. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290050110629, PubMed: 20945192 Noppeney, U., Price, C. J., Penny, W. D., & Friston, K. J. (2006). Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective re- sponses. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 437–445. https://doi.org/10.1093 /cercor/bhi123, PubMed: 15944370 Palliera, C., Devauchellea, A. D., & Dehaenea, S. (2011). Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108(6), 2522–2527. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1018711108, PubMed: 21224415 Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowl- edge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 976–987. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277, PubMed: 18026167 Peelen, M. V., Bracci, S., Lu, X., He, C., Caramazza, A., & Bi, Y. (2013). Tool selectivity in left occipitotemporal cortex develops without vision. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(8), 1225–1234. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00411, PubMed: 23647514 Pereira, F., Lou, B., Pritchett, B., Ritter, S., Gershman, S. J., Kanwisher, N., Botvinick, M., & Fedorenko, E. (2018). Toward a universal decoder of linguistic meaning from brain activation. Nature Communications, 9(1), 963. https://doi.org/10.1038 /s41467-018-03068-4, PubMed: 29511192 Price, A. R., Bonner, M. F., Peelle, J. E., & Grossman, M. (2015). Converging evidence for the neuroanatomic basis of combinato- rial semantics in the angular gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 3276–3284. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-14.2015, PubMed: 25698762 Price, A. R., Peelle, J. E., Bonner, M. F., Grossman, M., & Hamilton, R. H. (2016). Causal evidence for a mechanism of semantic in- tegration in the angular gyrus as revealed by high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(13), 3829–3838. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.3120 -15.2016, PubMed: 27030767 Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98(2), 676–682. https://doi.org/10.1073 /pnas.98.2.676, PubMed: 11209064 Rice, G. E., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Hoffman, P. (2015). The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowl- edge: An ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging stud- ies. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 4374–4391. https://doi.org/10.1093 /cercor/bhv024, PubMed: 25771223 Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., McClelland, J. L., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2004). Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: A neuropsycho- logical and computational investigation. Psychological Review, 111, 205–235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.205, PubMed: 14756594 Santi, A., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2010). fMRI adaptation dissociates syntac- tic complexity dimensions. Neuroimage, 51(4), 1285–93. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.034, PubMed: 20338244 (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657–661. https://doi .org/10.1038/nrn2213, PubMed: 17700624 Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. N., & Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677–694. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001, PubMed: 23177955 Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L.W. (2005). Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1602–1608. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor /bhi038, PubMed: 15703257 Tempini, M. L. G., Price, C. J., Josephs, O., Vandenberghe, R., Cappa, S. F., Kapur, N., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1998). The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing. Brain, 121, 2103–2118. https://doi.org/10.1093/ brain/121.11.2103, PubMed: 9827770 van der Laan, L. N., de Ridder, D. T. D., Viergever, M. A., & Smeets, P. A. M. (2011). The first taste is always with the eyes: A meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage, 55, 296–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .neuroimage.2010.11.055, PubMed: 21111829 Visser, M., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Semantic processing in the anterior temporal lobes: A meta-analysis of Neurobiology of Language 370 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Combining concepts across categorical domains the functional neuroimaging literature. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1083–1094. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn .2009.21309, PubMed: 19583477 Wang, X., Peelen, M. V., & Han, Z., Caramazza, A., & Bi Y. (2016). The role of vision in the neural representation of unique entities. Neuropsychologia, 87, 144–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .neuropsychologia.2016.05.007, PubMed: 27174518 Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107(Pt 3), 829–854. https://doi.org/10.1093 /brain/107.3.829, PubMed: 6206910 Xu, Y., Lin, Q., Han, Z., He, Y., & Bi, Y. (2016). Intrinsic functional network architecture of human semantic processing: Modules and hubs. Neuroimage, 132, 542–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .neuroimage.2016.03.004, PubMed: 26973170 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 3 3 5 4 1 9 2 8 8 9 3 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 3 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Neurobiology of Language 371RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image

Télécharger le PDF