RECHERCHE

RECHERCHE

Parallel processing relies on a distributed,
low-dimensional cortico-cerebellar architecture

Eli J. Müller1,2, Fulvia Palesi3,4, Kevin Y. Hou1,2, Joshua Tan1,2, Thomas Close5,6,7,
Claudia A. M.. Gandini Wheeler-Kingschott3,4,8, Egidio D’Angelo3,4,
Fernando Calamante2,6,8, and James M. Shine1,2

1Complex Systems Research Group, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
2Brain and Mind Centre, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
3Brain Connectivity Research Center, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
4Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
5National Imaging Facility, Sydney, NSW, Australia
6School of Biomedical Engineering, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
7Sydney Imaging, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
8NMR Research Unit, Queen Square Multiple Sclerosis Centre, Faculty of Brain Sciences, UCL Queen
Square Institute of Neurology, UCL, Londres, ROYAUME-UNI

Mots clés: IRMf, Dual-task, Cerebellum, Cerebral cortex, Diffusion, Parallel

ABSTRAIT

A characteristic feature of human cognition is our ability to ‘multi-task’—performing two or
more tasks in parallel—particularly when one task is well learned. How the brain supports
this capacity remains poorly understood. Most past studies have focussed on identifying
the areas of the brain—typically the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex—that are required to
navigate information-processing bottlenecks. In contrast, we take a systems neuroscience
approach to test the hypothesis that the capacity to conduct effective parallel processing relies
on a distributed architecture that interconnects the cerebral cortex with the cerebellum.
The latter structure contains over half of the neurons in the adult human brain and is well
suited to support the fast, effective, dynamic sequences required to perform tasks relatively
automatically. By delegating stereotyped within-task computations to the cerebellum, le
cerebral cortex can be freed up to focus on the more challenging aspects of performing the
tasks in parallel. To test this hypothesis, we analysed task-based fMRI data from 50 participants
who performed a task in which they either balanced an avatar on a screen (équilibre),
performed serial-7 subtractions (calculation) or performed both in parallel (dual task). Using a
set of approaches that include dimensionality reduction, structure-function coupling, et
time-varying functional connectivity, we provide robust evidence in support of our hypothesis.
We conclude that distributed interactions between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum are
crucially involved in parallel processing in the human brain.

RÉSUMÉ DE L'AUTEUR

How does the brain support the performance of multiple complex tasks, in parallel? Le
distributed architecture of the cerebellum is ideally placed to interact with the cerebral cortex,
creating complex channels for segregated information processing that afford the execution
of parallel tasks. Ici, we apply time-resolved functional connectivity analyses to functional

un accès ouvert

journal

Citation: Müller, E. J., Palesi, F., Hou,
K. Y., Tan, J., Close, T., Gandini
Wheeler-Kingschott, C. UN. M., D’Angelo,
E., Calamente, F., & Shine, J.. M.. (2023).
Parallel processing relies on a
distributed, low-dimensional cortico-
cerebellar architecture. Réseau
Neurosciences, 7(2), 844–863.
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00308

EST CE QUE JE:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00308

Informations complémentaires:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00308

Reçu: 22 Juillet 2022
Accepté: 11 Janvier 2023

Intérêts concurrents: Les auteurs ont
a déclaré qu'aucun intérêt concurrent
exister.

Auteur correspondant:
James M.. Shine
mac.shine@sydney.edu.au

Éditeur de manipulation:
Lucina Uddin

droits d'auteur: © 2023
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Publié sous Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International
(CC PAR 4.0) Licence

La presse du MIT

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

t

/

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

t

.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

Parallel processing:
The running of two or more
processes in tandem.

Cerebellum:
A physically small but neuronally
dense structure in the hindbrain
important for sensorimotor
adaptation and anticipation.

MRI data collected while individuals performed a dual task that required either balancing,
calculating, or the two in tandem. We found robust evidence for distinct patterns of cortico-
cerebellar connectivity as a function of task performance.

INTRODUCTION

How do distributed whole-brain neural activity patterns give rise to human cognitive function?
This question lies at the heart of modern psychology and neuroscience but, despite decades of
neuroimaging experiments, we still do not have a clear answer. One reason is that conven-
tional neuroimaging methods applied to data from cognitive tasks typically represent the brain
as a static snapshot of independent parts or at best, ‘functionally connected’ pairs of brain
régions (John et al., 2022). Another important issue is that neuroimaging experiments are usu-
ally designed to identify regions that are most selectively associated with a specific task, mais
are less well suited to distinguishing the presence of multiple concurrent cognitive constructs
within the same task (Poldrack, 2012). For these reasons, many leading theories in cognitive
neuroscience have relied on relatively static descriptions of the ‘key brain regions involved’ in
a particular task.

In contrast to this view, there is evidence to suggest that the neural implementation of
cognitive function in humans is far more dynamic and integrative (Eisenreich et al., 2017).
In solving real world problems, we rarely isolate a specific cognitive capacity, such as focussed
attention or resistance to distraction, but instead combine multiple cognitive constructs
together in order to solve challenges in real time (Poldrack et al., 2011). Consider an experi-
enced driver navigating heavy highway traffic in the pouring rain—the driver must remain
focussed on the road, ensure the windshield wipers are on, regularly check their blind spots
and also keep the pedals depressed at the appropriate level. This view of cognitive function in
the real world is crucially dependent on the parallel processing of multiple distinct challenges;
cependant, for the reasons outlined above, we still lack a satisfying description of how the
human brain is capable of supporting parallel processing.

To facilitate complex coordinated behavioural responses underpinned by similarly complex
spatiotemporal activity patterns, the brain may first learn to execute at least one of the com-
putations automatically (c'est à dire., without paying close, conscious attention to the completion of
the task). Pour y parvenir, the system must be capable of responding to specific contexts with a
high degree of spatial and temporal precision (Schmitz & Duncan, 2018). Secondly, le
responses must be relatively error free and reliable. Enfin, the system must be able to be trig-
gered in the presence of a specific stimulus or context without the need for deliberate atten-
tion. Without making the responses to different computational burdens relatively stereotyped
in this fashion, performing two (ou plus) computations in parallel would require the prioritisa-
tion of one of the computations, likely to the detriment of the other task(s). En outre, any two
tasks learned by the same network could potentially run into structural interference (Petri et al.,
2021), particularly if the networks required to complete the overlapping tasks use similar
cortical regions.

Surtout, the architecture of the cerebellum is ideally suited to fulfil each of the features
required for automatic processing, both in the sensorimotor and cognitive domains (D’Angelo,
2019; D’Angelo & Casali, 2013; Ramnani, 2014; Shine et al., 2019). D'abord, the cerebellum is
organized in parallel modules with different cerebrocortical regions (D’Angelo & Casali,
2013). In direct contrast to the basal ganglia, the internal circuitry of the cerebellar cortex

Neurosciences en réseau

845

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

t

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

.

t

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

Cerebral cortex:
The thin, outer layer of the
telencephalon important for
deliberate, conscious processing.

Dual task:
The performance of two
simultaneous tasks, one of which is
typically assumed to be easier to
automatise than the other.

Low-dimensional:
A system whose activity can be
expressed through a smaller number
of components without a substantial
loss in explained variance.

consists of sparse, distributed connectivity patterns that likely support dimensionality expan-
sion (Cayco-Gajic & Silver, 2019), rather than reduction (as is the case for the basal ganglia;
Bar-Gad et al., 2003; Wilson, 2013). En outre, the glutamatergic outputs of the cerebellum
through the deep cerebellar nuclei innervate ‘core’ thalamic nuclei (Kuramoto et al., 2009),
which project to the granular layers of the frontal cortex (Preuss & Sage, 2022) in a much more
precise manner than the ‘matrix’ thalamic nuclei. There is also evidence that cerebellar cir-
cuits can condition on their own outputs, and hence learn to execute specific sequences of
effects based on triggering context signals (Khilkevich et al., 2018). Anatomically, the cerebel-
lum is bidirectionally interconnected with multiple cerebrocortical areas, with major tracts
connecting the dentate nucleus to the frontal and prefrontal cerebral cortex, along with other
associative areas (Palesi et al., 2015, 2017). Functionally, the cerebellum plays a critical role in
shaping complex functional network dynamics (Palesi et al., 2020), as evidenced by its role in
both resting-state (Castellazzi et al., 2014, 2018) and task-related neuroimaging studies
(Alahmadi et al., 2016; Balsters & Ramnani, 2011; Casiraghi et al., 2019; Shine et al.,
2019). Based on these architectural features and relationships with complex, dynamic neuro-
imaging patterns, we hypothesized that connections between the cerebellar cortex and cere-
bellum are crucial for the facilitation of parallel processing. Using a set of approaches that
include dimensionality reduction, structure-function coupling, and time-varying functional
connectivité, we provide robust evidence in support of our hypothesis.

RÉSULTATS

To test this hypothesis, we reanalysed an existing fMRI dataset (Papegaaij et al., 2017) consist-
ing of 50 healthy individuals dual task in a 3T MRI scanner with their feet resting on a force
plate (Figure 1A), and their vision oriented towards a two-dimensional avatar that tilted for-
ward and backward. There were three distinct trial types: during balance blocks (Figure 1B,
blue), participants had to stabilize the slow fluctuations of the avatar using forward and back-
ward movements on the force plate; during calculation blocks (Figure 1C, red), subjects had to
track between three and four audible beeps, and then subtract that number, multiplied by 7,
from a cue number presented prior to the trial; and during dual-task blocks (Figure 1D, purple),
subjects performed both trials simultaneously.

Brain State Signatures During Dual-Task Performance

D'abord, we compared the BOLD patterns associated with the performance of the three different
tasks blocks. Spécifiquement, we created a difference map between the average group-level β
parameters estimated from 400 cortical and 28 cerebellar regions of interest for the balance
and calculation trials (Δ) (Figure 1E). By comparing this difference map to the β map from the
dual-task trials—r(Δ, βDT)—we could determine whether performing the two tasks in tandem
led to a brain map that was more or less like one or the other single tasks—a positive corre-
lation with this map (λ1) was suggestive of the dual task reflecting the more challenging
calculation task, a negative correlation with the less challenging balance task, and a null cor-
relation with the notion of optimally splitting activity between the two (or a pattern orthogonal
to the two single tasks). Consistent with the second option, we found that the low-dimensional
signature of dual-task performance was more similar to the calculation β map than the balance
β map (r = 0.192 ± 0.05, p = 6.5 × 10−5; Figure 1F), suggesting that during the dual-task trials,
the cerebral cortex and cerebellum configured their activity to ensure the effective completion
of the calculation trials.

Neurosciences en réseau

846

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

t

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

t

.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

Chiffre 1. Low-dimensional balance between integration and segregation during dual-task performance. (UN) participants lay supine in an MRI
scanner, with their legs controlling a force plate. (B) Balance trials (blue) involved a dynamically moving avatar that the participant had to
match. (C) calculation trials involved listening to a series of beeps, and then subtracting the multiple of 7 times the number of beeps from a cue
number (red). (D) dual-task trials required performing both tasks, simultaneously (purple). (E) The calculation trials recruited increased BOLD in
fronto-parietal and visual cortices, along with right superior cerebellum, whereas balance trials were associated with increased BOLD in lateral
visual cortex, medial motor cortex, and parietal operculum. (F) the dual-task β map across all 50 subjects was more similar to the calculation
β map (c'est à dire., positive correlation with λ1) than the balance β map (c'est à dire., inverse correlation with λ1); *** p < 0.001. Despite the brain states during dual-task trials having more in common with the calculation than the balance trials, close examination of the RMS error of the balance portion of the dual- task trials suggests that subjects were performing the task as well as than when they performed the balance trial on its own (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: p = 0.358). So how was the brain con- figured on these dual-task trials in order to mediate this stability? Based on previous empirical (Balsters & Ramnani, 2011) and theoretical (D’Angelo & Casali, 2013; Shine, 2020; Shine & Shine, 2014) work, we hypothesized that the distributed architecture integrating the cerebral cortex and cerebellum should be important for mediating this putative parallel processing per- formance. One straightforward prediction is that balancing multiple tasks at the same time should recruit more regions of the cerebellum, and hence that cerebellar blood flow should be more strongly associated with dual-task performance than either the balance or calculation task alone. We found evidence to confirm this hypothesis—namely, greater proportion of cer- ebellar regions were associated with a positive mean β value in dual task as compared to bal- ance and/or calculation trials (67.3% vs. 35.7% and 39.3%, respectively; χ2 (2, N = 50) = 249.6, p < 1.0 × 10−4). Unique Patterns of Cortico-Cerebellar Functional Connectivity During Dual-Task Performance Given that the dual-task trials were more similar to calculation trials than balance trials (Figure 1F), how was the brain capable of supporting multiple tasks at the same time? We hypothesized that balance, calculation, and dual-task trials should have unique patterns of cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity that could allow the brain to support multiple channels of communication within the same system. To test this hypothesis, we calculated the time-varying functional connectivity between all cortical and cerebellar parcels using the Multiplication of Temporal Derivatives approach (window = 20 TRs; Shine et al., 2015) and then contrasted the three trial types with one another. We observed robust differences between the three trial types (Figure 2). For instance, calculation trials (when compared to balance trials) were associated with widespread cortico-cerebellar connectivity between Network Neuroscience 847 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking lobule V and the majority of cortical networks, as well as more targeted connections between VIIIa/IX and primary sensorimotor networks (Figure 2A). In contrast, balance trials (when compared to calculation trials) showed predominant increases in intermediate cerebellar lobules (e.g., Crus I and II) with higher order cortical networks. In contrast, dual-task trials were associated with heightened fronto-parietal connections with intermediate cerebellar lobules, particularly Crus I and VIIIa, when compared to both balance (Figure 2B) and calculation trials (Figure 2C). Having confirmed a robust relationship between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum during dual-task performance, we next asked whether cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity patterns differentiated between correct and error dual-task trials. To test this hypothesis, we fit a General Linear Model to each dual-task trial, independently, for each cortico-cerebellar time-varying connectivity score. We then split dual-task trials into correct (accurate calculation and small RMS error [<50% of population distribution]) and incorrect (inaccurate calculation, large RMS error [>50%] or both) trials and compared (using a set of independent-samples
t tests) the task-based functional connectivity between cortical and cerebellar parcels as a
function of effective dual-task performance. We conducted a permutation test (5,000 itera-
tion) to determine the likelihood of each edge being distinct between the two groups by
chance. To summarize these results, we computed the mean significant β-value for the func-
tional connectivity between each cerebellar lobule (averaged across hemispheres, and ignor-
ing the connections of the vermis; from the cerebellar SUIT atlas (Diedrichsen, 2006)) et
each of 7 pre-identified cortical networks (the Yeo 7 parcellation from the 400-region Schaefer
atlas (Schaefer et al., 2018; Chiffre 3). We found a robust increase in task-based functional
connectivity between the ventral attention network ( VAN) and lobules Crus II, VIIb, VIIIa
and VI (Chiffre 3), as well as more distributed connections between lobule X and multiple cor-
tical subnetworks. In contrast, Crus I was relatively functionally disconnected from all cortical

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

/

t

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

.

t

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Chiffre 2. Cortico-cerebellar task-based functional connectivity. (UN) patterns of task-based cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity during
calculation (red) when compared to balance (blue) trials—upper: mean task-based connectivity strength for cerebral cortex and cerebellum;
lower: mean task-based connectivity strength collapsed into 7 Yeo networks (columns) et 10 average lobules (rows). (B) similar maps for dual
task (purple) versus balance. (C) similar maps for dual task versus calculation. Note: results were consistent for left and right hemispheres. VIS =
visuel; SM = somatomotor; DAN = dorsal attention network; VAN = ventral attention network; LIM = limbic network; CON = control network;
DMN = default mode network.

Neurosciences en réseau

848

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

L'intégration:
The formation of a unified or
coordinated whole—in the case of
brain networks, the presence of
relatively diffuse connections across
brain regions.

Segregation:
The formation of setting something
apart from others—in the case of
brain networks, the presence of
tight-knit subcommunities.

Increased cortico-cerebellar task-based functional connectivity associated with success-
Chiffre 3.
ful dual-task performance. Gauche: Key cortical and cerebellar regions with heightened task-based
functional connectivity during dual-task trials with correct versus incorrect answers. Droite: Mean
significant β-value (cortical sub-network [Yeo 7 atlas] vs. cerebellar lobule [SUIT atlas]) associated
with task-based functional connectivity values for correct versus incorrect dual-task performance
(p < 0.001; random permutation test). VIS = visual; SM = somatomotor; DAN = dorsal attention network; VAN = ventral attention network; LIM = limbic network; CON = control network; DMN = default mode network. networks (except VAN) during effective dual-task performance, which is consistent with known patterns of cerebellar lesion-related cognitive impairments (Ilg et al., 2013). Dual-Task Performance Balances Network Integration and Segregation One way in which the distributed cortico-cerebellar architecture could facilitate effective par- allel processing is by striking an effective balance between integration and segregation (Bassett et al., 2015, p. 201; Mohr et al., 2016; Shine & Poldrack, 2017). In previous work, we have used a combination of time-varying functional connectivity and a topological measure that quan- tifies network-level integration—the participation coefficient (PC; Shine et al., 2016)—to dem- onstrate that the systems-level network structure of functional connectivity changes during task performance, with cognitively challenging tasks requiring higher integration than relatively simple tasks (Shine et al., 2016). From this, we predicted that the balance task should be rela- tively segregated (i.e., low PC), the calculation task should be relatively integrated (i.e., high PC), and the dual-task trials should strike a balance between the two extremes (i.e., intermediate PC). Using our standard time-varying analysis (see Methods), we observed robust evidence for our predictions (Figure 4; F2,147 = 3.41; p = 0.036). In addition, although the dual-task topo- logical pattern was positively correlated with the average of balance and calculation (r = 0.464; p < 0.001), it was not a direct superposition of the two maps, suggesting topological reconfig- uration during the different task states. Together, these results confirm that parallel processing in the brain is supported by a topological balance between integration and segregation. Cortico-Cerebellar Activity Flow Mapping The input and output streams of cerebral cortex and cerebellum interact via distinct white mat- ter pathways. Importantly, while the structural connections between these two structures are reciprocal, they are imbalanced (Palesi et al., 2015, 2017)—different pathways exist from the cerebral cortex to the cerebellum than from the cerebellum to the cerebral cortex. Specifically, thick-tufted layer V pyramidal neurons in the deep layers of the cerebral cortex send projec- tions to the mossy fibre pathway of the cerebellum (via the pontine nuclei), thus forming the cortico-ponto-cerebellar (CPC) tract (Figure 5A). In contrast, the cerebral cortex receives feed- back from the cerebellum via the deep cerebellar nuclei, which project via the ‘Core’ nuclei of the thalamus—that is, the cerebello-thalamo-cortical (CTC) tract (Figure 5B). Plastic changes Network Neuroscience 849 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking Figure 4. Parallel processing balances integration and segregation. Balance trials were associated with relative segregation (low PC; blue), calculation trials with relative integration (high PC; red), and dual-task trials with a balance between integration and segregation (intermediate PC; purple); F2,147 = 3.41; p = 0.036. Thick lines represent the median value for each group. between the mossy fibre pathway and the Purkinje cells of the cerebellar cortex are proposed to act as a major site for the refinement of automatized behaviour (D’Angelo et al., 2016; Ramnani, 2014; Shine, 2020; Shine & Shine, 2014) and hence, the capacity to perform mul- tiple tasks simultaneously. From our observations that the time series of the cerebral cortex and cerebellum were highly coordinated during dual-task behaviour, we hypothesized that the specific patterns of BOLD activity in both the cortex and cerebellum should be related to the intersection between prior BOLD activity in the cerebellum (via the CTC) and cerebral cortex (via the CPC). To test this hypothesis, we adapted the activity flow mapping approach (Cole et al., 2016) to incorporate the structural connectivity between the cortex and cerebellum. Specifically, we extracted 9 × 107 structural connectivity weights for both the contralateral CPC (Figure 6A, orange) and CTC (Figure 6B, green) tracts (Palesi et al., 2017) from a single healthy 26– 30-year-old female (ID no. 100307) from the Human Connectome Project (a single subject connectome was chosen so as to retain precision in the parcel-to-parcel connectivity estimates for both CPC and CTC—note, however, that maps were highly similar to those previously extracted from 28 healthy participants from the HCP (Palesi et al., 2017)). While both tracts are overexpressed in the frontal cortices, there were relatively more CPC projections from the parietal lobes and more CTC projections that innervate the frontal cortex, which is consistent with known anatomical projection patterns (D’Angelo & Casali, 2013; Prevosto & Sommer, 2013; Ramnani, 2006; Shine, 2020). A parsimonious interpretation of these data is that the frontal cortex benefits from the information provided to the cerebellum by posterior cortices that process potential opportunities for action (also known as affordances; Pezzulo & Cisek, 2016). If cortico-cerebellar communication is required for effective dual-task performance, then blood flow within either the cerebral cortex or cerebellum during dual-task trials should be predictive of subsequent blood flow (assuming sufficient delay) within the cortical (or Network Neuroscience 850 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l Figure 5. White matter streamlines interconnecting the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex. (A) the cortico-ponto-cerebellar (CPC) tract sends projections from the cortex via the pontine nuclei into the mossy fibres of the cerebellum; (B) the cerebello-thalamo-cortical (CTC) tract derives from the deep cerebellar nuclei, which project back via the core thalamic nuclei to the cerebral cortex. Tracts created using mrtrix were projected onto a T1-weighted structural image from individual 100307 from the Human Connectome Project (de-faced to preserve autonomy). The colours of each tract represent the primary direction of each tract: blue = inferior-superior; red = left-right; green = anterior-posterior. f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 cerebellar) regions to which they are connected by white matter projections. To create an esti- mate of what these predicted BOLD responses should be, we created two template maps—one for predicted cerebellar activity (estimated cerebellar activity: ACTX = WCBM . CPC) and one for predicted cortical activity (estimated cortical activity: ACBM = WCTX . CTC)—by multiply- ing the cortico-cerebellar structural connectivity matrices with the preprocessed BOLD pattern observed during the three different trial types. We then correlated these prediction vectors with the actual BOLD patterns in the respective regions. If the observed patterns of activity were similar, we can conclude that BOLD activity patterns were intimately related to the reciprocal structural connections between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum. Across all three trial types, both cortico-cerebellar (via CPC; Figure 6C, circles) and cerebello-cortical (via CTC; Figure 6D, squares) activity flow patterns were significantly greater for actual versus randomly shuffled data (all p < 0.05), suggesting that functional activity was coordinated by connections both from the cerebral cortex to the cerebellum (i.e., CPC) and vice versa (i.e., CTC) across all tasks. Interestingly, despite the consistent positive relationships, cerebello-cortical connections (i.e., CTC) were more robustly able to predict subsequent cortical patterns than cortico-cerebellar connections (i.e., CPC), suggesting that the feedback from the cerebellum to the cerebral cortex was more crucial for task performance. Finally, we Network Neuroscience 851 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 6. Cortico-cerebellar structure-function mapping across trial types. (A) normalized (in log10 of white matter connectivity) map of projections from the cerebral cortex to cerebellum via CPC (orange). (B) normalized (in log10 of white matter connectivity) map of projections from cerebellum to the cerebellar cortex via CTC (green). (C) activity flow mapping (Cole et al., 2016) between cerebellar BOLD patterns predicted from CPC tract in balance (Bal, blue), calculation (Calc, orange), and dual-task (DT, purple) trials (circles); see Methods for details. (D) the same for cortical BOLD patterns predicted from the CTC tract (squares). All activity flow map correlations were greater than permuted null levels. found that the match between ACTX/ACBM and the raw data was greater in correct versus incorrect dual-task trials for both cerebral cortex (T = 2.397, p = 0.017) and cerebellum (T = 2.049, p = 0.041), further confirming the importance of cortico-cerebellar interaction for parallel processing. DISCUSSION In this study, we used systems-level neuroimaging analysis to demonstrate that robust interac- tions between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum are associated with effective dual-task per- formance. We hypothesized that, through distributed white matter pathways that interconnect these major cortical systems, the brain can differentiate different task contexts so as to effec- tively maintain the performance of two computational tasks in parallel. To test this hypothesis, we analysed BOLD data from the cerebral cortex and cerebellum, and in doing so demon- strated that dual-Task performance recruited heightened cerebellar activity (Figure 1) and func- tional connectivity between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum (Figures 2 and 3) that was linked to the balance between integration and segregation (Figure 4) and related to the struc- tural connections between the cerebellum and cerebral cortex (Figures 5 and 6). Together, these results highlight the importance of systems-level interactions in the manifestation of complex cognitive capacities. Our results clearly demonstrate that models that incorporate the cerebellum and its massive, high-dimensional architecture provide a more parsimonious account for how the brain can Network Neuroscience 852 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking Automaticity: The ability to perform a behaviour without deliberate, focussed attention. balance the challenges inherent with parallel processing (Balsters & Ramnani, 2011; D’Angelo & Casali, 2013; Shine, 2020; Wu et al., 2013). The distributed circuits that interconnect the cerebral cortex and cerebellum are optimally set up to fulfil this capacity. Specifically, the major output of the cerebral cortex—layer V PT-type pyramidal neurons—provides the primary afferent input to the cerebellar cortex (i.e., granule cells), by way of the pontine nuclei (D’Angelo & Casali, 2013; Kratochwil et al., 2017; Shine, 2020). Following a massive dimensionality expansion that has been argued to facilitate pattern separation (Cayco-Gajic & Silver, 2019), the outputs of the cerebellum (the deep cerebellar nuclei) send large glutamatergic projections to the ventral tier of the thalamus (Prevosto & Sommer, 2013), wherein they innervate the cerebral cortex. The thalamic targets of the cerebellum then go on to drive activity in the cerebral cortex, typically in a high-frequency, precise fashion (Nashef et al., 2022) that we have argued form the basis of relatively automatic modes of behaviour (Shine, 2020; Shine & Shine, 2014). Here, we extend these functional neuroanatomical principles to incorporate the completion of challenging dual tasks, thus augmenting and reinforcing conclusions from previous functional neuroimaging work on dual-task performance (Balsters & Ramnani, 2011; Shine & Poldrack, 2017; Wu et al., 2013). We anticipate that similar patterns will be observed in future experiments that interrogate different types of dual tasks, particularly those in which one (or both) of the tasks is capable of relative automaticity. Whether such automaticity benefits extend to purely perceptual tasks, such as the attentional blink (Sergent & Dehaene, 2004), is an interesting open question for future work. The topological signature of functional networks estimated from BOLD data have previ- ously been linked to effective performance on cognitive tasks. For instance, an integrated brain has been linked to the completion of a range of complex tasks, such as those that probe work- ing memory (Cruzat et al., 2018; Fransson et al., 2018; Shine et al., 2016), logical reasoning (Hearne et al., 2017), and attentional tracking (Mäki-Marttunen, 2021; Wainstein et al., 2021). In contrast, a relatively segregated functional network has been linked to relative sensorimotor automaticity (Bassett et al., 2015; Mohr et al., 2016), as well as to attentional vigilance (Sadaghiani et al., 2015). Our results are consistent with the spectrum implied by these pre- vious results—the balance task, which presumably tapped into relatively well-learned behav- iours, was associated with a segregated functional network; and the calculation task, which likely required more focussed, flexible attention, was associated with a relatively integrated network. Interestingly, although the dual-task trials were arguably more challenging than the calculation trials on their own, the topology of the network actually demonstrated a balance between integration and segregation, suggesting that performing tasks in parallel requires an ability to avoid topological extremes, perhaps so as to maximise information-processing capa- bilities (Sporns, 2013). In addition, there are theoretical reasons to believe that the finite nature of biological networks may imbue specific limits on the number of possible tasks that can be run in parallel, although we expect that the high-dimensional architecture of the cerebellum (Cayco-Gajic & Silver, 2019) will likely boost this capacity, particularly as a function of expe- rience (Shine, 2020; Shine & Shine, 2014). Precisely which systems in the brain help to control this balance remains an open question; however, there are intriguing results that suggest that the neuromodulatory system may play a crucial role in this process (Breton-Provencher et al., 2022; Shine, 2020; Shine et al., 2021). Systems-level neuroimaging analysis provides an integrated perspective of cognitive capac- ities; however, BOLD dynamics are necessarily indirect, that is, they don’t measure neural activity directly, but rather filtered through the low-dimensional lens of perfusion (Aquino et al., 2014; Pang et al., 2016). While the BOLD signal remains a robust measurement for neural signalling (Attwell & Iadecola, 2002; Moore & Cao, 2008), it only reveals a part of Network Neuroscience 853 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking how the brain functions. This is particularly true for the cerebellar cortex, whose complex, convoluted anatomy (Caligiore et al., 2016; D’Angelo & Casali, 2013) and idiosyncratic firing properties (Khilkevich et al., 2018; Kostadinov et al., 2019; Person & Raman, 2011) render simple, linear readouts of neural activity from BOLD problematic. Specifically, there is evi- dence to suggest that BOLD measurements in the cerebellar cortex predominantly track activ- ity in the mossy fibre pathway (via the CPC; Caesar et al., 2003; Mathiesen et al., 2000), whereas outputs from the Purkinje cells (via the CTC) are more difficult to characterize with BOLD signalling (Diedrichsen et al., 2019; Thomsen et al., 2009). While this does suggest caution with respect to the interpretation of our results, it makes the presence of robust cerebello-cortical activity flow mapping via the CTC (Figure 6D) all the more fascinating of a result, as it suggests that the fate of the Purkinje cells is relatively sealed by the specific pat- tern of mossy fibre inputs that they received, although we anticipate that this mapping is likely augmented by the process of learning, that is, it should be less profound when facing highly novel task contexts. Irrespectively, we hope that by consolidating analysis from multiple neu- roimaging techniques, we have provided a robust illustration of changes to cortico-cerebellar circuits during a parallel processing task. The capacity to perform tasks in parallel clearly scales positively with experience. In the future, it will be fascinating to examine the interactions between the cerebral cortex and cer- ebellum as individuals learn to perform individual tasks to relative automaticity. There is robust empirical previous work linking cerebellar output with highly overtrained behaviours in rodents (Callu et al., 2013). Similar arguments have been made when analysing automaticity in the performance of challenging cognitive tasks (Balsters & Ramnani, 2011). Interestingly, there is also evidence suggesting that, over the course of learning a simple sensorimotor task, the brain shifts from a relatively integrated to a segregated architecture (Bassett et al., 2015; Mohr et al., 2016). This suggests a novel prediction: the extent to which a particular task has been well learned will lead to relative segregation of the topological network signature of the brain, which in turn will make the task easier to automatise, and hence to combine success- fully with other, more challenging dual tasks. One factor that was not well controlled in this study was cognitive load, which is known to play an important role in our capacity to perform multiple tasks in parallel (Just et al., 2001; Michael et al., 2001; Whelan, 2007). Simply put, it is much easier to perform two tasks simul- taneously if (at least) one of the tasks is either highly automatic or is sufficiently easy that its performance requires little to no focussed attention (Fischer & Plessow, 2015). In these cases, the simpler or more automatic task can be performed with minimal awareness, freeing up higher cognitive systems to aid in the completion of the second, harder/more challenging task. In our study, the balance task was presumed to be easier than the calculation task, as partic- ipants were expected to have been unlikely to have practiced the subtraction of the digit “7” from random large numbers, whereas balance is something many of us perform so much as to take it for granted. In future studies, it will be important to attempt to stack together multiple tasks that are difficult to perform in the same manner, such as comprehending an auditory stream while performing a calculation on concurrent visual input. Although we anticipate that both the cerebellum and cerebral cortex would be engaged in such a task, it is less likely that effective performance would be as crucially dependent on their interaction, as the mechanism we propose invokes the cerebellar-mediated anticipation of expected consequences as a means for freeing up higher cognitive resources (Ramnani, 2014; Shine, 2020; Shine & Shine, 2014). It is not currently clear whether these anticipatory processes are as important in the more deliberate, flexible stages of cognitive processing that would be required to complete two more deliberate cognitive tasks simultaneously. Network Neuroscience 854 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking Here, we have demonstrated that dynamic interactions between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum are critically related to the performance of a challenging dual task. Future research is required to determine whether similar principals are related to parallel processing of other simultaneous cognitive and perceptual challenges, as well as across distinct spatiotemporal scales. METHODS Experimental Setup The functional data from this study arose from a re-analysis of a previously published dataset (Papegaaij et al., 2017); here, we will include the minimal information required to interpret the results, and point the interested reader to the original study for full details. 50 healthy female par- ticipants (mean age = 49 ± 20 years; Papegaaij et al., 2017) lay supine in the MRI scanner with their feet against a custom-made force platform attached to the MRI bed (Figure 1A; sample frequency of 100 Hz), with the position of the force platform was adjusted to subject height. To minimize excessive head movement, participants were pulled towards the force platform using thick elastic ropes attached to a hip belt (Papegaaij et al., 2017). A four-button device was placed underneath the right hand for the calculation task. The tasks were projected onto a white screen placed at the head of the scanner. Participants could see the screen via a mirror attached to the head coil. During the balance task, an avatar in the shape of a woman was displayed on the screen. The avatar swayed forward and backward. Participants were instructed to try to keep the avatar in the upright position by increasing or decreasing the level of plantar flexion force measured by the load cell. As in normal standing, increasing the plantar flexion force led to a backward sway (and v.v.). At the start of every balance condition, participants were given 2 seconds to bring the avatar in the upright position. After these 2 seconds, a disturbance signal was added, causing the avatar to sway forward and backward. To keep the avatar upright, participants had to counteract these disturbances. The disturbance signal was made by combining 15 sinusoidal signals with random phases and with frequency characteristics based on an average frequency spectrum of centre of pressure movement during upright standing (0.025–1 Hz), measured in 10 young and 10 old adults. The maximum amplitude of the disturbance was ±30°. The error for each balance trial was created by calculating the sum of the root-mean-squared error between the optimal balanced avatar (i.e., 900) and the position of the actual avatar. Trials were subsequently median split to identify ‘good’ and ‘bad’ balance trials. The calculation task consisted of serial subtractions with increments of seven—at the begin- ning of each trial, a number between 50 and 100 was projected on the screen for 2 seconds, after which a plus sign was displayed on the screen and a beep was generated every 3 to 4 seconds through an MRI compatible headphone (MR confon Optime 1, Magdeburg, Germany), with a total of four beeps per trial. Participants were instructed to subtract the num- ber 7 with every beep. At the end of each trial, four answer possibilities were displayed on the screen: one indicating the correct answer, two erroneous answers, and the option that none of the other answers is correct. Participants indicated which answer they thought was correct by pressing the corresponding button of the four-button device. During the dual-task condition, subjects performed the balance and calculation tasks simultaneously. The distribution of RMS errors in the balance trials and dual-task trials were compared using a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. An fMRI block design was used to alternate between the three conditions: balance, calcu- lation, and dual task. Every participant performed 12 blocks, each block including one trial of Network Neuroscience 855 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 7 2 8 4 4 2 1 1 8 5 2 7 n e n _ a _ 0 0 3 0 8 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking each condition (thus three trials), with the order of the conditions randomized, both across blocks and between subjects. At the end of every block a 15-second rest period was given in which the participants fixated their gaze on a plus sign. MRI Acquisition and Preprocessing Brain imaging was performed on a 3-T SIEMENS Magnetom Skyra System (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 20-channel head/neck coil. For functional scans, a T2*-weighted multiband gradient echo-planar imaging sequence was used (TR = 700 msec, TE = 30 msec, flip angle = 55°, 48 axial slices, slice thickness = 3 mm, no gap, in-plane resolution 3 × 3 mm) (Feinberg et al., 2010). After the functional scanning session, a high-resolution magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) T1-weighted sequence (TR = 2,100 msec, TE = 4.6 msec, TI = 900 msec, flip angle = 8°, 192 contiguous slices, voxel resolution 1 mm3, FOV = 256 × 256 × 192 mm, iPAT factor of 2) was obtained in sagittal orientation. These ana- tomical scans were used to coregister the functional runs using SPM 12. The anatomical scan was segmented using the SPM tissue probability maps. The functional data were preprocessed as part of a different study (Papegaaij et al., 2017). For each subject, interscan movement was corrected by realigning and unwarping the data, with the first scan as a reference. All func- tional scans were then coregistered to the anatomical scan and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template brain via the forward deformations revealed by the structural segmentation. Movement in the scanner was assessed by calculating framewise dis- placement (FD) from the derivatives of the six rigid body realignment parameters estimated during standard volume realignment, as well as the root-mean-square change in BOLD signal from volume to volume (aka DVARS). Across the cohort, head motion was found to be minimal (group mean FD = 0.183 ± 0.08 mm; group mean DVARS = 0.811 ± 0.13). Temporal artifacts were identified in each dataset by calculating FD from the derivatives of the six rigid body realignment parameters estimated during standard volume realignment (Power et al., 2014), as well as the root-mean-square change in BOLD signal from volume to volume (DVARS). Frames associated with FD > 0.25 mm or DVARS > 2.5% were identified;
cependant, as no participants were identified with greater than 10% of the resting time points
exceeding these values, no trials were excluded from further analysis.

Brain Parcellation

Following preprocessing, the mean time series was extracted from 400 predefined cortical par-
cels using the Schaefer atlas (Schaefer et al., 2018) et 28 predefined cerebellar parcels from
the SUIT atlas (Diedrichsen, 2006) (cerebellar nuclei were not included). The mean BOLD
signal intensity from each region was extracted and then used for subsequent analyses.

General Linear Model and Principal Component Analysis

A general linear model was fit to preprocessed, parcellated BOLD data with separate terms
modelling each trial type (c'est à dire., équilibre, calculation, and dual task). The event time series used
to analyse the task included a convolution with a canonical haemodynamic response function.
The proportion of cerebellar regions associated with positive cerebellar β-values was com-
pared across balance, calculation and dual-task trials using a χ2 test with degrees of freedom =
(rows – 1) × (columns – 1) = (3 – 1) × (2 – 1) = 2.

The average β-value for the balance and calculation trials were demeaned and analysed
with a principal component analysis. The coefficient of the leading principal component
was correlated with the mean β map from the balance and calculation trials to demonstrate

Neurosciences en réseau

856

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

t

/

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

t

.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

its utility as a linear decoder between balance and calculation. The dot product between the
dual-task β map for each subject and the leading principal component was calculated, et
then subjected to a one-sample t test to determine whether the loading was more similar to
calculation (positive loadings) or balance (negative loadings).

Time-Varying Functional Connectivity

We used the multiplication of temporal derivatives (MTD) approche (Shine et al., 2015) à
calculate time-resolved dynamic functional connectivity between the selected ROIs; code is
freely available at https://github.com/macshine/coupling/ with a window size of 20 TRs (résultats
were stable for window sizes of 10–50 TR). For each node, n, with time points, t, a vector of
t − 1 temporal derivatives was calculated and normalized (temporal derivatives divided by the
standard deviation of temporal derivatives, p). Alors, we created a matrix of functional cou-
pling between the ith and jth nodes for each time point, by multiplying the temporal deriva-
tives of each pair of nodes across each time point.
(cid:2)
(cid:2)

(cid:3)
(cid:3)

X

tþw

1

(1)

MTDijt ¼

w

t

dtit (cid:2) dtjt
(cid:2) σdtj
σdti

where dt is the first temporal derivative of the i th and j th time series, and σ standard deviation
of the temporal derivative, w is the window length of the simple moving average (Shine et al.,
2015). The MTD values for the cortico-cerebellar system (c'est à dire., 400 × 28 = 11,200 edges) étaient
entered into a similar general linear model to the cortico-cerebellar BOLD values, with a per-
mutation test (5,000 iterations) used to test for statistical significance.

Modularity Maximization

The Louvain modularity algorithm from the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (BCT; Rubinov &
Sporns, 2010; https://www.brain-connectivity-toolbox.net) was used on the neural network
edge weights to estimate community structure. The Louvain algorithm iteratively maximizes
the modularity statistic, Q, for different community assignments until the maximum possible
score of Q has been obtained (see Equation 2). The modularity of a given network is therefore
a quantification of the extent to which the network may be subdivided into communities with
stronger within-module than between-module connections.
(cid:4)
X
w þ
ij

(cid:5)
δMi Mj

(cid:5)
δMi Mj

QT ¼

− eþ
ij

− e−
ij

w −
ij

X

(2)

(cid:4)

1
υþ þ υ−

1
υþ

ij

ij

where v is the total weight of the network (sum of all negative and positive connections), wij is
the weighted and signed connection between regions i and j, eij is the strength of a connection
divided by the total weight of the network, and δMiMj
is set to 1 when regions are in the same
community and 0 otherwise; + and − superscripts denote all positive and negative connec-
tion, respectivement.

For each epoch, we assessed the community assignment for each region 500 times and a
consensus partition was identified using a fine-tuning algorithm (BCT). We calculated all graph
theoretical measures on unthresholded, weighted, and signed connectivity matrices (Rubinov
& Sporns, 2010). The stability of the γ parameter was estimated by iteratively calculating the
modularity across a range of γ values (0.5–2.5; mean Pearson’s r = 0.859 ± 0.01) on the
time-averaged connectivity matrix for each subject—across iterations and subjects, a γ value
de 1.0 was found to be the least variable, and hence was used for the resultant topological
analyses.

Neurosciences en réseau

857

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

t

/

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

t

.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

Participation Coefficient

The participation coefficient, PC, quantifies the extent to which a region connects across all
modules (c'est à dire., between-module strength) and has previously been used to successfully charac-
terize hubs within brain networks (Shine et al., 2016, 2019). The PC for each region was
calculated within each temporal window using Equation 3, where kisT is the strength of the
positive connections of region i to regions in module s at time T, and kiT is the sum of strengths
of all positive connections of region i at time T. Negative connections were discarded prior to
calculation. The PC of a region is therefore close to 1 if its connections are uniformly distrib-
uted among all the modules and 0 if all of its links are within its own module.

PC ¼ 1 −

X

nM
s¼1

(cid:6) (cid:7)2
κisT
κiT

(3)

The PC for each parcel was compared across balance, calculation and dual-task trials using

paired t tests (FDR q = 0.05).

Diffusion MRI Analysis
Data were selected from a single 26–30-year-old female subject from the HCP (code: 100307).
The minimally processed HCP diffusion dataset (which included correction for motion,
susceptibility distortions, gradient nonlinearity and eddy currents) were subject to additional
image processing, which multishell multitissue constrained spherical deconvolution to gener-
ate the fibre orientation distribution (FOD) in each voxel (Jeurissen et al., 2014; Tournier et al.,
2004, 2007). These steps were implemented in accordance with previous work (Civier et al.,
2019) and were performed using the MRtrix software package (https://www.mrtrix.org;
Tournier et al., 2012, 2019).

The T1-weighted images were used to generate a so-called ‘five-tissue-type’ (5TT) image
(R.. E. Smith et al., 2012) using FSL (S. M.. Smith et al., 2004); the 5TT image classifies the voxel
into one of five tissue types: cortical grey matter, subcortical grey matter, white matter, cere-
brospinal fluid, and ‘5th type’ (par exemple., pathology). The FOD data and the 5TT image were used to
generate 120 million streamlines using the anatomically constrained tractography framework
(R.. E. Smith et al., 2012), using dynamic and the second-order integration over fibre orientation
distributions (iFOD2; Tournier et al., 2012) probabilistic fibre-tracking algorithm, using default
MRtrix parameters, with the exception of FOD cutoff 0.06, maximum length 250 mm, step size
1 mm, and backtrack specified. This set of streamlines is referred to as the whole-brain-
tractogram thereafter.

Spécifiquement, we calculated from a highly curated tractography rendering of the cerebro-
cerebellar loop, after thresholding the streamlines to eliminate possible spurious tracts. Un
average tract obtained from 5 à 10 à 28 sujets, thresholded to represent the group, may
lose the finer details of the connectome that are key when using a ∼400 region grey matter
parcellation atlas as in this work. On the other hand, the connectome from the union of the
tracts, if not thresholded, would inflate this finer connectivity (if thresholded, these connec-
tions would be downweighted). The impact of averaging individual subject streamlines on
the actual connectome has to be demonstrated in a separate study, as the high variability of
the streamlines is likely to correspond to a fairly stable connectome.

The cerebello-thalamo-cortical (CTC) and cortico-ponto-cerebellar (CPC) tracts were
extracted from the whole-brain tractogram by using contralateral cerebral and cerebellar
cortices, cerebellar peduncles, contralateral red nuclei, and thalami as regions of interest

Neurosciences en réseau

858

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

/

t

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

.

t

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

(for more details, see Palesi et al., 2015, 2017). To define the strength of the cerebellar con-
nectivity with each of brain parcel, the log10 of the number of streamlines was used to weight
the CTC and CPC tracts (Abos et al., 2019; Palesi et al., 2021). To ensure that the single-subject
connectome was representative of the group-level parcellation, we calculated the DICE
coefficient between the mean map of both the CTC and CPC tracts (L and R) of a further 27
subjects in MNI space (including only those voxels that were common to at least 70% of sub-
projets, c'est, less than the 90,000,000 streamlines used for the individual connectome); le
DICE was 0.7, suggesting strong correspondence between our single subject (who preserved
the fine-scale nature of the connectome) and the group template.

Cortico-Cerebellar Activity Flow Mapping

To determine whether cortico-cerebellar interactions could transform cortical or cerebellar
task-evoked activity into respective cerebello-cortical task activity, we modified the activity
flow mapping procedure (Cole et al., 2016) to incorporate estimates of cortico-cerebellar
(CPC) and cerebello-cortical (CTC) structural connectivity. Spécifiquement, for each trial type,
block and subject, we calculated:

ACTX ¼ W t

CBM

⋅ CPC

ACBM ¼ W t

CTX

⋅ CTC

(4)

(5)

where Wt is the evoked response estimate for every cortical (WCTX) or cerebellar (WCBM)
parcel, CPC and CTC are the structural connectivity matrices described above, and ACTX
and ACBM are the predicted activity pattern for each subgroup. For each trial type, block
and subject, the predicted cortical and cerebellar activity patterns were then empirically com-
pared to the observed activity patterns using Pearson correlations. A series of t tests were used
to compare the Pearson’s correlation loadings, with the nonmatching predictions (par exemple., en utilisant
the cortical BOLD for balance trials to predict cerebellar BOLD for calculation trials) used a
simple null model that contained all the same spectral features but spatiotemporal sequences
that did not match the data. Enfin, we created separate null distributions following a random
permutation (Nichols & Holmes, 2002) of both CPC and CTC, separately (each with 5,000
iterations).

REMERCIEMENTS

Data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (PI: David Van
Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that
support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for
Systems Neuroscience, Washington University.

INFORMATIONS À L'APPUI

Supporting information for this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00308.

CONTRIBUTIONS DES AUTEURS

Eli J. Müller: Conceptualisation; Enquête; Méthodologie; Rédaction – révision & édition.
Fulvia Palesi: Analyse formelle; Méthodologie; Rédaction – révision & édition. Kevin Y. Hou: Formal
analyse; Méthodologie. Joshua Tan: Rédaction – révision & édition. Thomas Close: En écrivant -
revoir & édition. Claudia A. M.. Gandini Wheeler-Kingschott: Enquête; Rédaction – révision

Neurosciences en réseau

859

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

t

/

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

t

.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

& édition. Egidio D’Angelo: Enquête; Rédaction – révision & édition. Fernando Calamante:
Analyse formelle; Enquête; Méthodologie; Rédaction – révision & édition. James Shine: Con-
ceptualization; Conservation des données; Analyse formelle; Enquête; Méthodologie; Project adminis-
tration; Ressources; Logiciel; Surveillance; Validation; Visualisation; Rédaction – ébauche originale;
Rédaction – révision & édition.

INFORMATIONS SUR LE FINANCEMENT

James Shine, National Health and Medical Research Council (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039
/501100000925), Award ID: 1193857.

RÉFÉRENCES

Abos, UN., Baggio, H. C., Segura, B., Campabadal, UN., Uribe, C.,
Giraldo, D. M., Perez-Soriano, UN., Muñoz, E., Compta, Y.,
Junque, C., & Marti, M.. J.. (2019). Differentiation of multiple sys-
tem atrophy from Parkinson’s disease by structural connectivity
derived from probabilistic tractography. Rapports scientifiques, 9(1),
16488. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-52829-8, PubMed:
31712681

Alahmadi, UN. UN. S., Samson, R.. S., Gasston, D., Pardini, M., Friston,
K. J., D’Angelo, E., Toosy, UN. T., & Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M..
(2016). Complex motor task associated with non-linear BOLD
responses in cerebro-cortical areas and cerebellum. Brain Struc-
ture and Function, 221(5), 2443–2458. https://doi.org/10.1007
/s00429-015-1048-1, PubMed: 25921976

Aquino, K. M., Robinson, P.. UN., & Drysdale, P.. M.. (2014). Spatio-
temporal hemodynamic response functions derived from physi-
ology. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 347, 118–136. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.12.027, PubMed: 24398024

Attwell, D., & Iadecola, C. (2002). The neural basis of functional
brain imaging signals. Trends in Neurosciences, 25(12), 621–625.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02264-6, PubMed:
12446129

Balsters, J.. H., & Ramnani, N. (2011). Cerebellar plasticity and the
automation of first-order rules. Journal des neurosciences, 31(6),
2305–2312. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4358-10.2011,
PubMed: 21307266

Bar-Gad, JE., Morris, G., & Bergman, H. (2003). Information pro-
cessation, dimensionality reduction and reinforcement learning in
the basal ganglia. Progrès en neurobiologie, 71(6), 439–473.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2003.12.001, PubMed:
15013228

Bassett, D. S., Lequel, M., Wymbs, N. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2015).
Learning-induced autonomy of sensorimotor systems. Nature
Neurosciences, 18(5), 744–751. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3993,
PubMed: 25849989

Breton-Provencher, V., Drummond, G., Feng, J., Li, Y., & Sur, M..
(2022). Spatiotemporal dynamics of norepinephrine during
learned behavior. Nature, 606(7915), 732–738. https://doi.org
/10.1038/s41586-022-04782-2, PubMed: 35650441

Caesar, K., Gold, L., & Lauritzen, M.. (2003). Context sensitivity of
activity-dependent increases in cerebral blood flow. Procédure
of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(7), 4239–4244. https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0635075100, PubMed: 12655065

Caligiore, D., Pezzulo, G., Baldassarre, G., Bostan, UN. C.,
Strick, P.. L., Doya, K., Helmich, R.. C., Dirkx, M., Houk, J.,
Jörntell, H., Lago-Rodriguez, UN., Galea, J.. M., Miall, R.. C.,
Popa, T., Kishore, UN., Verschure, P.. F. M..
J., Zucca, R., &
(2016). Consensus paper: Towards a systems-
Herreros,
level view of cerebellar
fonction: The interplay between
cerebellum, basal ganglia, and cortex. The Cerebellum, 16(1),
203–229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-016-0763-3, PubMed:
26873754

je.

Callu, D., Lopez, J., & El Massioui, N. (2013). Cerebellar deep
nuclei involvement in cognitive adaptation and automaticity.
Apprentissage & Mémoire, 20(7), 344–347. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm
.030536.113, PubMed: 23772087

Casiraghi, L., Alahmadi, UN. UN. S., Monteverdi, UN., Palesi, F., Castellazzi,
G., Savini, G., Friston, K., Gandini Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M.,
& D’Angelo, E. (2019). I see your effort: Force-related BOLD
effects in an extended action execution–observation network
involving the cerebellum. Cortex cérébral, 29(3), 1351–1368.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy322, PubMed: 30615116
Castellazzi, G., Bruno, S. D., Toosy, UN. T., Casiraghi, L., Palesi, F.,
Savini, G., D’Angelo, E., & Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M.. G.
(2018). Prominent changes in cerebro-cerebellar functional
connectivity during continuous cognitive processing. Frontiers
in Cellular Neuroscience, 12, 331. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel
.2018.00331, PubMed: 30327590

Castellazzi, G., Palesi, F., Casali, S., Vitali, P., Sinforiani, E.,
Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M., & D’Angelo, E. (2014). A compre-
hensive assessment of resting state networks: Bidirectional
modification of functional integrity in cerebro-cerebellar net-
works in dementia. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 223. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00223, PubMed: 25126054

Cayco-Gajic, N. UN., & Silver, R.. UN. (2019). Re-evaluating circuit
mechanisms underlying pattern separation. Neurone, 101(4),
584–602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.044,
PubMed: 30790539

Civier, O., Forgeron, R.. E., Yeh, C.-H., Connelly, UN., & Calamante, F.
(2019). Is removal of weak connections necessary for graph-
theoretical analysis of dense weighted structural connectomes
from diffusion MRI? NeuroImage, 194, 68–81. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.039, PubMed: 30844506

Cole, M.. W., Ito, T., Bassett, D. S., & Schultz, D. H. (2016). Activity
flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task

Neurosciences en réseau

860

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

t

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

.

t

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

activations. Neurosciences naturelles, 19(12), 1718–1726. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1038/nn.4406, PubMed: 27723746

Cruzat, J., Deco, G., Tauste-Campo, UN., Principe, UN., Costa, UN.,
Kringelbach, M.. L., & Rocamora, R.. (2018). The dynamics of
human cognition: Increasing global integration coupled with
decreasing segregation found using iEEG. NeuroImage, 172,
492–505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.064,
PubMed: 29425897

D’Angelo, E. (2019). The cerebellum gets social. Science, 363(6424),
229–229. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw2571, PubMed:
30655429

D’Angelo, E., & Casali, S. (2013). Seeking a unified framework for
cerebellar function and dysfunction: From circuit operations to
cognition. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 6, 116. https://est ce que je.org/10
.3389/fncir.2012.00116, PubMed: 23335884

D’Angelo, E., Mapelli, L., Casellato, C., Garrido, J.. UN., Luque, N.,
Monaco, J., Prestori, F., Pedrocchi, UN., & Ros, E. (2016). Distrib-
uted circuit plasticity: New clues for the cerebellar mechanisms
of learning. The Cerebellum, 15(2), 139–151. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1007/s12311-015-0711-7, PubMed: 26304953

Diedrichsen, J.. (2006). A spatially unbiased atlas template of the
human cerebellum. NeuroImage, 33(1), 127–138. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056, PubMed: 16904911
Diedrichsen, J., King, M., Hernandez-Castillo, C., Séréno, M., &
Ivry, R.. B. (2019). Universal transform or multiple functionality?
Understanding the contribution of the human cerebellum across
task domains. Neurone, 102(5), 918–928. https://est ce que je.org/10.1016
/j.neuron.2019.04.021, PubMed: 31170400

Eisenreich, B. R., Akaishi, R., & Hayden, B. Oui. (2017). Control with-
out controllers: Toward a distributed neuroscience of executive
control. Journal des neurosciences cognitives, 29(10), 1684–1698.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01139, PubMed: 28430042

Feinberg, D. UN., Moeller, S., Forgeron, S. M., Auerbach, E., Ramanna,
S., Glasser, M.. F., Miller, K. L., Voiture d'Ugur, K., & Yacoub, E. (2010).
Multiplexed echo planar imaging for sub-second whole brain
FMRI and fast diffusion imaging. PLoS One, 5(12), e15710.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015710, PubMed:
21187930

Fischer, R., & Plessow, F. (2015). Efficient multitasking: Parallel
versus serial processing of multiple tasks. Frontiers in Psychology,
6, 1366. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01366, PubMed:
26441742

Fransson, P., Schiffler, B. C., & Thompson, W. H. (2018). Brain net-
work segregation and integration during an epoch-related working
memory fMRI experiment. NeuroImage, 178, 147–161. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.040, PubMed: 29777824
Hearne, L. J., Cocchi, L., Zalesky, UN., & Mattingley, J.. B. (2017).
Reconfiguration of brain network architectures between
resting-state and complexity-dependent cognitive reasoning.
Journal des neurosciences, 37(35), 8399–8411. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1523/JNEUROSCI.0485-17.2017, PubMed: 28760864

Ilg, W., Christensen, UN., Mueller, Ô. M., Goericke, S. L., Giese,
M.. UN., & Timmann, D. (2013). Effects of cerebellar lesions on
working memory interacting with motor tasks of different com-
plexities. Journal de neurophysiologie, 110(10), 2337–2349.
https://est ce que je.org/10.1152/jn.00062.2013, PubMed: 23966680

Jeurissen, B., Tournier, J.-D., Dhollander, T., Connelly, UN., & Sijbers,
J.. (2014). Multi-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution for

improved analysis of multi-shell diffusion MRI data. NeuroImage,
103, 411–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07
.061, PubMed: 25109526

John, Oui. J., Sawyer, K. S., Srinivasan, K., Müller, E. J., Munn, B. R., &
Shine, J.. M.. (2022). It’s about time: Linking dynamical systems
with human neuroimaging to understand the brain. Réseau
Neurosciences, 6(4), 960–979. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a
_00230, PubMed: 36875012

Just, M.. UN., Carpenter, P.. UN., Keller, T. UN., Emery, L., Zajac, H., &
Thulborn, K. R.. (2001). Interdependence of nonoverlapping cortical
systems in dual cognitive tasks. NeuroImage, 14(2), 417–426.
https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0826, PubMed: 11467915
Khilkevich, UN., Zambrano, J., Richards, M.-M., & Mauk, M.. D.
(2018). Cerebellar implementation of movement sequences
through feedback. eLife, 7, e06262. https://doi.org/10.7554
/eLife.37443, PubMed: 30063004

Kostadinov, D., Beau, M., Blanco-Pozo, M., & Häusser, M.. (2019).
Predictive and reactive reward signals conveyed by climbing fiber
inputs to cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neurosciences naturelles, 22(6),
950–962. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0381-8, PubMed:
31036947

Kratochwil, C. F., Maheshwari, U., & Rijli, F. M.. (2017). The long
journey of pontine nuclei neurons: From rhombic lip to cortico-
ponto-cerebellar circuitry. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 11, 33.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2017.00033, PubMed: 28567005
Kuramoto, E., Furuta, T., Nakamura, K. C., Unzai, T., Hioki, H., &
Kaneko, T. (2009). Two types of thalamocortical projections from
the motor thalamic nuclei of the rat: A single neuron-tracing
study using viral vectors. Cortex cérébral, 19(9), 2065–2077.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn231, PubMed: 19174446
Mäki-Marttunen, V. (2021). Pupil-based states of brain integration
across cognitive states. Neurosciences, 471, 61–71. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.07.016, PubMed: 34303781
Mathiesen, C., Caesar, K., & Lauritzen, M.. (2000). Temporal cou-
pling between neuronal activity and blood flow in rat cerebellar
cortex as indicated by field potential analysis. Journal of Phys-
iology, 523(1), 235–246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793
.2000.t01-1-00235.x, PubMed: 10673558

Michael, E. B., Keller, T. UN., Carpenter, P.. UN., & Just, M.. UN. (2001).
fMRI investigation of sentence comprehension by eye and by
ear: Modality fingerprints on cognitive processes. Cerveau humain
Cartographie, 13(4), 239–252. https://doi.org/10.1002/ hbm.1036,
PubMed: 11410952

Mohr, H., Wolfensteller, U., Betzel, R.. F., Mišić, B., Sporns, O.,
Richiardi, J., & Ruge, H. (2016). Integration and segregation of
large-scale brain networks during short-term task automatization.
Communications naturelles, 7, 13217. https://est ce que je.org/10.1038
/ncomms13217, PubMed: 27808095

Moore, C. JE., & Cao, R.. (2008). The hemo-neural hypothesis: Sur
the role of blood flow in information processing. Journal of Neu-
rophysiology, 99(5), 2035–2047. https://est ce que je.org/10.1152/jn
.01366.2006, PubMed: 17913979

Nashef, UN., Cohen, O., Perlmutter, S. JE., & Prut, Oui. (2022). A cere-
bellar origin of feedforward inhibition to the motor cortex in
non-human primates. Cell Reports, 39(6), 110803. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2022.110803, PubMed: 35545040

Nichols, T. E., & Holmes, UN. P.. (2002). Nonparametric permutation
tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples.

Neurosciences en réseau

861

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

/

t

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

.

t

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

Human Brain Mapping, 15(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1002
/hbm.1058, PubMed: 11747097

Palesi, F., De Rinaldis, UN., Castellazzi, G., Calamante, F., Muhlert,
N., Chard, D., Tournier, J.. D., Magenes, G., D’Angelo, E., &
Gandini Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M.. (2017). Contralateral
cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathways reconstruction in humans in
vivo: Implications for reciprocal cerebro-cerebellar structural
connectivity in motor and non-motor areas. Rapports scientifiques,
7(1), 12841. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13079-8,
PubMed: 28993670

Palesi, F., Ferrante, M., Gaviraghi, M., Misiti, UN., Savini, G., Lascialfari,
UN., D’Angelo, E., & Gandini Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M.. (2021).
Motor and higher-order functions topography of the human
dentate nuclei identified with tractography and clustering
méthodes. Human Brain Mapping, 42(13), 4348–4361. https://
doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25551, PubMed: 34087040

Palesi, F., Lorenzi, R.. M., Casellato, C., Ritter, P., Jirsa, V., Gandini
Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M., & D’Angelo, E. (2020). The impor-
tance of cerebellar connectivity on simulated brain dynamics.
Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 14, 240. https://est ce que je.org/10
.3389/fncel.2020.00240, PubMed: 32848628

Palesi, F., Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., Muhlert, N., Castellazzi,
G., Chard, D., D’Angelo, E., & Wheeler-Kingshott, C. UN. M..
(2015). Contralateral cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathways with
prominent involvement of associative areas in humans in vivo.
Structure et fonction du cerveau, 220(6), 3369–3384. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0861-2, PubMed: 25134682

Pang, J.. C., Robinson, P.. UN., & Aquino, K. M.. (2016). Response-
mode decomposition of spatio-temporal haemodynamics. Jour-
nal of The Royal Society Interface, 13(118), 20160253. https://
doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2016.0253, PubMed: 27170653

Papegaaij, S., Hortobágyi, T., Godde, B., Kaan, W. UN., Erhard, P., &
Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2017). Neural correlates of motor-cognitive
dual-tasking in young and old adults. PLoS One, 12(12),
e0189025. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189025,
PubMed: 29220349

Person, UN. L., & Raman, je. M.. (2011). Purkinje neuron synchrony
elicits time-locked spiking in the cerebellar nuclei. Nature,
481(7382), 502–505. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10732,
PubMed: 22198670

Petri, G., Musslick, S., Dey, B., Özcimder, K., Tourneur, D., Ahmed,
N. K., Willke, T. L., & Cohen, J.. D. (2021). Topological limits to
the parallel processing capability of network architectures. Nature
Physics, 17, 646–651. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41567-021-01170-x
Pezzulo, G., & Cisek, P.. (2016). Navigating the affordance land-
scape: Feedback control as a process model of behavior and cog-
nition. Tendances des sciences cognitives, 20(6), 414–424. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.013, PubMed: 27118642

Poldrack, R.. UN. (2012). The future of fMRI in cognitive neurosci-
ence. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1216–1220. https://est ce que je.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2011.08.007, PubMed: 21856431

Poldrack, R.. UN., Kittur, UN., Kalar, D., Miller, E., Seppa, C., Gil, Y.,
Parker, D. S., Sabb, F. W., & Bilder, R.. M.. (2011). The Cognitive
Atlas: Toward a knowledge foundation for cognitive neurosci-
ence. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 5, 17. https://est ce que je.org/10
.3389/fninf.2011.00017, PubMed: 21922006

Power, J.. D., Mitra, UN., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, UN. Z., Schlaggar, B. L.,
& Petersen, S. E. (2014). Methods to detect, characterize, et

remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 84,
320–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048,
PubMed: 23994314

Preuss, T. M., & Sage, S. P.. (2022). Evolution of prefrontal cortex.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(1), 3–19. https://est ce que je.org/10.1038
/s41386-021-01076-5, PubMed: 34363014

Prevosto, V., & Sommer, M.. UN. (2013). Cognitive control of move-
ment via the cerebellar-recipient thalamus. Frontiers in Systems
Neurosciences, 7, 56. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00056,
PubMed: 24101896

Ramnani, N. (2006). The primate cortico-cerebellar system:
Anatomy and function. Nature Revues Neurosciences, 7(7),
511–522. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1953, PubMed: 16791141
Ramnani, N. (2014). Automatic and controlled processing in the
corticocerebellar system. In Cerebellar learning (pp. 255–285).
Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier. https://est ce que je.org/10.1016
/B978-0-444-63356-9.00010-8, PubMed: 24916296

Rubinov, M., & Sporns, Ô. (2010). Complex network measures of
brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage, 52(3),
1059–1069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003,
PubMed: 19819337

Sadaghiani, S., Poline, J.-B., Kleinschmidt, UN., & D'Esposito, M..
(2015). Ongoing dynamics in large-scale functional connectivity
predict perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
ences of the United States of America, 112(27), 8463–8468.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1420687112, PubMed: 26106164
Schaefer, UN., Kong, R., Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Zuo, X.-N.,
Holmes, UN. J., Eickhoff, S. B., & Yeo, B. T. T. (2018). Local-global
parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic func-
tional connectivity MRI. Cortex cérébral, 28(9), 3095–3114.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179, PubMed: 28981612

Schmitz, T. W., & Duncan, J.. (2018). Normalization and the cholin-
ergic microcircuit: A unified basis for attention. Trends in Cogni-
tive Sciences, 22(5), 422–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018
.02.011, PubMed: 29576464

Sergent, C., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness a gradual phe-
nomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the
attentional blink. Sciences psychologiques, 15(11), 720–728. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00748.X, PubMed: 15482443
Shine, J.. M.. (2020). The thalamus integrates the macrosystems of
the brain to facilitate complex, adaptive brain network dynamics.
Progrès en neurobiologie, 199, 101951. https://est ce que je.org/10.1016/j
.pneurobio.2020.101951, PubMed: 33189781

Shine, J.. M., Bissett, P.. G., Cloche, P.. T., Koyejo, O., Balsters, J.. H.,
Gorgolewski, K. J., Moodie, C. UN., & Poldrack, R.. UN. (2016).
The dynamics of functional brain networks: Integrated network
states during cognitive task performance. Neurone, 92(2),
544–554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.018,
PubMed: 27693256

Shine, J.. M., Breakspear, M., Cloche, P.. T., Ehgoetz Martens, K. UN.,
Shine, R., Koyejo, O., Sporns, O., & Poldrack, R.. UN. (2019).
Human cognition involves the dynamic integration of neural
activity and neuromodulatory systems. Neurosciences naturelles,
22(2), 289–296. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0312-0,
PubMed: 30664771

Shine, J.. M., Koyejo, O., Cloche, P.. T., Gorgolewski, K. J., Gilat, M., &
Poldrack, R.. UN. (2015). Estimation of dynamic functional connec-
tivity using multiplication of temporal derivatives. NeuroImage,

Neurosciences en réseau

862

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

t

/

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

.

t

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Cortico-cerebellar dual tasking

122, 399–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07
.064, PubMed: 26231247

Shine, J.. M., Müller, E. J., Munn, B., Cabral, J., Moran, R.. J., &
Breakspear, M.. (2021). Computational models link cellular
mechanisms of neuromodulation to large-scale neural dynamics.
Neurosciences naturelles, 24(6), 765–776. https://est ce que je.org/10.1038
/s41593-021-00824-6, PubMed: 33958801

Shine, J.. M., & Poldrack, R.. UN. (2017). Principles of dynamic net-
work reconfiguration across diverse brain states. NeuroImage,
180, 396–405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08
.010, PubMed: 28782684

Shine, J.. M., & Shine, R.. (2014). Delegation to automaticity: Le
driving force for cognitive evolution? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8,
90. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00090, PubMed: 24808820
Forgeron, R.. E., Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., & Connelly, UN. (2012).
Anatomically-constrained tractography: Improved diffusion MRI
streamlines tractography through effective use of anatomical
information. NeuroImage, 62(3), 1924–1938. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.005, PubMed: 22705374

Forgeron, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M.. W., Beckmann, C. F.,
Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., Bannister, P.. R., De Luca,
M., Drobnjak, JE., Flitney, D. E., Niazy, R.. K., Saunders, J., Vickers,
J., Zhang, Y., De Stefano, N., Brady, J.. M., & Matthieu, P.. M..
(2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis
and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23(Supplément 1), S208–S219.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051, PubMed:
15501092

Sporns, Ô. (2013). Network attributes for segregation and integra-
tion in the human brain. Opinion actuelle en neurobiologie, 23(2),
162–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.015, PubMed:
23294553

Thomsen, K., Piilgaard, H., Gjedde, UN., Bonvento, G., & Lauritzen,
M.. (2009). Principal cell spiking, postsynaptic excitation, et
oxygen consumption in the rat cerebellar cortex. Journal de
Neurophysiologie, 102(3), 1503–1512. https://est ce que je.org/10.1152/jn
.00289.2009, PubMed: 19571198

Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., & Connelly, UN. (2007). Robust deter-
mination of the fibre orientation distribution in diffusion MRI:
Non-negativity constrained super-resolved spherical deconvolu-
tion. NeuroImage, 35(4), 1459–1472. https://est ce que je.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2007.02.016, PubMed: 17379540

Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., & Connelly, UN. (2012). MRtrix: Diffu-
sion tractography in crossing fiber regions. International Journal
of Imaging Systems and Technology, 22(1), 53–66. https://doi.org
/10.1002/ima.22005

Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., Gadian, D. G., & Connelly, UN.
(2004). Direct estimation of the fiber orientation density function
from diffusion-weighted MRI data using spherical deconvolution.
NeuroImage, 23(3), 1176–1185. https://est ce que je.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2004.07.037, PubMed: 15528117

Tournier, J.-D., Forgeron, R., Raffelt, D., Tabbara, R., Dhollander, T.,
Pietsch, M., Christiaens, D., Jeurissen, B., Yeh, C.-H., & Connelly,
UN. (2019). MRtrix3: A fast, flexible and open software framework
for medical image processing and visualisation. NeuroImage,
202, 116137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019
.116137, PubMed: 31473352

Wainstein, G., Rojas-Líbano, D., Medel, V., Alnæs, D., Kolskår,
K. K., Endestad, T., Laeng, B., Ossandon, T., Crossley, N., Matar,
E., & Shine, J.. M.. (2021). The ascending arousal system promotes
optimal performance through mesoscale network integration in a
visuospatial attentional task. Neurosciences en réseau, 5(4), 890–910.
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00205, PubMed: 35024535

Whelan, R.. R.. (2007). Neuroimaging of cognitive load in instruc-
tional multimedia. Educational Research Review, 2(1), 1–12.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2006.11.001

Wilson, C. J.. (2013). Active decorrelation in the basal ganglia. Neu-
roscience, 250, 467–482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience
.2013.07.032, PubMed: 23892007

Wu, T., Liu, J., Hallett, M., Zheng, Z., & Chan, P.. (2013). Cerebel-
lum and integration of neural networks in dual-task processing.
NeuroImage, 65, 466–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2012.10.004, PubMed: 23063842

Neurosciences en réseau

863

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

t

/

e
d
toi
n
e
n
un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

je

F
/

/

/

/

/

7
2
8
4
4
2
1
1
8
5
2
7
n
e
n
_
un
_
0
0
3
0
8
p
d

t

.

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image
RESEARCH image

Télécharger le PDF