REPORT

REPORT

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge
in Native and Non-Native Reading

Yevgeni Berzak1,2 and Roger Levy2

1Faculty of Data and Decision Sciences, TechnionIsrael Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel
2Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachussets Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA

un accès ouvert

journal

Mots clés: eye movements, reading, language learning, L1, L2

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

.

/

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

ABSTRAIT

The detailed study of eye movements in reading has shed considerable light into how
language processing unfolds in real time. Yet eye movements in reading remain inadequately
studied in non-native (L2) readers, even though much of the world’s population is multilingual.
Here we present a detailed analysis of the quantitative functional influences of word length,
frequency, and predictability on eye movement measures in reading in a large, linguistically
diverse sample of non-native English readers. We find many similar qualitative effects as in
L1 readers, but crucially also a proficiency-sensitive “lexicon-context tradeoff ”. The most
proficient L2 readers’ eye movements approach an L1 pattern, but as L2 proficiency
diminishes, readers’ eye movements become less sensitive to a word’s predictability in context
and more sensitive to word frequency, which is context-invariant. This tradeoff supports a
rational, experience-dependent account of how context-driven expectations are deployed in
L2 language processing.

INTRODUCTION

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye movements in reading provide fine-grained information about how language understand-
ing unfolds in real time in the human mind, and present one of the most detailed pictures of
the perception–inference–action cycle in human behavior for complex informational domains
( Just & Carpenter, 1980; Rayner, 1998). The large majority of work in eye movements in read-
ing focuses on native-language (L1) reading. Cependant, many of the people in the world are
multilingual, and a large amount of reading is done in non-native (L2) languages, making L2
reading an under-studied area. This is especially the case for English, where about 75% de
English speakers are not native (Crystal, 2003).

One of the most significant advances in eye movements research over the past several
decades has been the development of quantitative models of the relationship between linguis-
tic properties of words and eye movements in reading (Kliegl et al., 2004; Rayner et al., 2004,
2011, entre autres). A key finding of this line of work is the identification of three key lin-
guistic properties of words, often referred to as “benchmark” word properties or the “big
three”, which systematically explain substantial variance in mean fixation times: word length,
word frequency and word predictability. These effects have been shown to apply across lan-
guages, and their functional form has been studied in L1 (Kliegl et al., 2004; Forgeron & Levy,
2013; Wilcox et al., 2020). Cependant, only a few studies have examined benchmark word

Citation: Berzak, Y., & Levy, R.. (2023).
Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic
Knowledge in Native and Non-Native
Reading. Open Mind: Discoveries
in Cognitive Science, 7, 179–196.
https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00084

EST CE QUE JE:
https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00084

Supplemental Materials:
https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00084

Reçu: 1 Février 2023
Accepté: 19 Mars 2023

Intérêts concurrents: The authors
declare no conflict of interests.

Auteur correspondant:
Yevgeni Berzak
berzak@technion.ac.il

droits d'auteur: © 2023
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Publié sous Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International
(CC PAR 4.0) Licence

La presse du MIT

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

property effects in L2 reading (Cop, Keuleers, Drieghe, & Duyck, 2015; Mor & Prior, 2022;
Whitford & Titone, 2012, 2017), and both their functional form in L2 and their relation to lan-
guage proficiency are currently unknown.

Ici, we address these gaps by conducting a quantitative investigation of benchmark word
property effects in English L2 reading and compare them to English L1 reading. Our analysis is
performed at a scale and level of detail greater than previously possible, due to the introduc-
tion of CELER (Berzak et al., 2022), a large and linguistically diverse sample of L2 reading.
CELER has 69 L1 participants and 296 L2 participants from five typologically diverse native
language backgrounds: Arabic, Chinese, Japonais, Portuguese and Spanish. Differently from
other eye movements in L2 reading datasets such as GECO (Cop, Drieghe, & Duyck, 2015)
and MECO-L2 (Kuperman et al., 2022), CELER includes scores on a standardized English pro-
ficiency test. This facilitates a comprehensive characterization of the trajectory of benchmark
word property effects in L2 reading as a function of English proficiency.

More broadly, our study poses the following key question: how does the role of linguistic
context in generating expectations during reading vary depending on a comprehender’s lan-
guage proficiency? We address this question within a theoretical framework of rational pro-
cessing efficiency. This framework predicts that an optimal system might use contextualized
expectations less than context-independent expectations the lower the language proficiency of
the speaker. One reason for this is that context-contingent expectations are statistically intrin-
sically harder to estimate than context-independent expectations. Donc, the less language
experience the speaker has, the more they might rely on more reliable context-independent
expectations. Another reason is that context-contingent expectations likely are computation-
ally more difficult to deploy—they have to be updated in real time as the context evolves, et
the speed of expectation deployment may be lower when the speaker has less experience
with a language. Accordingly, less proficient speakers may need to rely more on context-
independent expectations that are easier to estimate and deploy.

We test this prediction by taking advantage of the fact that word frequency and word
predictability effects manifest ubiquitously and strongly during reading. Based on previous
travail (Howes & Solomon, 1951; Forgeron & Levy, 2013), we operationalize these measures as
negative log-frequency and negative log-predictability, or surprisal. We compare frequency
and surprisal effects in L2 versus L1 readers of English, and in readers of varying L2 profi-
ciency, using standard fixation measures of progressively longer duration, thereby supporting
a detailed comparison between participants as online language processing unfolds over
temps. We perform three analyses in which we examine the functional form of benchmark
word property effects, their magnitude, and how they depend on language proficiency. These
analyses build on prior work that estimates frequency and predictability effects in L1 and L2
using linear modeling (Cop, Keuleers, et coll., 2015; Mor & Prior, 2022; Whitford & Titone,
2012, 2017). We go beyond this prior work by characterizing the functional form of these
effects, explicitly comparing them to one another, estimating their dependence on language
proficiency using standardized test scores, and using a larger and more linguistically diverse
dataset.

MÉTHODES

Dataset

We use the CELER dataset (Berzak et al., 2022) which contains 365 participants (296 L2 and
69 L1). The L2 participants come from five different L1s: Arabic, Chinese, Japonais,

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

180

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

.

/

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

Portuguese and Spanish. Each participant in CELER reads 156 randomly selected sentences
from the Wall Street Journal ( WSJ) (Charniak et al., 2000; Marcus et al., 1993). Of these, 78
sentences are unique to each participant (Individual Regime), et 78 are presented to all par-
ticipants (Shared Regime).

To encourage attentive reading, upon completion of reading each sentence participants
answered a yes/no question about its content, and were subsequently informed if they
answered the question correctly. Le 78 questions for the Shared Regime sentences are read-
ing comprehension questions that were composed manually by the experimenters. The ques-
tions for the Individual Regime sentences were generated automatically, and ask whether a
given word appeared in the sentence. Figure S10 in the Supplemental Materials depicts the
scores of the L2 participants on all the 156 questions against their MPT English proficiency
scores described below. All but one participant have above chance performance on these
questions.

All the L2 participants were assessed for English proficiency in lab using the listening com-
prehension and grammar sections of the Michigan English Placement Test (MPT) Form B. Le
test materials have 50 multiple choice questions, avec 20 listening comprehension questions
et 30 written grammar questions. The test score is computed as the number of correct
answers for these questions, with possible scores ranging from 0 à 50. The test scores corre-
spond to CEFR levels (Council of Europe, 2001) approximately as follows: 0–16 A1, 17–21 A2,
22–31 B1, 32–36 B2, 37–50 C1 (Berzak et al., 2022).

To avoid overfitting to a small set of sentences, we use the Individual Regime materials,
comprising 28,457 sentences and 320,221 words. Following standard practice, we exclude
out-of-vocabulary words, skipped words, words with punctuation, numbers, and words that
begin or end a sentence. This leads to a total of 28,099 sentences and 181,448 words used
in our analyses.

Word Property Annotations

Each word wi in CELER is annotated with its negative log-frequency (negative log-unigram
probability): −log2 p(wi), surprisal: −log2 p(wi|w1, , wi−1) and word length. Frequency counts
are taken from the standard frequency list SUBTLEX-US (Brysbaert & Nouveau, 2009). Surprisal
values are computed using the state-of-the-art language model GPT2 (Radford et al., 2019).
In cases where the GPT tokenizer splits a word into multiple tokens, we sum the surprisal
values of those tokens. Word length values exclude punctuation.

GAM Model

Analysis 1 Chiffre 1 presents GAM fits for the relation between benchmark word properties and
raw reading times in L1 and L2. The curves are fitted using mgcv (1.8-31) with cubic regression
splines ( Wood, 2004). We use the bam function ( Wood et al., 2015) with fast REML smoothing
parameter estimation. Surprisal curves are fitted with the model:

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

/

.

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

RT ∼ s surp; bs ¼ “cr”; k ¼ 20

ð

(cid:1)
Þ þ s surppr
(cid:1)
Þ þ te freqpr
þ te freq; len; bs ¼ “cr”
þ s subj; bs ¼ “re”
ð
Þ þ s subj; surp; bs ¼ “re”
þ te subj; freq; len; bs ¼ “re”

Þ

ð

ð

ð

(cid:3)
; bs ¼ “cr”; k ¼ 20
(cid:3)

; lenpr ; bs ¼ “cr”
Þ

(1)

181

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

.

/

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Chiffre 1. GAM fits for the relation between benchmark word properties (current word) and raw reading times using the model in Equa-
tion 1 et 2. Upper three rows depict slowdown effects in ms as a function of frequency, surprisal and word length for First Fixation, Gaze
Duration and Total Fixation, with bootstrapped 95% intervalles de confiance. Curves are depicted in blue for L1 and in red for L2. At the top left is
the significance of the quadratic term when replacing the word property smooth term of the current word with a linear and quadratic terms.
‘***’ p < 0.001, ‘**’ p < 0.01. ‘*’ p < 0.05, ‘(.)’ p > 0.05. Bottom row: Density plots for frequency, surprisal and word length values. Key results:
Superlinear curves for frequency and surprisal in L2 and for frequency Gaze Duration in L1. Stronger superlinearity in L2 than in L1 for both
frequency and surprisal. Stronger superlinearity for frequency than for surprisal within both L1 and L2.

and frequency and word length curves are fitted with the model:

RT ∼ s surp; bs ¼ “cr”; k ¼ 20

ð

(cid:3)
; bs ¼ “cr”; k ¼ 20

(cid:1)
Þ þ s surppr
(cid:1)
(cid:3)
; bs ¼ “cr”; k ¼ 20
Þ þ s freqpr
(cid:5)
Þ þ s lenpr ; bs ¼ “cr”
Þ þ s subj; surp; bs ¼ “re”

Þ

ð

Þ þ s subj; len; bs ¼ “re”

ð

Þ

ð

ð

(cid:4)

þ s freq; bs ¼ “cr”; k ¼ 20
þ s len; bs ¼ “cr”
þ s subj; bs ¼ “re”
þ s subj; freq; bs ¼ ”re”

ð

ð

(2)

where pr indicates a property of the previous word (to account for spillover effects; Rayner,
1998).

Terms with bs = “re” correspond to participant level random effects (not included in the
model of Smith & Levy, 2013). We estimate 95% confidence intervals using the bootstrapping
method of Smith and Levy (2013). We test for non-linearity of a predictor by replacing its

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

182

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

smooth term s in the GAM model with a linear and a quadratic terms, and a random slope for
the quadratic term, and testing for the significance of the quadratic term, keeping all the other
predictors unchanged.

Taille de l'effet

In Analysis 2 et 3, we compute per participant effect sizes for our three word properties—
frequency, surprisal, and length. If we were using ordinary linear regression, the regression
coefficients for these word properties would quantify the effect sizes, but with a GAM there
is no single regression coefficient for each word property. Plutôt, for each participant i we fit
a the GAM model in Equations 1 et 2 without random effects (c'est à dire. without the “re” terms),
and quantify the participant’s effect size using the average slowdown for word property p as
follows:

S l o w d o w n i;p ¼ 1
Cj

j

X

w2C

ð
si;p p wð

Þ

Þ

(3)

where p 2 {freq, surp, len} and si,p is the (potentially nonlinear) partial effect of the word prop-
erty in participant’s i GAM model; we evaluate this partial effect at the property’s value for
the word, c'est à dire., p(w). C is the entire corpus. Par exemple, if the shape of participant i’s sur-
prisal effect were linear with slope 5 ms per bit, then for a word w with a surprisal of 6 bits,
si,p(surp(w)) would be 30 ms.

In Figure 2 we present the mean current word property effects for raw reading times across
participants in L1 and L2. Chiffre 3 depicts current word property effects for raw reading times
as a function of English proficiency as measured by the MPT test. A GAM is then fitted to the
resulting by-participant L2 slowdown effects to reveal the relationship between participant
effect sizes and English proficiency.

The Lexicon–Context Tradeoff

The difference between the contributions of frequency and surprisal in Figure 4 is computed
for each participant i as the difference between the respective slowdown effects, c'est à dire.:

Diff i

¼ 1
Cj

j

X

w2C

ð
si; freq freq wð

Þ

ð
Þ − si;surp surp wð

Þ
Þ

(4)

The Relationship Between Proficiency and Response to Word Properties

In Analysis 3, we conduct five statistical tests to answer key questions about the shape of the
relationship between MPT-measured English proficiency and the sensitivity of eye movement
measures to word frequency, length, and surprisal, as detailed below. Each test involves fitting
a multiple regression model and testing the significance of the model term corresponding to
the question. For these analyses, native English speakers are assigned the maximum possible
MPT score.

D'abord, are the effects of L2 English proficiency non-linear? To answer this question we con-

ducted the following tests:

m1 : response ∼ s MPT
m2 : response ∼ MPT þ MPT 2

ð

Þ

Þ
ð
testing the smooth term
ð
testing the quadratic term

Þ

Deuxième, are the most proficient L2 speakers’ effect sizes indistinguishable from those of native
speakers? To answer this question we included a 0/1 predictor variable indicating whether the

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

183

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

.

/

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

Chiffre 2. Mean per subject slowdown effects in ms with 95% confidence intervals based on the GAM model in Equations 1 et 2 fitted
separately for each subject (without subject random effects). The slowdown effect for each subject is calculated using Equation 3. Top left:
statistical significance of a t-test for the difference between English L1 and English L2. ‘***’ p < 0.001, ‘**’ p < 0.01. ‘*’ p < 0.05, ‘(.)’ p > 0.05.
Key results: Frequency effects are larger in L2 than L1 for all fixation measures. Surprisal effects are larger in L2 than L1 for Gaze Duration and
Total Fixation. Differences between L1 and L2 are larger for frequency than for surprisal. While in L1 Total Fixation Duration surprisal is larger
than frequency ( p < 0.001), in L2 the relative importance of frequency remains larger than surprisal ( p < 0.01). i . / / 1 0 1 1 6 2 o p m _ a _ 0 0 0 8 4 2 1 3 3 8 3 9 o p m _ a _ 0 0 0 8 4 p d / . i f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 participant was an English L1 reader, and tested the significance of this term for the following two models: m3 : response ∼ English þ MPT m4 : response ∼ English þ MPT þ MPT 2 ð ð including only participants with above(cid:2)median MPT Þ including all participants Þ Third, among above-average L2 readers, do the effect sizes increase with decreasing profi- ciency? To answer this question we tested the significance of the linear MPT term in the fol- lowing model applied to participants with above-median MPT score: m5 : response ∼ MPT In Figures 3 and 4, the statistical significance levels p1 − p5 correspond to these tests for models m1 − m5 respectively. Preregistration The analyses in this paper were pre-registered at http://osf.io/azrh3 for v1 of CELER which comprises 182 participants (Part 1). The remaining data from 183 participants (Part 2) was OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science 184 Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge Berzak and Levy l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u o p m i / l a r t i c e - p d f / d o i / i . / / 1 0 1 1 6 2 o p m _ a _ 0 0 0 8 4 2 1 3 3 8 3 9 o p m _ a _ 0 0 0 8 4 p d / . i f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 3. Slowdown effects associated with benchmark word properties of the current word for raw reading times as a function of English proficiency. Each blue circle is a single L2 speaker. The y axis is the mean slowdown effect for the word property from Equation 3, from the GAM model in Equations 1 and 2, fitted separately for each participant and measure. The x axis is the MPT English proficiency score. The blue line is a GAM fit through the L2 slowdown effects Mean Slowdown ∼ s(MPT ), and the red line is the mean of the L1 slowdown effects, both with 95% confidence intervals. Key results: U shaped relation between proficiency and responsiveness to frequency across all three fixation measures, as well as surprisal for Gaze Duration and Total Fixation. No statistical difference between L2 speakers at the highest proficiency levels and L1 speakers, with the exception of Total Fixation surprisal. Statistical significance of relevant hypothesis tests indicated in top left (see Methods). ‘***’ p < 0.001, ‘**’ p < 0.01. ‘*’ p < 0.05, ‘(.)’ p > 0.05.

Chiffre 4. The frequency–surprisal slowdown effect difference (Équation 4) for current word raw reading times, as a function of English
proficiency. Each blue circle is a single L2 speaker; curves are obtained from the GAM model in Equations 1 et 2 fitted separately for each
participant and fixation measure. The x axis is the MPT English proficiency score. The blue line is a GAM fit through the L2 values, and the red
line is the mean of the L1 values, both with 95% intervalles de confiance. Key results: L1: the importance of frequency compared to surprisal
decreases in the transition from First Fixation to Total Fixation. L2: In each measure the importance of frequency compared to surprisal
decreases with proficiency. Highly proficient L2 speakers reach a pattern similar to L1 speakers. Statistical significance of relevant hypothesis
tests indicated in top left; see also Methods. ‘***’ p < 0.001, ‘**’ p < 0.01. ‘*’ p < 0.05, ‘(.)’ p > 0.05.

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

185

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

held out for confirmatory analyses. In Figures S13–S20 of the Supplemental Materials, nous
provide the analyses results separately for Part 1 and Part 2. The results are highly consistent
across the two parts, as well as with all 365 participants.

RÉSULTATS

Analysis 1: Functional Form

Our first analysis characterizes the functional form of the relation between reading times and
benchmark word properties in English L1 and L2. In this and the following two analysis we
focus on the effects of frequency and predictability, while also presenting word length results
for additional reference. Following Howes and Solomon (1951) et d'autres, models of eye
movements in reading generally assume a linear relationship between log-frequency and read-
ing times (Engbert et al., 2005; Reichle et al., 2003). Cependant, recent work has suggested that
this relation might be superlinear in the low frequency range (Kuperman & Van Dyke, 2013;
White et al., 2018; Wotschack & Kliegl, 2013). This result is in line with word recognition
studies which yielded a superlinear relation for frequency in both L1 and L2, with a stronger
superlinearity for L2 compared to L1 (Diependaele et al., 2013; Lemhöfer et al., 2008). Pour
predictability, Smith and Levy (2013), Goodkind and Bicknell (2018), and Wilcox et al.
(2020) found a linear relationship between reading times in L1 and predictability as measured
by corpus based surprisal.

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

.

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Despite these advances, the functional form of both frequency and predictability effects in
L2 reading has not been characterized to date. Prior work that examined frequency effects in
L1 and L2 assumed a linear effect of log frequency on reading times (Cop, Keuleers, et coll.,
2015; Mor & Prior, 2022; Whitford & Titone, 2012). Whitford and Titone (2017) and Mor
and Prior (2022) assumed a linear effect of (untransformed) predictability. Here we relax this
linearity assumption, using nonparametric statistical methods to find the functional form best
supported by the data.

In this and subsequent analyses we quantify word predictability as corpus based surprisal
(Hale, 2001; Levy, 2008) using the GPT2 language model (Radford et al., 2019) whose sur-
prisal estimates were shown to correlate well with reading times in English L1 (Heilbron et al.,
2022; Wilcox et al., 2020). We use three standard fixation measures:

First Fixation: the duration of the first fixation on a word.

1.
2. Gaze Duration: the time from first entering the word to first leaving it.
3.

Total Fixation: the sum of all fixations on a word.

These three measures stand in a temporally monotonic inclusion relationship: the earliest time
period of a word’s Gaze Duration is its First Fixation, and the earliest time period of a word’s
Total Fixation time is its Gaze Duration. Hence measures 1–3 capture successively later stages
of language processing (Inhoff, 1984; Liversedge & Findlay, 2000; Rayner, 1998).

We estimate the functional relationship between benchmark word properties and fixation
times using General Additive Models (GAMs). The model, specified in Equations 1 et 2, pre-
dicts reading times from the frequency, surprisal and word length of the current and the pre-
vious words. We fit this model separately for the L1 and L2 groups, and each of our three
fixation measures. We test for superlinearity of a predictor by replacing its smooth term in
the GAM model with linear and quadratic terms, and testing for the significance of the qua-
dratic term.

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

186

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

Chiffre 1 depicts the resulting curves for the current word. Figure S1 in the Supplemental
Materials further presents spillover effects from the previous word. For L1 frequency, nous
observe a linear relation for First Fixation while also obtaining evidence for superlinearity in
the low frequency range for Gaze Duration as suggested in (Kuperman & Van Dyke, 2013;
White et al., 2018; Wotschack & Kliegl, 2013). Visual inspection of Total Fixation suggests
a similar trend, although the superlinearity is not statistically significant. L1 surprisal curves
are linear, largely replicating prior work, with the exception of weakly superlinear curve for
Total Fixation. For L2, cependant, frequency effects are superlinear for all three measures and
surprisal effects are superlinear for Gaze Duration and Total Fixation. These outcomes do not
support the linearity assumption previously taken in the literature when analyzing frequency
and surprisal effects in L2 reading data. Figure S2 of the Supplemental Materials shows that
these functional forms are preserved after reading times normalization. Figures S3 and S4 in
the Supplemental Materials further break down the L2 current word results by native language,
indicating that the functional form results hold across the languages in our sample. Figure S11
further provides mean fixation durations by L1. Dans l'ensemble, we observe more superlinearity in L2
compared to L1 for both frequency and surprisal, and stronger superlinearity for frequency
than surprisal within L1 and L2. We further note substantial differences in the magnitude of
the L1 and L2 effects, with larger discrepancies between L1 and L2 for frequency than for sur-
prisal. We examine these differences further in Analysis 2.

The superlinearity of frequency effects can be interpreted with respect to lexical knowledge,
which is an important factor in reading comprehension ability (Perfetti, 2007). It is possible that
words which are not in the participant’s lexicon will introduce a substantial overhead to their
expected processing time from a linear function, resulting in a superlinear response. As the
probability of any lexical item being unknown to the speaker is higher in L2 than in L1, le
superlinearity is stronger for L2. De la même manière, the superlinearity in L2 surprisal is likely to be related
to the limited ability of this population to perform meaningful contextual integration. A key
avenue for future work will be developing a formal model for these curves, where a key challenge
will be accounting for the larger L1–L2 discrepancies for frequency as compared to surprisal.

Analysis 2: Magnitude

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

.

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

In our second analysis we use a summary view of Analysis 1, to quantify and compare the
overall magnitudes of benchmark word property effects in L1 and L2. Whitford and Titone
(2012), Cop, Keuleers, et autres. (2015) and Mor and Prior (2022) observed larger frequency effects
in L2 compared to L1. This outcome is consistent with studies which obtained larger L2 than
L1 frequency effects in single word recognition and production tasks, including lexical
décision (Duyck et al., 2008; Van Wijnendaele & Brysbaert, 2002), progressive demasking
(Diependaele et al., 2013; Lemhöfer et al., 2008), word naming (de Groot et al., 2002) et
picture naming (Gollan et al., 2008). Whitford and Titone (2017) found no evidence for L1
versus L2 differences for predictability effects, while Mor and Prior (2022) found larger L2 than
L1 effects for Total Fixation Duration. As Analysis 1 suggests superlinearity in many of the
relevant effects, here we examine their magnitude without assuming linear effect shapes.

To calculate effect magnitude, we fit the GAM model in Equations 1 et 2 pour chaque sujet
(without the by-subject random effects), and calculate the subject’s mean word property slow-
down effect across all the words in the corpus using Equation 3. Chiffre 2 depicts the average
slowdown effect across subjects for L1 and L2. Consistent with Whitford and Titone (2012),
Cop, Keuleers, et autres. (2015) and Mor and Prior (2022), the effect of frequency is larger in L2
than in L1 for all three fixation measures. Differently from Whitford and Titone (2017) et en

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

187

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

line with Mor and Prior (2022), we find that the effect of surprisal in L2 is larger than L1 for
Gaze Duration and Total Fixation. As observed in Analysis 1, L1 versus L2 differences for sur-
prisal are considerably smaller than for frequency. Surtout, we see that in the transition
from Gaze Duration to Total Fixation, L1 speakers end up with a substantially larger effect
for surprisal than for frequency, while in L2 frequency effects remain larger than surprisal.
We further note that the response to surprisal is delayed compared to frequency in both L1
and L2. Figure S5 in the Supplemental Materials depicts this analysis for speed normalized
fixation times, where we observe that larger L2 than L1 effects persist for frequency, but not
for surprisal. Figures S6 and S7 break down the analysis by native language, and suggest that
the results largely hold across different native languages. We also note potential magnitude
differences between native languages for frequency and word length, which are also apparent
in S3 and S4 of Analysis 1, whose investigation we leave for future work.

Taken together, the results of Analyses 1 et 2 exhibit a marked difference in the dynamics
of the frequency and surprisal predictors within and across the L1 and L2 groups. Le
differences are consistent with an interpretation that frequency and surprisal tap into different
cognitive processing mechanisms, where frequency is associated with lexical processing and
surprisal with contextual processing, with the former generally preceding the latter (Staub,
2011). This interpretation is also consistent with the observation that reading times in L2 differ
from L1 primarily in larger frequency effects which are more pronounced than surprisal even at
the latest stages of processing, suggesting a larger role for lexical processing compared to con-
textual processing as determinants of processing load in non-native language comprehension.

Analysis 3: Interaction with L2 Proficiency

Our final analysis examines how responsiveness to benchmark word properties depends
English L2 proficiency. In prior work, Whitford and Titone (2012) and Cop, Keuleers, et autres.
(2015) have found that the magnitude of L2 frequency effects in reading is inversely related
to self reported L2 exposure. Cependant, Cop, Keuleers, et autres. (2015) did not find such an inter-
action for linguistic proficiency, which was approximated in their study using the LexTALE
English vocabulary test (Lemhöfer & Broersma, 2012). In the word recognition domain,
Diependaele et al. (2013) did find a frequency—LexTALE proficiency interaction both in L1
and L2 speakers of English. Mor and Prior (2022) examined the relation between word pre-
dictability and proficiency, approximated from a combination of the Shipley vocabulary test
(Shipley, 1946) and the TOWRE reading fluency test (Torgesen et al., 1999), and found no
evidence for an interaction between the two.

Differently from previous approaches in the literature which use linear modelling (Cop,
Keuleers, et coll., 2015; Diependaele et al., 2013; Mor & Prior, 2022; Whitford & Titone,
2012), here we do not assume linearity and characterize the functional form of this interaction.
To this end, as in Analysis 2, we fit the GAM model in Equations 1 et 2 separately for each
participant (without the by-subject random effects), and compute an average word property
slowdown effect for each participant using Equation 3. We then fit a GAM through the result-
ing participant slowdown effects as a function of language proficiency, as measured by the
listening comprehension and grammar sections of the Michigan Placement Test (MPT).

Chiffre 3 presents the resulting curves against the mean slowdown effect of the L1 group. Nous
observe that the linear interaction approach in the literature underfits the data. For all three
measures, the frequency effect sizes are U-shaped; effect sizes initially increase with decreas-
ing proficiency, but the slope of this relationship diminishes or reverses with even lower pro-
ficiency. The highest proficiency L2 readers’ effect size is statistically indistinguishable from

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

188

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

/

.

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

native readers. These visually apparent results are statistically confirmed in five hypothesis
tests; see Methods. Similar results are observed for surprisal in Gaze Duration and Total
Fixation. Figure S8 of the Supplemental Materials shows that these results are preserved for
frequency when accounting for reading speed. Figure S21 of the Supplemental Materials further
shows that the results largely hold when replacing the MPT with the percentage of correctly
answered reading comprehension questions during the eye-tracking experiment.

This outcome opens an intriguing question on the role of text difficulty in the effect of word
properties on reading times. One possibility is that high discrepancy between text difficulty
and proficiency results in more text skimming, as evidenced by the decrease in the mean fix-
ation durations in the low proficiency range in Figure S12 in the Supplemental Material. Prior
research suggests that mindless reading weakens the response to word properties (Reichle
et coll., 2010). When low proficiency participants read challenging newswire text, they might
be engaging in skimming-like behavior more than higher proficiency readers, leading to faster
reading and smaller word property effects. Alternativement, the non-linear modulation of profi-
ciency on word property effects could also be invariant to the difficulty level of the text. Nous
leave this question for future research.

Enfin, in Figure 4 we fit individual participant models identical to those in Figure 3, et
then depict the mean difference between the slowdown effects associated with frequency and
surprisal across all the words in the corpus for each participant using Equation 4. Consistent
with the previous analyses, for L1 readers the importance of surprisal compared to frequency
increases from First Fixation to Total Fixation. L2 readers are able to rely increasingly more on
facilitation from context-based prediction, with the most proficient L2 readers reaching an
L1-like pattern in all three fixation measures. Figure S9 of the Supplemental Materials presents
similar results with normalized reading times. Figure S22 of the Supplemental Materials shows
that the results hold when using reading comprehension scores in place of the MPT.

DISCUSSION

Our analyses yield the following primary results.

1.

Functional Form: In L1, we find that fixation times are linear in surprisal, and weakly
superlinear in frequency. In L2, the relation between reading times and frequency, comme
well as surprisal, is superlinear. Dans l'ensemble, we observe stronger superlinearity for fre-
quency than surprisal across L1 and L2, and stronger superlinearity for L2 compared
to L1 across frequency and surprisal.

2. Magnitude: Both frequency and surprisal effects are larger in L2 compared to L1.
Plus loin, differences between L1 and L2 are larger for frequency than for surprisal.
Our analysis also yields differences in the time course of the response to frequency
and surprisal, both within and across the L1 and L2 groups. En particulier, differently
from L1 where the relative importance of frequency is smaller the later the stage of
processing captured by a fixation measures, in L2 frequency effects remain larger than
surprisal across all fixation measures.
Interaction with L2 proficiency: The modulation of L2 proficiency on frequency and
surprisal effects is non-linear; they increase as language proficiency decreases, alors
saturate and possibly decrease in the low proficiency range. The most proficient L2
speakers exhibit frequency and surprisal effects similar to those of L1 speakers.

3.

These results suggest that although L2 reading is qualitatively similar to L1, it also differs from
L1 in the dominance of frequency effects over surprisal effects. This outcome is consistent with

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

189

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

.

/

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

the theoretical account of rational processing, and suggests a key difference between L2 and
L1 in what we refer to as a lexicon–context tradeoff: context-based prediction plays a less cen-
tral role in affecting eye movements in reading for L2 speakers than for L1 speakers, but this is
modulated by L2 proficiency, with the most proficient L2 speakers approaching a fully L1-like
pattern. These results suggest that with language learning comes a gradual shift in the online
dynamics of language processing, away from lexical processing and towards contextual
traitement.

Our analyses speak to a number of fundamental questions in language processing and
language learning. D'abord, our results are largely consistent with “lexical entrenchment”
(Diependaele et al., 2013) and “weaker links” (Gollan et al., 2008) accounts, which posit
that linguistic knowledge is inversely related to frequency effects. Cependant, tracing how
benchmark word property effects depend on language proficiency reveals that this trajectory
is not monotonic.

Suivant, this work is pertinent to the general interpretation of frequency and surprisal effects in
reading. While many studies found both frequency and surprisal effects in reading, their co-
existence poses a theoretical challenge. Since log-frequency is formally simply unigram sur-
prisal, it might be expected to be subsumed by surprisal. It is currently an open question
whether frequency and surprisal encode different mechanisms, and recent experimental
results suggest that surprisal may indeed subsume frequency (Shain, 2019). Our results do
not support this view; they not only reinforce previous accounts of frequency effects being
present above and beyond surprisal, but also suggest different time course dynamics and dif-
ferent effects of frequency and surprisal within and across L1 and L2. This suggests that com-
mon computational constraints apply to both L1 and L2 readers, but are more severe for L2
readers, the more so the lower the reader’s proficiency. Dans l'ensemble, our results are consistent with
the interpretation that frequency taps into lexical processing while surprisal is associated with
contextual processing.

Enfin, our results indicate that the differences between L1 and L2 are substantially more
pronounced with respect to frequency than surprisal. This is reflected both in the larger gaps
between L1 and L2 frequency effects, as well as in the larger degree of superlinearity in the
relation between reading times and frequency as compared to surprisal. Plus loin, relative to
surprisal, frequency effects play a more dominant role in L2 compared to L1. Given these
résultats, and our association of frequency with lexical processing and surprisal with contextual
traitement, L1 speakers are able to rely more heavily on contextual facilitation. The process of
L2 learning involves a gradual shift in a lexicon-context tradeoff, with diminishing importance
of lexical processing to the overall process of language comprehension. The most proficient L2
speakers exhibit a tradeoff indistinguishable from L1.

A potential caveat to this interpretation of our results is the possibility that language model
based surprisals are a less accurate estimate of L2 subjective probabilities than of L1, and a
worse estimator for less proficient L2 speakers. The simplest account one could propose might
be that the learning and expectation-deployment mechanisms for L2 and L1 speakers are the
same, but L2 readers are disadvantaged by a smaller linguistic sample size than L1 readers
(amount of exposure to the language) for learning expectations. Smaller sample size means
more variability, and thus more potential discrepancy between L2 reader expectations and
the properties of the read texts, especially for rare events that are unlikely to have occurred
often in a reader’s experience. These discrepancies could thus lead to greater reading-time
penalties for rare and surprising words, potentially yielding both non-linear effect shapes (comme
seen in Analysis 1) and larger effect sizes (as seen in Analysis 2).

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

190

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

.

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

Cependant, based on initial computational simulations we have conducted to investigate
these issues, which we report in Appendix A, we believe that it is unlikely that an account
based solely on the size of the learning sample would satisfactorily explain the patterns we
see in our data: apparent nonlinearities resulting from sample size effects are minimal for word
frequency, and subjective expectations learned from small samples do not substantially mag-
nify apparent effect sizes for word frequency and surprisal estimates that are based on a larger
training corpus (in many cases, they lead to reduced effect sizes). Why the quantitative effect
shape and size effects are seen in our data is therefore, we believe, an important theoretical
and empirical question. In the future, machine learning techniques for estimating L2 lexica
and context-based word predictions could be used to develop and evaluate computationally
implemented hypotheses.

An additional limitation of this work is that CELER comprises out-of-context single
phrases. While single sentence corpora have been widely used in psycholinguistics,
conclusions drawn from such datasets regarding word predictability cannot take into account
extra-sentential context. Is is currently an open question whether our conclusions will
generalize to contextualized reading of full passages. We plan to examine this question in
future work.

Finalement, an important challenge in cognitive science is the formulation of computational
models which predict eye movements in reading based on fundamental principles of language
processing and acquisition. To accurately capture the full scope of human reading behavior,
such models will have to account for the variability in the linguistic knowledge and experience
of readers, and reproduce their effects on fixation times. Our study takes a step forward in
delineating the empirical landscape that will guide the development of such models.

REMERCIEMENTS

This work was supported NSF STC award CCF-1231216, MIT–IBM AI research lab, the MIT
Quest for Intelligence, NSF grant IIS-1815529, BCS-2121074 and ISF grant 2070358.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Code for this paper is available here: https://github.com/lacclab/traces-of-ling-knowledge.
Data is available here: https://github.com/berzak/celer.

RÉFÉRENCES

Berzak, Y., Nakamura, C., Forgeron, UN., Weng, E., Katz, B., Flynn, S., &
Levy, R.. (2022). CELER: A 365-participant corpus of eye move-
ments in L1 and L2 English reading. Open Mind, 6, 41–50.
https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00054, PubMed: 36439073
Brysbaert, M., & Nouveau, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis:
A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the
introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for
American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.977, PubMed: 19897807

Charniak, E., Blaheta, D., Ge, N., Hall, K., Hale, J., & Johnson, M..
(2000). BLLIP 1987–89 WSJ. Linguistic Data Consortium, 36.
Cop, U., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W. (2015). Eye movement patterns
in natural reading: A comparison of monolingual and bilingual
reading of a novel. PLOS ONE, 10(8), Article e0134008.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134008, PubMed:
26287379

Cop, U., Keuleers, E., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W. (2015). Frequency
effects in monolingual and bilingual natural reading.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(5), 1216–1234. https://est ce que je
.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0819-2, PubMed: 25877485

Council of Europe. (2001). Common European framework of refer-
ence for languages: Apprentissage, teaching, assessment. Cambridge
Presse universitaire.

Crystal, D. (2003). English as a global language. Ernst Klett

Sprachen.

de Groot, UN. M.. B., Borgwaldt, S., Bos, M., & van den Eijnden, E.
(2002). Lexical decision and word naming in bilinguals: Lan-
guage effects and task effects. Journal of Memory and Language,
47(1), 91–124. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2840

Diependaele, K., Lemhöfer, K., & Brysbaert, M.. (2013). The word
frequency effect in first-and second-language word recognition:
A lexical entrenchment account. Quarterly Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology, 66(5), 843–863. https://est ce que je.org/10.1080
/17470218.2012.720994, PubMed: 23025801

Duyck, W., Vanderelst, D., Desmet, T., & Hartsuiker, R.. J.. (2008).
in second-language visual word

The frequency effect

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

191

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

.

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

reconnaissance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 850–855.
https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.4.850, PubMed: 18792515

Engbert, R., Nuthmann, UN., Richter, E. M., & Kliegl, R.. (2005).
SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during read-
ing. Psychological Review, 112(4), 777–813. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777, PubMed: 16262468

Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R.. JE., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008).
More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging,
bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Mem-
ory and Language, 58(3), 787–814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml
.2007.07.001, PubMed: 19343088

Goodkind, UN., & Bicknell, K. (2018). Predictive power of word sur-
prisal for reading times is a linear function of language model
qualité. In Proceedings of the 8th Workshop on Cognitive Model-
ing and Computational Linguistics (CMCL 2018) (pp. 10–18).
Association for Computational Linguistics. https://est ce que je.org/10
.18653/v1/ W18-0102

Hale, J.. (2001). A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic
model. In Proceedings of the Second Meeting of the North Amer-
ican Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics on
Language Technologies (pp. 1–8). Association for Computational
Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.3115/1073336.1073357

source de guérison, M., Armeni, K., Schoffelen, J.-M., Hagoort, P., & de
Lange, F. P.. (2022). A hierarchy of linguistic predictions during
natural language comprehension. Actes de la Nationale
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 119(32),
A r t i c l e e 2 2 0 1 9 6 8 11 9 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 7 3 / p n a s
.2201968119, PubMed: 35921434

Howes, D. H., & Solomon, R.. L. (1951). Visual duration threshold
as a function of word-probability. Journal of Experimental Psy-
cologie, 41(6), 401–410. https://doi.org/10.1037/ h0056020,
PubMed: 14873866

Inhoff, UN. W. (1984). Two stages of word processing during eye
fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 23(5), 612–624. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022
-5371(84)90382-7

Just, M.. UN., & Carpenter, P.. UN. (1980). A theory of reading: Depuis
eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4),
329–354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.4.329,
PubMed: 7413885

Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R.. (2004). Length, fre-
quency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in
reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(1–2),
262–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000213

Kuperman, V., Siegelman, N., Schroeder, S., Acartürk, C., Alexeeva,
S., Amenta, S., Bertram, R., Bonandrini, R., Brysbaert, M.,
Chernova, D., Da Fonseca, S. M., Dirix, N., Duyck, W., Fella,
UN., Frost, R., Gattei, C. UN., Kalaitzi, UN., Lõo, K., Marelli, M.,
Usal, K. UN. (2022). Text reading in English as a second language:
Evidence from the multilingual eye-movements corpus. Études
in Second Language Acquisition, 45(1), 3–37. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1017/S0272263121000954

Kuperman, V., & Van Dyke, J.. UN. (2013). Reassessing word fre-
quency as a determinant of word recognition for skilled and
unskilled readers. Journal de psychologie expérimentale: Human
Perception and Performance, 39(3), 802–823. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1037/a0030859, PubMed: 23339352

Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M.. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick
and valid lexical test for advanced learners of English. Behavior
Research Methods, 44(2), 325–343. https://doi.org/10.3758
/s13428-011-0146-0, PubMed: 21898159

Lemhöfer, K., Dijkstra, T., Schriefers, H., Baayen, R.. H., Grainger, J.,
& Zwitserlood, P.. (2008). Native language influences on word

recognition in a second language: A megastudy. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: Apprentissage, Mémoire, et cognitif, 34(1),
12–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.12, PubMed:
18194052

Levy, R.. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cog-
nition, 106(3), 1126–1177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition
.2007.05.006, PubMed: 17662975

Liversedge, S. P., & Findlay, J.. M.. (2000). Saccadic eye movements
and cognition. Tendances des sciences cognitives, 4(1), 6–14. https://
est ce que je.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01418-7, PubMed: 10637617
Marcus, M.. P., Santorini, B., & Marcinkiewicz, M.. UN. (1993). Build-
ing a large annotated corpus of English: The Penn treebank.
Computational Linguistics, 19(2), 313–330. https://est ce que je.org/10
.21236/ADA273556

Merity, S., Xiong, C., Bradbury, J., & Socher, R.. (2016). Pointer
sentinel mixture models. arXiv:1609.07843. https://est ce que je.org/10
.48550/arXiv.1609.07843

Mor, B., & Prior, UN. (2022). Frequency and predictability effects in
first and second language of different script bilinguals. Journal de
Experimental Psychology: Apprentissage, Mémoire, et cognitif,
48(9), 1363–1383. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000927,
PubMed: 34498903

Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehen-
sion. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. https://est ce que je
.org/10.1080/10888430701530730

Radford, UN., Wu, J., Child, R., Luan, D., Amodei, D., & Sutskever, je.
(2019). Language models are unsupervised multitask learners.
OpenAI Blog, 1(8), Article 9.

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information pro-
cessation: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3),
372–422. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372,
PubMed: 9849112

Rayner, K., Ashby, J., Pollatsek, UN., & Reichle, E. D. (2004). Le
effects of frequency and predictability on eye fixations in
reading: Implications for the E-Z reader model. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: Perception humaine et performance, 30(4),
720–732. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.720,
PubMed: 15301620

Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., Drieghe, D., & Liversedge, S. P.. (2011).
Eye movements and word skipping during reading: Effects of
word length and predictability. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: Perception humaine et performance, 37(2), 514–528.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020990, PubMed: 21463086

Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, UN. (2003). The E-Z reader
model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other
models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 445–476. https://
doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X03000104, PubMed: 15067951
Reichle, E. D., Reineberg, UN. E., & Schooler, J.. W. (2010). Eye
movements during mindless reading. Sciences psychologiques,
21(9), 1300–1310. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610378686,
PubMed: 20679524

Shain, C. (2019). A large-scale study of the effects of word fre-
quency and predictability in naturalistic reading. In Proceedings
of the 2019 Conference of the North American Chapter of the
Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language
Technologies, Volume 1 (Long and Short Papers) (pp. 4086–4094).
Association for Computational Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.18653
/v1/N19-1413

Shipley, W. C. (1946). Institute of living scale. Western Psycholog-

ical Services.

Forgeron, N. J., & Levy, R.. (2013). The effect of word predictability on
reading time is logarithmic. Cognition, 128(3), 302–319. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.013, PubMed: 23747651

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

192

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

.

/

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

Staub, UN. (2011). Word recognition and syntactic attachment in
reading: Evidence for a staged architecture. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: General, 140(3), 407–433. https://doi.org
/10.1037/a0023517, PubMed: 21604914

Torgesen, J.. K., Wagner, R.. K., & Rashotte, C. UN. (1999). Test of word

reading efficiency. Pro-Ed.

Van Wijnendaele, JE., & Brysbaert, M.. (2002). Visual word recogni-
tion in bilinguals: Phonological priming from the second to the
first language. Journal de psychologie expérimentale: Human
Perception and Performance, 28(3), 616–627. https://est ce que je.org/10
.1037/0096-1523.28.3.616, PubMed: 12075892

Blanc, S. J., Drieghe, D., Liversedge, S. P., & Staub, UN. (2018). Le
word frequency effect during sentence reading: A linear or non-
linear effect of log frequency? Journal trimestriel d'expérimentation
Psychologie, 71(1), 46–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218
.2016.1240813, PubMed: 27760490

Whitford, V., & Titone, D. (2012). Second-language experience
modulates first- and second-language word frequency effects:
Evidence from eye movement measures of natural paragraph
reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(1), 73–80. https://
doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0179-5, PubMed: 22042632

Whitford, V., & Titone, D. (2017). The effects of word frequency
and word predictability during first- and second-language para-
graph reading in bilingual older and younger adults. Psychologie
and Aging, 32(2), 158–177. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000151,
PubMed: 28287786

Wilcox, E. G., Gauthier, J., Hu, J., Qian, P., & Levy, R.. (2020). Sur
the predictive power of neural language models for human
real-time comprehension behavior. In Proceedings of the 42nd
Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1707–1713).
Cognitive Science Society.

Wood, S. N. (2004). Stable and efficient multiple smoothing
parameter estimation for generalized additive models. Journal
of the American Statistical Association, 99(467), 673–686.
https://doi.org/10.1198/016214504000000980

Wood, S. N., Goude, Y., & Shaw, S. (2015). Generalized additive models
for large data sets. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series C:
Applied Statistics, 64(1), 139–155. https://doi.org/10.1111/rssc.12068
Wotschack, C., & Kliegl, R.. (2013). Reading strategy modulates
parafoveal-on-foveal effects in sentence reading. Quarterly Jour-
nal of Experimental Psychology, 66(3), 548–562. https://doi.org
/10.1080/17470218.2011.625094, PubMed: 22026498

APPENDIX A: SIMULATION-BASED ANALYSIS OF THE EFFECT OF QUANTITY
OF LINGUISTIC EXPOSURE ON ESTIMATED WORD FREQUENCY AND
SURPRISAL EFFECTS

Taille de l'effet

One question raised in the discussion of our results is whether the difference in word fre-
quency and surprisal effect sizes for L1 and L2 populations found in Analysis 2 might derive
simply from differences in the amount of speakers’ linguistic experience. Theoretically this is
possible: intuitively, the less linguistic experience from which a speaker’s subjective word fre-
quencies and surprisals are derived, the more variable they will be from speaker to speaker
and the more they will differ from estimates derived from a large reference corpus used by a
researcher, inflating the effect sizes of frequency and surprisal as estimated from the reference
corpus.1 However, it is not clear whether this effect of variability on effect size would be sub-
stantial enough to account for the L1/L2 differences we see in our data. To get a handle on this
question, we turn to a simulation-based approach.

Our simulations have the following structure. We start with the Wikitext-2 dataset (Merity
et coll., 2016) and use relative frequency estimation to estimate unigram and bigram models of
English (treating the corpus as a loop to avoid issues with beginning and end of corpus). Nous
then use the bigram model to sample a reference corpus (simulating the dataset used by a
researcher to train a language model), a large corpus (simulating an L1 speaker’s linguistic
experience), a small corpus (simulating an L2 speaker’s linguistic experience), and a reading
corpus (simulating the texts on which eye movement measures during reading are collected

1 This can be mathematically justified. Surprisal and negative log-frequency are convex functions, Jensen’s
Inequality states that for a function f that is nonlinear on the set of values that can be taken by a random variable
X, E[F(X)] ≥ f(E[X]). In our setting, x is the subjective frequency or conditional probability of a given word, E[X] est
the population-average conditional probability (which by hypothesis is well estimated by the large reference
corpus), and Jensen’s inequality tells us that the variability of x will magnify word frequency and surprisal
effects. The less linguistic experience of the reader, the more variable their word log-frequency and surprisal
estimates will be, and thus the greater the magnification of the effect.

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

193

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

.

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

/

.

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

.

/

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Figure A1. Histograms of coefficient estimates for word frequency and surprisal effects on con-
tinuous eye movement measure based on simulated corpora of varying size (see main text for
details). Red dashed lines indicate mean estimate. The mean estimated frequency effect is very
slightly smaller (closer to zero) when based on larger corpora than on smaller corpora; the mean
estimated surprisal effects are indistinguishable.

from L1 and L2 speakers). Bigram and unigram models are relative-frequency estimated for the
reference, grand, and small corpora, and reading-time data are generated stochastically for the
reading corpus with word surprisal and negative log-frequency effects from the (je) petit; et
(ii) grand; unigram and bigram models respectively.2 Linear regression is then used to estimate
the effects of reference corpus log-frequency and surprisal on the resulting two reading-time
datasets, and we investigated the distributions of these word effects. The true linear model
coefficients relating word surprisal and negative log-frequency (from small or large corpora)
to the reading-time measure are βSurprisal = βFrequency = 1, and residual error is taken to be nor-
mally distributed with standard deviation 5.

Histograms of these coefficient estimates are shown in Figure A1. For surprisal (both corpus
sizes) and small-corpus word frequency, the mean coefficient estimates are around 0.9,
smaller than the true underlying coefficient values of 1, due to the effect of measurement error
from using the reference corpus-derived predictor values instead of the true subjective values
(from the large/small corpora). Cependant, for large-corpus word frequency effects, the mean
coefficient estimate is right around the true underlying coefficient value of 1.

2 When generating the reading-time data, we exclude tokens from the reading corpus that involve zero-count
events in any of the reference, petit, or large corpora.

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

194

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

We conclude from this simulation that a pure ”smaller sample size” account is unlikely to
satisfactorily explain the differing effect sizes observed in our datasets: at least as far as our
simulation suggests, apparent frequency effects would be larger for L1 speakers than for L2
speakers, due to the more severe measurement error for estimation of L2 word frequencies.

Effect Shape

In Analysis 1 we found clear evidence of superlinearity in word frequency and surprisal effects
for L2 speakers, whereas these effects were much closer to linear in L1 speakers. As with the
effect size question addressed earlier in this Appendix, an important question here is whether
this superlinearity might be a straightforward consequence of the amount of linguistic experi-
ence from which a reader derives subjective word frequency and surprisal. This argument
would proceed as follows: for readers with less experience (smaller sample size from which
learning occurs) like L2 readers, log-frequency and surprisal estimates will be more variable
than for readers with more experience like L1 readers. Due to the log transform relating fre-
quency and word probability to reading-time measures, this will translate into more variability
in effect sizes in low-probability regions, thus affecting lower-frequency and higher-surprisal
words more (and for surprisal affecting rarer contexts more). Donc, even if true underlying sub-
jective log-frequency and surprisal accounts are linear, it could look like superlinearity in
“true” L1-like log-frequency and surprisal (estimated from more data).

Cependant, we have investigated this effect through simulations, and to our surprise it turns
out to be very small. Our simulations focused on word frequency and proceeded as follows.
We collect a word frequency distribution from Wikitext-2, and then downsample the distribu-
tion by a factor of 200. We then assume that word frequency effects are linear in the down-
sampled word frequencies, generate random word-average RTs, and analyze the shape of the
“true” ( Wikitext-2) log-frequency effect on RTs using GAMs and quadratic regression. In some

je

D
o
w
n
o
un
d
e
d

F
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
je
r
e
c
t
.

m

je
t
.

/

e
d
toi
o
p
m

je
/

je

un
r
t
je
c
e

p
d

F
/

d
o

je
/

je

/

.

/

1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
2
1
3
3
8
3
9
o
p
m
_
un
_
0
0
0
8
4
p
d

/

.

je

F

b
oui
g
toi
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Effect of less linguistic exposure as the basis of a word frequency. The nonlinearity
Figure A2.
induced by sampling error due to the small sample size assumed for the non-native reader is min-
imal, and confined to the far low-frequency tail of the graph.

OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science

195

Eye Movement Traces of Linguistic Knowledge

Berzak and Levy

samples the shape can turn out to be superlinear, but the superlinearity is minimal and con-
fined to only the very rarest words—it is the slight upward bend in the rightmost part of the
graph in Figure A2, and rarely reaches statistical significance at p < 0.05 in simulations. We conclude from this that a pure “smaller sample size” account is unlikely to be enough to fully explain the superlinearity effects we observed—especially because our superlinearity is at least as pronounced in log-frequency as in surprisal, whereas on a pure sample-size account we would expect, if anything, the reverse (since the downsampling is even more extreme when it is context-specific). We believe that additional mechanisms beyond raw quantity of linguistic experience will need to be appealed to in order to explain the superlinearities seen in L2 readers’ word frequency and surprisal effects. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u o p m i / l a r t i c e - p d f / d o i / i . / / 1 0 1 1 6 2 o p m _ a _ 0 0 0 8 4 2 1 3 3 8 3 9 o p m _ a _ 0 0 0 8 4 p d . / i f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science 196REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image
REPORT image

Télécharger le PDF