RESEARCH ARTICLE

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Distinguishing Syntactic Operations in the Brain:
Dependency and Phrase-Structure Parsing

Alessandro Lopopolo1,2, Antal van den Bosch3

,
, and Roel M. Willems2,4,5

Karl-Magnus Petersson4

a n o p e n a c c e s s

j o u r n a l

1Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Germany
2Center for Language Studies, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
3Meertens Institute, Royal Netherlands Academy of Science and Arts, Amsterdam, Netherlands
4Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands
5Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands

Keywords: neural basis of syntactic processing, dependency grammar, phrase structure grammar,
functional MRI, anterior temporal pole, superior temporal gyrus

ABSTRACT
Finding the structure of a sentence—the way its words hold together to convey meaning—is a
fundamental step in language comprehension. Several brain regions, including the left inferior
frontal gyrus, the left posterior superior temporal gyrus, and the left anterior temporal pole, are
supposed to support this operation. The exact role of these areas is nonetheless still debated. In
this paper we investigate the hypothesis that different brain regions could be sensitive to different
kinds of syntactic computations. We compare the fit of phrase-structure and dependency structure
descriptors to activity in brain areas using fMRI. Our results show a division between areas
with regard to the type of structure computed, with the left anterior temporal pole and left inferior
frontal gyrus favouring dependency structures and left posterior superior temporal gyrus
favouring phrase structures.

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

INTRODUCTION

When presented with a sentence, all we have is a sequence of visual or auditory stimuli. A key
assumption within psycholinguistics is that humans parse this sentence, that is, they construct a
syntactic structure that represents the relation between its components. The question we address in
this study is whether different brain regions are sensitive to different kinds of syntactic structure
building. We compare the predictive power of syntactic structural measures derived from two
different families of grammars—dependency grammar and phrase-structure grammar—with regard
to the brain activity in language processing areas recorded during naturalistic text listening.

Brain Areas Underpinning Syntactic Analysis

Sentence processing involves at least two operations: the retrieval of the meaning of single linguistic
units from semantic memory (i.e., the mental lexicon), and the computation of the meaning of the
structures derived from the combination of these more basic units. This second subprocess likely
requires the contribution of some sort of structural analysis, that is, the analysis of the syntactic con-
figuration of the words making up the sentence. In this section we review and motivate a selection
of cortical areas that, not uncontroversially, seem to support structural analysis.

Citation: Lopopolo, A., van den Bosch, A.,
Petersson, K.-M., & Willems, R. M. (2021).
Distinguishing syntactic operations in the
brain: Dependency and phrase-structure
parsing. Neurobiology of Language,
2(1), 152–175. https://doi.org/10.1162
/nol_a_00029

DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00029

Supporting Information:
https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00029

Received: 10 February 2020
Accepted: 16 November 2020

Competing Interests: The authors have
declared that no competing interests
exist.

Corresponding Author:
Alessandro Lopopolo
lopopolo@uni-potsdam.de

Handling Editor:
Liina Pylkkanen

Copyright: © 2021 Massachusetts
Institute of Technology. Published
under a Creative Commons Attribution
4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license.

The MIT Press

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

The literature reports the involvement of a network of mostly left-lateralised cortical regions
including the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), the left posterior superior temporal gyrus (pSTG),
and the left anterior temporal pole (ATP). There is, however, controversy concerning which brain
areas are crucially involved in syntactic processing. A considerable body of literature does report
left IFG and left pSTG activation during syntactic processing as opposed to a baseline, usually
consisting of random sequences of words (Caramazza & Zurif, 1976; Friederici et al., 2005;
Pallier et al., 2011; Snijders et al., 2008; Tyler, Randall, & Stamatakis, 2008; Zaccarella &
Friederici, 2015; Zaccarella et al., 2015). However, several other studies do not report activity
in left IFG and left pSTG (Bemis & Pylkkänen, 2011; Humphries et al., 2006; Rogalsky &
Hickok, 2008), despite using paradigms similar to the above mentioned studies. Moreover, doubts
concerning the effective involvement of these areas in syntactic processing are cast by neuropsy-
chological observations. For instance, lesions to the IFG lead to what is clinically known as Broca’s
aphasia. These aphasic patients do not perform significantly differently from healthy controls on
grammaticality judgments (Linebarger et al., 1983; Wulfeck & Bates, 1991). Similarly, lesion anal-
yses seem to point towards a lack of effect of lesions located in the IFG and pSTG on the perfor-
mance in basic sentence comprehension (Dronkers et al., 2004; Thothathiri et al., 2012). These
studies do not target specific syntactic structures or syntactic structure processing directly.
Nonetheless, both tasks—grammaticality judgement and sentence comprehension—are likely
to require the computation and the analysis of the syntactic structure of the presented stimuli.

Acknowledging this inconsistency in the literature regarding the involvement of frontal and
posterior temporal regions, Matchin et al. (2017) proposed the hypothesis that the left IFG and
pSTG may in fact not play a necessary role in syntactic processing. Instead, they claim that these
areas are involved only in top-down syntactic prediction, supporting further compositional syn-
tactic operations in the left ATP and the left angular gyrus (AG). The idea that there is a division of
labour between the IFG and pSTG, on the one hand, and the ATP and AG on the other was also
suggested by an earlier study by Pallier et al. (2011). In this study, Pallier and colleagues analysed
the activity recorded during natural language sentence and jabberwocky sentence reading.
Searching for brain regions where activation positively correlated with the size of the linguistic
constituents, their results isolated a network of left-hemispheric regions that could be dissociated
into two major subsets. The left IFG and pSTG showed constituent size effects regardless of
whether actual content words were present or were replaced by pseudowords ( jabberwocky
stimuli). On the other hand, the ATP, the anterior superior temporal sulcus, and the temporo-
parietal junction showed constituent size effects only in the presence of lexico-semantic informa-
tion, suggesting that they may encode sentence-level semantic compositionality.

Besides the above mentioned studies, there is a large literature claiming that the left ATP plays
an uncontroversially central role in linguistic processing and has been reported as a central hub for
lexical, semantic, and syntactic compositionality. Several studies have pointed to the involvement
of the left ATP in processing sentence and phrasal structure. By contrasting the activity recorded
during the reading of sentences and of word lists, works such as Mazoyer et al. (1993), Friederici,
Meyer, and von Cramon (2000), Humphries et al. (2006, 2007), and Stowe et al. (1998) reported
an increase in activity in the ATP for sentence comprehension as compared to word lists. The role
of the ATP in processing composition is confirmed by another series of studies which focused on
more specific types of syntactic structures. Rather than looking at sentences as a whole, these anal-
yses focused on simple phrasal processing, consisting of the composition of adjectives and nouns
(e.g., red apple) (Baron & Osherson, 2011; Baron et al., 2010; Bemis & Pylkkänen, 2011; Bemis &
Pylkkänen, 2013). These results are confirmed also for a wider range of phrasal and syntactic com-
positional types and cross-language by Westerlund et al. (2015), and across visual and auditory
modality by Bemis and Pylkkänen (2013).

Neurobiology of Language

153

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Next to its involvement in syntactic processing, the left ATP is also considered central in
semantic memory, a putative subcomponent of long-term memory storing information about
the meaning of linguistic units. The first and most compelling proof of this role of the ATP is given
by studies on semantic dementia, in which patients showing atrophy of the ATP show a significant
impairment in their ability to retrieve and recognise concepts (Hodges, Graham, & Patterson,
1995; Hodges et al., 1992; Mummery et al., 2000; Rogers, Ralph, et al., 2004). This is confirmed
also by a large neuroimaging literature (Bright et al., 2005; Gauthier et al., 1997; Moss et al.,
2004; Rogers, Hocking, et al., 2006; Tyler et al., 2004). These findings were summarised by
Patterson et al. (2007) and led to the formulation of the hub-and-spoke model, which posits that
concepts are represented by a network of sensorimotor representations converging in the left ATP,
which acts as a hub collecting and controlling modality specific features in order to produce su-
pramodal representations. Following the studies on sentential and phrasal processing, and
Patterson’s hub-and-spoke model of semantic memory, it appears that the ATP could play a role
in two distinct kinds of composition: one merging words into larger structures (phrases and sen-
tences), and one composing meaning out of more basic semantic features, possibly grounded in
sensory-motor representations. This led Westerlund and Pylkkänen (2014) to compare the involve-
ment of the ATP between tasks requiring syntactic and lexical semantic processing, concluding that
the two processes might indeed be substantiated by the same cortical mechanism.

What Form of Syntax?

In the previous section we saw how the debate on the cortical involvement during structural sen-
tence analysis generally points to areas in the left IFG, the left pSTG, and the ATP regions. The
question we address in this study is whether they are involved differently in specific syntactic
computations.

In this paper we compare phrase-structure grammars (PSG) (Borsley, 1998; Chomsky, 1957,
1965) and dependency grammars (DG) (Mel’c(cid:1)uk, 1988; Kübler et al., 2009; Tesnière, 2015)
as two types of structure the brain potentially computes as part of sentence comprehension.
The two grammars differ in a number of aspects. DG builds structures solely on the words and
on binary relations holding between them, whereas PSG relies on grouping words into phrases
that can in turn be grouped into larger phrases, implying a hierarchical structure composed by
both surface forms (the words of the sentence) and nonobservable abstract nodes that are
assumed to be computed by the human brain. With regard to this study, our aim is not to prove
that one grammar is a better formalism than the other. We intend to investigate whether and how
the language network in the brain is sensitive to measures derived from both of them. Below
we describe in more detail the way these two kinds of grammar differ from each other. To the
best of our knowledge, in the field of neurobiology of language, only the present work and the
work by Li and Hale (2019) address the two grammars together in the same study. Measures
derived from PSG alone have been used in various works (e.g., Brennan et al., 2016; Frank
et al., 2012; Nelson et al., 2017).

Inspired by the previous literature we conducted a region of interest (ROI) analysis focusing
on the left IFG (pars opercularis, triangularis, and orbitalis separately), the left ATP, and the left
STG. We adopted the standard parcellation provided by the commonly used automated anatom-
ical labeling atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2001), which does not divide the STG into subregions.
Moreover, the reason for focusing on the IFG subparts is based on Hagoort (2005), which pro-
poses a division of labor inside the IFG depending on the type of binding mechanism performed.
We fitted separate linear mixed-effect (LME) models predicting the activity recorded in these
areas during naturalistic language listening, using as regressors of interest the structural measures

Phrase-structure grammar (PSG):
Formalism describing the structure
of a sentence by grouping words
(terminal nodes) into phrases
(non-terminal nodes), and phrases
into higher-level phrases.

Dependency grammar (DG):
Formalism describing the structure
of a sentence by assigning binary
head-dependent relations
between pairs of words.

Neurobiology of Language

154

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

mentioned above. Note that our regressors specify the amount of syntactic processing at each
word in our stimuli. These analyses allowed us to identify which area is more sensitive to which
type of structural description (PSG or DG). We then conducted a psychophysiological interaction
(PPI) analysis investigating how the interaction between each of our ROIs and the rest of the brain
is modulated by its preferred structural description from the previous lme-analysis.

Our study is clearly related to the work of Brennan et al. (2016), which showed that activity in
the anterior and posterior portions of the left temporal cortex can be predicted using metrics
derived from the phrasal structure of the stimuli. Our study is also related to Li and Hale
(2019), which followed up on Brennan and colleagues’ study and introduced a measure (struc-
tural distance) that combines information from both the phrasal structure and the dependency
structure of the stimuli and is intended as a complexity metric quantifying the difficulty implied
in memory retrieval. The metric is obtained by counting the number of phrases connecting
two words linked by a dependency relation, and it is shown to be able to explain activity in
the right anterior and left posterior temporal lobes. Nonetheless, the present study differs
from Brennan et al. (2016) and Li and Hale (2019) by separately investigating the effect of
dependency and phrasal structure processing in the brain. More specifically, we use measures
derived from DG and PSG that are explicitly, and on purpose, kept distinct, under the hypothesis
that different parts of the brain might be differentially sensitive to them. Moreover, from a
theoretical point of view, Li and Hale’s structural metric is intended as a complexity metric
quantifying the difficulty implied in memory retrieval. In contrast, we interpret our metrics
instead as correlates of the number of operations necessary to integrate each word into its struc-
tural context. Therefore, they are intended as a measure of the effort required by syntactic
integration.

Syntactic Parsing

The assumption presented in the Introduction is that in order to interpret a sentence, the human
brain has to establish relations between the words that compose it. For instance, words alone, in
isolation, cannot convey the full description of a situation or a state. The following list of words—
paper, you, this, and read—becomes a suitable description of the action you are performing now
only if the relations that the predicate read entertains with the subject you and the object this
paper (in turn substantiated by the relation between the determiner this and the noun paper)
are established by your brain. The set of structural relations between the words constitutes the
syntactic structure of the sentence. The process that allows you to compute such structure (i.e.,
to derive relations given a sequence of words) is usually referred to as parsing. The meaning of
words, their grammatical category, their relations, and their dependencies with one another
determine the interpretation of the sentence they constitute.

Syntactic Structures

We distinguish between two main approaches to characterize the syntactic structure of a
sentence PSGs and DGs. Given a sentence, both grammars produce a hierarchical structure
linking or grouping the words in a structure rooted in a governing node (the root node). The main
difference between the two is that PSG assumes the existence of phrase structures grouping and
governing pairs (if the parse tree is binary) of words, whereas DG relies only on word pairings
linked by syntactic relations. At a high level of abstraction, what sets a DG structure apart from
one derived according to PSG is the fact that DG structures are flatter than PSG structures because
they lack phrasal constituents. The structure only consists of the words in a sentence and an
associated set of directed binary grammatical relations that hold among them. The only nodes

Neurobiology of Language

155

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Terminal nodes:
The surface form elements in the
sentence structure according to PSG
(i.e., the words of the sentence).

in the DG structure are terminal nodes corresponding to surface lexical items as they are encoun-
tered by the human reader; no non-terminal, non-observed abstract nodes are introduced. In the
following section we expand on the fundamental differences between PSG and DG from both a
structural and theoretical point of view.

Phrase structure

Phrase-structure grammars define parse structures of sentences as trees composed by terminal
and non-terminal nodes. Non-terminal nodes correspond—usually—to phrasal categories as
defined by the grammar in use, while terminal nodes (the leaf nodes of the tree) are assigned
to the surface forms of the parsed sentence (i.e., its words). Phrase nodes are assigned labels
corresponding to syntactic phrasal categories such as noun phrase (NP), verb phrase ( VP), adver-
bial phrase (AP), and determiner phrase (DP).

If the tree is binary (in our definition of a phrase-structure parse we adopt only binarized trees)
phrasal nodes can have a maximum of two child nodes that can be either other phrasal nodes or
leaf nodes (words). A parent node can only consist of a phrasal node; it is also referred as non-
terminal. Words can only be children of non-terminal phrasal nodes and are referred to as termi-
nal or leaf nodes because they are not hierarchically higher than any other node. Besides phrasal
and leaf nodes, the phrase-structure parse also contains a root node. A root node is a node that is
not a child of any other node. Given a parse of a sentence, the resulting tree contains only one root
node. The root note corresponds to the category S, governing the sentence as a whole.

(1) The man saw a brown dog in the park.

As an example, as displayed in Figure 1, the parse of Sentence 1 contains eight labeled phrase
structures, including S, and constitutes a nested binary-branching tree. The words of the sentence
(the, man, saw, a, brown, dog, in, the, and park) correspond to the terminal nodes. Following the
structure of the parse tree in a top-down fashion: S branches into NP and VP, respectively. The
left-hand child (NP) is composed of a determiner leaf node the and a noun man; whereas the right-
hand child of S ( VP) has in turn as left-hand child a terminal node (the finite verb saw) and as its
right-hand side child another noun phrase (NP). This last NP branches into another NP and a

Figure 1. Phrase-structure parse of Sentence 1.

Neurobiology of Language

156

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

prepositional phrase (PP). These two last phrases both split into a left-hand terminal child (respec-
tively a and in) and into NP as a right-hand child. The latter two are both composed of terminal
nodes (brown, dog, the, and park).

Dependency structure

Dependency grammar describes a sentence as a set of relations between pairs of words—a head
and a dependent—composing it. The relations can be called dependencies and correspond to
grammatical functions. The relations, and the words they link, are the only elements composing
the structure (Kübler et al., 2009; Mel’cuk, 1988; Tesnière, 2015). In a dependency structure the
finite verb is often taken to be the structural hub of the sentence. All other words are either directly
or indirectly connected to the verb by dependencies.

Take for instance Sentence 1 above. The dependency graph in Figure 2 represents the depen-
dency structure of the sentence in terms of typified head-dependent relations: The main verb
(saw) acts as head for man and dog, with which the verb is in a subject and an object relation
respectively. A dependent of one dependency relation can in turn be the head of another. For
instance dog is head of brown, with which it is linked by a modifier relation, and also head of
article a via a determiner relation. Dependencies can be instantiated between words far apart in
the sequential structure of the sentence.

Relations that hold between words are captured in structurally different manners by depen-
dency structure and phrase-structure parses of the same sentence. Take for instance the relation
between saw and dog, respectively the main verb and the direct object in Sentence 1. As it is
apparent from the graph path between these two items in Figure 3, DS directly captures their
predicate-object relation by means of a simple directed edge (Figure 3b), whereas PS relies on
three intervening noun phrases and a governing verb phrase (Figure 3a).

The Relation Between Dependency and Phrase-Structure Grammars

Dependency grammar and phrase-structure grammar are two different syntactic formalisms, using
different structural primitives (dependency relations and phrases). There has been some discus-
sion in the field of theoretical linguistics with regard to whether they capture the same information
or to what degree the structures they sanction are equivalent (Hays, 1964; Jung, 1998).

Discussing the linguistic information the two grammars capture, Rambow (2010) notes that,
from a theoretical linguistic point of view, dependency and phrase-structure describe distinct
syntactic entities, and thus are not strictly equivalent. Dependencies capture direct relations
between words, identical to thematic functions such as subject, object, modifier, etc. Syntactic
phrase-structure, on the other hand, is not so much about functional relations between words,
but about the recursive grouping of sentence constituents (words and phrases), such that at each
level, each grouping acts as a syntactic unit (Schneider, 1998). Moreover, according to Jung (1998)

Figure 2. Dependency parse of Sentence 1.

Neurobiology of Language

157

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Figure 3. Comparison between the structures mediating the relation between the sentence’s main
verb (saw) and its object (dog) in the phrase-structure (a) and dependency structure (b) parses of
Sentence 1.

only dependencies can express the syntactic word-to-word relations of a sentence, but only con-
stituency can express the linear order of a sentence. Jung, therefore, sees the two grammars as
complementary and not equivalent.

Following these last observations, we consider dependency and phrase-structure distinct,

and the type of information that they capture as nonequivalent.

Hypotheses Concerning Brain Areas Involvement

Li and Hale (2019) showed that after controlling for structural distance (which combines both
phrasal and dependency information), phrase structural predictors do not significantly contribute
to the explanation of activity in the anterior temporal lobe. We hypothesize that predictors solely
based on DG might significantly predict activity in the anterior temporal lobe. This intuition is also
grounded on the results reported by Westerlund et al. (2015), which showed modulation of ATP
activity as a function of the type of syntactic relation entertained by pairs of words (e.g., adjective-
noun, adverb-verb). Word pairs, entertaining relations comparable to dependency relations,
elicited larger activation in the left ATP compared to random pairings of words, suggesting that
this area might play a role in syntactic processing in lines with DG.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants and Stimuli

We re-analysed data from an fMRI study on language comprehension of auditory presented
narrative texts (Lopopolo, Frank, van den Bosch, Nijhof, & Willems, 2018). Here we briefly
present the data collection procedure, preprocessing, and stimuli employed. Full details can
be found in the original articles (Lopopolo, Frank, van den Bosch, Nijhof, & Willems, 2018;
Willems et al., 2016). (The dataset is available at https://osf.io/utpdy/.)

Twenty-four healthy, native speakers of Dutch (8 males; mean age 22.9, range 18–31), without
psychiatric or neurological problems, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and without
hearing problems, took part in the experiment. All participants except one were right-handed
by self-report, and all participants were naive with respect to the purpose of the experiment.
Written informed consent was obtained in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and

Neurobiology of Language

158

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

the study was approved by the local ethics committee (Central Committee on Research Involving
Human Subjects, CMO region Arnhem–Nijmegen, The Netherlands, protocol number 2001/
095). Participants were paid either in money or in course credit at the end of the study.

Stimuli consisted of three excerpts from three distinct literary novels extracted from the Spoken
Dutch Corpus (Corpus Gesproken Nederlands [CGN]; (Oostdijk, 2000). We used the audio
recordings of these texts, and no other data or metadata from the CGN was used for our analyses.
The excerpts were spoken at a normal rate, in a quiet room, by female speakers (one speaker per
story). Stimulus durations were 3:49 min (622 words), 7:50 min (1,291 words), and 7:48 min
(1,131 words). Reversed speech versions of the stories were created with Audacity 2.03
(https://www.audacityteam.org/). These were used as a low-level baseline in the analysis.

Procedure

Participants passively listened to the three narratives and their reversed versions (for a total of six
runs) inside the MRI scanner. Each story and its reversed speech counterpart were presented
following each other. Half the participants started with a non-reversed stimulus, and half with
a reversed speech stimulus. Participants were instructed to listen to the materials attentively,
which in practice is only possible for the three narratives, and not for the reversed speech coun-
terparts. There was a short break after each fragment.

Stimuli were presented with Presentation 16.2 (https://www.neurobs.com/). Auditory stimuli
were presented through MRI-compatible earphones. In order to make sure participants could
correctly perceive the stimuli, the actual experimental sessions were preceded by an in-scanner
volume test, in which a fragment from another story with a comparable voice and sound quality
was presented and the volume was adjusted to the optimal level based on feedback from the
participant.

After the scanning session, participants were tested for their memory and comprehension of
the stories. The participants were not informed in advance about the test in order to avoid
attentional biases during the passive listening to the stories.

FMRI Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

Images of blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) changes were acquired on a 3-T Siemens
Magnetom Trio scanner (Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. Pillows and tape
were used to minimize participants’ head movement, and the earphones that were used for
presenting the stories reduced scanner noise. Functional images were acquired using a fast
T2*-weighted 3D echo planar imaging sequence (Poser et al., 2010), with high temporal resolu-
tion (time to repetition: 880 ms, time to echo: 28 ms, flip angle: 14°, voxel size: 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.5 mm,
36 slices). High resolution (1 × 1 × 1.25 mm) structural (anatomical) images were acquired using
a T1 sequence.

Preprocessing was performed using SPM8 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and MATLAB
2010b (https://www.mathworks.nl/). The first four volumes were removed to control for T1 equil-
ibration effects. Rigid body registration was used to realign images. Images were realigned to the
first image within each run. The mean of the motion-corrected images was then brought into the
same space as the individual participant’s anatomical scan. The anatomical and functional scans
were spatially normalized to the standard MNI template, and functional images were re-sampled
to 2 × 2 × 2 mm voxel sizes. Finally, an isotropic 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian
kernel was used to spatially smooth the motion-corrected and normalized data.

Neurobiology of Language

159

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Parser:
A software that computes the structure
(the parse) of a sentence according to a
grammar.

Syntactic Measures

Both dependency and phrase-structure parses of the sentences composing the stimulus texts
were derived using a computational parser developed and trained for Dutch (ALPINO; van
Noord, 2006). ALPINO has been profusely used in several studies requiring syntactic analyses
of Dutch linguistic material, ranging from natural language processing to psycholinguistic and
neurolinguistic studies. We have expanded the section describing the parser with a more compre-
hensive review of previous neurolinguistic and psycholinguistic studies using this parser to derive
measures from phrase-structure and dependency grammars. More specifically, Bastiaanse et al.
(2009) used ALPINO to generate dependency structures from corpus data used to investigate
the effects of frequency and complexity on agrammatic production. Similarly, Lopopolo, Frank,
van den Bosch, and Willems (2019) used ALPINO to study the relationship between dependency
structures and patterns of eye movement during reading, demonstrating the validity of ALPINO as
a tool for DG-based structural analyses. On the other hand, Brouwer et al., 2010 used ALPINO to
obtain PSG representations in their study of Dutch syntactically ambiguous structures. Moreover,
studies conducted by Kos and colleagues (Kos, van den Brink, & Hagoort, 2012; Kos, van den
Brink, Snijders, et al., 2012; Kos, Vosse, van den Brink, & Hagoort, 2010) made extensive use
of the Dutch treebank (CLEF corpus), which was obtained by using ALPINO (van der Beek
et al., 2002).

The grammar implemented by ALPINO grammar is a wide-coverage head-driven phrase
structure grammar. Nonetheless it has been augmented to enable it to output dependency
structures compatible with our definition of DG and based on the guidelines of the CGN
(Oostdijk, 2000).

The output of ALPINO is able to return sentence parses consistent with the principles of PSG,
as well as DG. Moreover, it is able to generate both these two types of parses from within the same
framework, making it convenient and allowing us to avoid inconsistencies derived from using
different parsers built and trained on different data. From each of these parse structures we derive
a measure approximating the operations performed in order to integrate each word into the syn-
tactic structure computed at the point of its presentation. The next sections will describe these
measures in detail.

Dependency parse

In order to describe the dependency structure of a sentence, the ALPINO parser creates a structure
composed by dependency triples consisting of a head word, the type of dependency relation, and
its dependent word. A parse is produced for each sentence independently; therefore, no relation
can be assigned between words belonging to different sentences.

In order to describe the operation required to integrate a word at a time into the incrementally
built dependency structure of the sentence we adopted the number of left-hand side relations
entertained by each word. As described above, every word in a sentence entertains at least
one relation with another word in the same sentence. Every non-final and non-initial word can
have relations with a variable number of other words on its right and its left. Logically, a sentence-
initial word can only have relations with words to its right, and a sentence-final word can only be
linked to words on its left. In order to quantify the operations required to integrate a word w into
the structure constructed up to its presentation, only relations with a head and possible depen-
dents on the left-hand side of w are counted. In other words, from the dependency structure of a
sentence, we count the number of left hand-side edges for each word w in the sentence (depen-
dency structure left relations, or DSlrels, see Table 1).

Neurobiology of Language

160

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Table 1. Number of left-hand dependency relations (DSlrels) per word w in the example Sentence 1

DSlrels

The
0

man
1

saw
1

a
0

brown
0

dog
3

in
1

the
0

park
1

For example, the word dog in the sentence has two dependent relations with two words to its
left (a and brown), no dependents to its right, but one head to its left (saw). The word park, being
sentence-final, does not have any links on its right, but it has one head (in) and one dependent
(the) to its left. From a neurobiological point of view, the assumption is that all dependency
relations have equal cost. Under our hypothesis, each relation engenders an equivalent BOLD
response independently from its type and the distance between head and dependents.

Phrase-structure parse

As for the phrase-structure parse, the texts of the three stories presented to the participants
were fed to the ALPINO toolbox for Dutch natural language processing in order to generate
this time a phrase-structure parse for each sentence (van Noord, 2006).

In order to quantify the number of syntactic operations per word required to construct a
phrase-structure parse of the input sentence, we measured the number of closed phrase structures
allowed after the introduction of each novel word (PSxps). Such a measure is computed by con-
sidering whether a word or phrase is a right-hand or left-hand side child of its parent phrasal node.
In case the word in question is the right-hand side child, the parent phrasal node is considered
complete and therefore closed. This proceeds recursively, evaluating whether a closed phrasal
node is in its turn the right-hand side child of a higher order parent phrasal node, allowing it to be
closed. For instance, according to the phrase-structure parse of Sentence 1 (Figure 1), the first
instance of the word The is the left-hand side child of an NP structure; for this reason this NP is
not complete and cannot be closed. Therefore, the value of PSxps for The is 0. On the other hand,
man is the right-hand side child of the same NP and therefore this phrase structure can be closed
at this word position, allowing the assignment of value 1 to man. Following the same reasoning,
dog is the left-hand side child of another NP allowing for its closure. This last NP is in turn the
left-hand side of a higher NP structure. Therefore the word dog is assigned value 2 because its
presentation allows for the completion of two nested phrase structures. Table 2 contains the
PSxps values for the whole Sentence 1.

This measure is computed under the following simplifying assumptions: that phrase-structure
trees are binary (i.e., as explained above, that they can have only two children), and that parsing
proceeds incrementally left-to-right. Similarly to what was stated for dependency relations
above, the assumption is that all the integration of phrase structures have equal cost. Under this
assumption, each phrase engenders an equivalent BOLD response independently from its type
(NP, VP, etc.) and distance from the root node.

Controlling for Lexical Frequency and Word Surprisal

To control for other factors known to influence brain activation during language comprehen-
sion, we added log-transformed lexical frequency and surprisal as covariates to the analysis

Table 2. Number of closed phrase structures (PSxps) per each word w in the example Sentence 1

PSxps

The
0

man
1

saw
0

a
0

brown
0

dog
2

in
0

the
0

park
5

Neurobiology of Language

161

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

(Lopopolo, Frank, van den Bosch, & Willems, 2017; Willems et al., 2016). Log2-transformed
lexical frequency per word was computed using the SUBTLEX NL corpus (Keuleers et al.,
2010). Surprisal was computed from a second-order Markov model, also known as a trigram
model, trained on a random selection of 10 million sentences (comprising 197 million word
tokens; 2.1 million types) from the Dutch Corpus of Web (NLCOW2012; Schäfer & Bildhauer,
2012). Surprisal of word wt is the negative logarithm of the conditional probability of encoun-
tering wt after having read sequence wt−2, wt−1, or −log P(wt|wt−2, wt−1). The computation was
performed by the SRILM toolbox (Stolcke, 2002).

Analyses

Our main analysis consists of ROI-wise linear model fitting using as predictors the syntactic struc-
ture measures described in the previous sections, together with lexical frequency and surprisal as
regressors of no interest.

Besides ROI analyses, we also conducted whole-brain PPI analyses and whole-brain general
linear model analyses. The former was performed in order to test the interaction between the
regions of the language network and the rest of the brain with regard to the type of syntactic struc-
ture considered in this study. The latter was instead performed in order to have a wider—less
biased—view of the possible division of labour between dependency and phrase-structure
parsing in the brain.

ROI Analyses

We chose six separate left-hemisphere anatomical ROIs to selectively test the contribution of our
two syntactic measures as predictors of BOLD activity. These regions were the STG (including
Wernicke’s area), the middle temporal pole (mATP), the superior temporal pole (sATP), the IFG
pars opercularis (IFG_oper), the IFG pars triangularis (IFG_tri), and the IFG pars orbitalis
(IFG_orb). The regions are defined following the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas
(Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2001) as implemented in SPM12. We then computed the average
BOLD signal for each of our 24 participants and six ROIs.

For each of the six ROIs, we fitted three LME models predicting the average BOLD signal. The first
model (Base, 2 below) contains as predictors only probabilistic information (lexical frequency and
surprisal) relative to each word. Estimates from the motion-correction algorithm (three rotations and
three translations per run) were additionally added as regressors of no interest. In order to assess the
effect of dependency and phrase-structure measures to each ROI’s BOLD signal, models 3 and 4
were fitted with one of our syntactic measures (DSlrels, PSxps) each in addition to the same covar-
iates of the Base model. In addition, we included by-subject random intercepts, as well as the by-
subject random slopes for surprisal and log-transformed word frequency.

(2) Base model: BOLD 1 + lexf req + surprisal + m1 + m2 + m3 + m4 + m5 + m6 + (1|

subject) + (1 + surprisal|subject) + (1 + lexf req|subject)

(3) DSlrels model: BOLD 1 + lexf req + surprisal + DSlrels + m1 + m2 + m3 + m4 + m5 +

m6 + (1|subject) + (1 + surprisal|subject) + (1 + lexf req|subject)

(4) PSxps model: BOLD 1 + lexf req + surprisal + P Sxps + m1 + m2 + m3 + m4 + m5 + m6 +

(1|subject) + (1 + surprisal|subject) + (1 + lexf req|subject)

We compared the syntactic models (DSlrels and PSxps) against the Base model in order to test
whether the introduction of the syntactic measure significantly improved the fit to the data. We
also directly compared the DSlrels and the PSxps models in order to test for specific syntactic

Neurobiology of Language

162

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

structure selectivity in our six ROIs. Model comparisons were performed using the likelihood-
ratio test. The models were fit by maximum likelihood.

PPI Analysis

The ROI analysis introduced above was aimed at determining the contribution of our structural
measures to the activity of left inferior frontal, superior temporal, and antero-temporal regions
of the brain—areas that are claimed to be responsible for structural analysis of linguistic stim-
uli. In order to investigate the interplay between these (and other) brain regions, we addition-
ally investigated PPIs. PPI is a functional brain connectivity analysis method, developed to
estimate context-dependent changes in functional connectivity cortical areas (Friston, 2011;
Friston, Büchel, et al., 1997). It models the way brain activity is determined by the activity of
a preselected seed region when modulated by experimental conditions or parameters (modula-
tor). The analysis takes the activity of the seed region (the physiological component) and a mod-
ulator (the psychological component) and fits a voxel-wise linear model using as predictor of
interest the product of these two components (the psychophysiological interaction). In this
way the PPI identifies brain regions whose activity depends on an interaction between psycho-
logical context (the task) and physiological state (the time course of brain activity) of the seed
region (O’Reilly et al., 2012).

The activity of each seed region was computed by fitting a general linear model containing as
predictors, our structural measures, and as covariates, lexical frequency and surprisal and para-
metric head movement. The eigenvalue of the voxels inside the ROI showing suprathreshold
activation for the regressor of interest was used to compute the physiological component of
the PPI. This was conducted at single-subject level with a significance level of p < 0.05. The regressor of interest used for ROI-wise voxel selection acted also as the psychological modulator for the subsequent PPI analyses proper, which consisted of fitting another subject-level whole- brain general linear model using as regressor of interest the product of the seed activity and the modulator measures, and as covariates the seed activity and modulator themselves. The goal was to then identify those voxels (both at single subject and group level) that respond significantly to the interaction between seed activity and modulator. Whole-Brain Analysis At the single-subject level, the observed BOLD time course in each voxel was subjected to a regres- sion analysis, testing for voxels in which the covariates of interest (DSlrels, PSxps) explain a signif- icant proportion of variance of that voxel time course (Friston, Holmes, et al., 1995). Before the actual analysis, one regressor modelling the duration of each single word was created for each story. This regressor was convolved with the hemodynamic response function to account for the delay in BOLD activation respective to stimulus presentation. Besides the covariates of interest, log-transformed lexical frequency per word—computed using the SUBTLEX-NL corpus (Keuleers et al., 2010)—and per-word surprisal were introduced. They were used as regressor of no interest to statistically factor out effects of stochastic properties of the words. The estimates from the mo- tion correction algorithm (three rotations and three translations per run) were additionally added as regressors of no interest. We were interested in assessing which voxels are more sensitive to DSlrels as compared to PSxps and vice versa. In order to do so, we contrasted these two regressors of interest in order to identify voxels that are selective for each one of the regressors over and above the contribution of the other (DSlrels > PSxps, PSxps > DSlrels). The significance of these contrasts was assessed by computing the
t statistic over participants of this difference score for each voxel in the brain. The resulting multiple

Neurobiology of Language

163

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Table 3.
of two syntactic measures derived either from dependency and phrase-structure parses

Likelihood ratio test between the Base model and each of the two models fitted with one

DSlrels

PSxps

ROI
STG

mATP

sATP

IFG_oper

IFG_tri

IFG_orb

(cid:1)2

1.1009

12.7514

2.0497

3.8301

2.9034

0.7947

p
0.2941

0.0003*

0.1522

0.0500

0.0884

0.3727

(cid:1)2

41.8453

0.7733

0.2541

0.8991

0.3062

0.3617

p
0.0000*

0.3792

0.6142

0.3430

0.5800

0.5476

Note. * p < 0.002, Bonferroni-adjusted alpha 0.05/18, 6 ROIs and 3 models. comparison problem was solved by means of combining a p < 0.05 voxel threshold with a cluster extent threshold determined by means of 1,000 Monte Carlo simulations, after estimation of the smoothness of the data (Slotnick et al., 2003) applied for each separate contrast both for the single and the total models. Clusters of a size exceeding the number of voxel thresholds corresponded to statistically significant effects ( p < 0.05 level, corrected for multiple comparisons). l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 1 1 5 2 1 9 0 8 8 7 9 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 2 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 RESULTS ROI Analyses Comparison against the Base model We computed the likelihood-ratio test for the difference in fit between the Base model and each of the two syntactic models above across the six ROIs. This allowed us to test whether the introduc- tion of syntactic measures significantly improves the fit of the LME model to the BOLD signal. The results of these analyses are reported in Table 3 and Figure 4. Figure 4. Cortical illustration of the likelihood ratio test between the Base model and the DSlrels model (a), and between the Base model and PSxps model (b). Green indicates ROIs where the likelihood ratio test returns significant results after Bonferroni correction (adjusted alpha = 0.002, 0.05/18, 6 ROIs and 3 models). Red indicates ROIs for which the syntactic measure does not significantly improve fit. Note the different selectivity between anterior and posterior temporal areas of the network with regard to the type of syntactic structure. Neurobiology of Language 164 Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain Table 4. Likelihood ratio test between dependency relations and phrase structures Model comparison PSxps > Dslrels

DSlrels > PSxps

ROI

STG

mATP

IFG_oper

IFG_tri

(cid:1)2

40.7176

11.9757

2.9306

2.5970

p
0.0009*

0.0009*

0.0430

0.0540

Note. * p < 0.004, Bonferroni-adjusted alpha 0.05/12, 6 ROIs and 2 models. Tests were conducted using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels of 0.002 per test (0.05/18, 6 ROIs, and 3 models). The results indicate that there was a strong effect of PSxps in the left STG (41.8453, p = 0.0000). On the other hand, the DG structure building measure—DSlrels—had a strong effect in the left middle TP (12.7514, p = 0.0003). A weaker effect of DSlrels was also present on the left IFG pars opercularis (3.8301, p = 0.0500), but not significant after Bonferroni correction. Comparison between models Table 4 contains the results of the log-likelihood test between the DSlrels and PSxps models. Syntactic operation measures of DSlrels and PSxps were directly compared to assess the prom- inence of one or the other as predictor of activity inside our ROI pool. Tests were conducted using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels of 0.004 per test (0.05/12, 6 ROIs, and 2 models). The left STG confirms a strong preference for the phrase-structure measure, with the model fitted with PSxps significantly outperforming the model fitted with DSlrels. DSlrels instead outperforms its PSG counterpart in the middle ATP and in the pars opercularis and trian- gularis of the IFG, yet not surviving Bonferroni correction. PPI Analyses l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 1 1 5 2 1 9 0 8 8 7 9 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 2 9 p d . / l The results presented in the previous section highlight a preferential selectivity for dependency structure in the left ATP and IFG, and a selectivity for phrase structure in the left STG. In this section we present the results of PPI analyses aimed at assessing the relation between activity in our ROIs as modulated by the processing of either phrase structure or dependency structure. Since the STG showed selectivity for phrase structures, and the IFG and ATP for depen- dency structures, we conducted three separate whole-brain PPI analyses. We first checked for brain areas in which the activity was driven by STG activity (physiological seed) modulated by f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Table 5. Results of the PPI analyses using as seed the left STG and as modulator PSxps Area left IFG (triangularis) left PCG (premotor) left MTG (auditory) left CS (primary motor) right CS (primary motor) right PCG (premotor) MNI coord. −50 22 0 −58 −4 28 −34 −30 10 −54 −12 36 36 −14 38 36 −12 45 T 3.08 3.16 3.12 3.00 4.20 3.62 Cluster size 94 2922 p 0.003 0.002 0.003 0.003 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 10896 Neurobiology of Language 165 Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain Figure 5. Results of the PPI analyses using as seed the left STG and as modulator PSxps. PSxps structural measure (psychological modulator). We then used the activity of either IFG or ATP as physiological seeds, and DSlrels as modulator in order to assess the contribution of these areas and structure to the activity of the rest of the brain during language processing. Table 5 and Figure 5 report the results of the PPI analyses using STG as physiological seed and PSxps as activity modulator. The results highlighted large clusters in the bilateral central sulci (CS) and precentral gyri (PCG) encompassing both bilateral primary motor and premotor cortices. Activation was also observed for the bilateral posterior temporal and perisylvian cortices. Interestingly, activity in the left IFG was also driven by the interaction between the activity in the left STG and the PSxps measure. Tables 6 and 7, and Figures 6 and 7 report the results of using DSlrels as modulator and ATP and IFG as physiological seeds respectively. These results indicate that the activity of the left ATP, modulated by DSlrels, explains the activity in a limited set of clusters located in the bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC). Activity in the left IFG modulated by DSlrels explains the activity in the left supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and AG in the posterior perisylvian cortex. Activity in the bilateral anterior PFC is also driven by this interaction, as is part of the middle banks of the right STG. Table 6. Results of the PPI analyses using as seed the left ATP and as modulator DSlrels Area left caudate MNI coord. −24 0 20 right dorsolateral PFC 44 26 40 T 3.27 3.34 p 0.002 0.002 Cluster size 3640 142 Table 7. Results of the PPI analyses using as seed the left IFG and as modulator DSlrels Area left IFG (triangularis) left SMG left AG left anterior PFC right anterior PFC right SPG MNI coord. −58 16 6 −54 −40 34 −64 −42 26 −44 44 20 44 44 22 30 −66 62 T 3.64 3.71 3.18 3.36 3.72 3.29 p p < 0.001 p < 0.001 0.002 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 0.002 Cluster size 100 625 484 1014 512 166 Neurobiology of Language l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 1 1 5 2 1 9 0 8 8 7 9 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 2 9 p d . / l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain Figure 6. Results of the PPI analyses using as seed the left ATP and as modulator DSlrels. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u n o / l / l a r t i c e - p d f / / / / 2 1 1 5 2 1 9 0 8 8 7 9 n o _ a _ 0 0 0 2 9 p d / . l f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 7. Results of the PPI analyses using as seed the left IFG and as modulator DSlrels. Whole-Brain Analyses Tables 8 and 9 contain the results of the whole-brain analysis contrasting PSG and DG measures. Contrasting PSxps and DSlrels (PSxps > DSlrels, 8) highlights the role of the IFG (orbitalis),
AG, fusiform, and hippocampus in the left hemisphere, and of the bilateral occipital cortex.

Table 8. Whole-brain results areas that are more sensitive to PSxps as compared to DSlrels
(PSxps > DSlrels)

Area
left hippocampus

left MTG

left fusiform

left occipital

left AG

left IFG (orbitalis)

right occipital

MNI coord.
−32 −14 −18

−34 8 −22

−52 −42 −14

−12 −90 −10

−30 −72 38

−20 30 −10

42 −80 8

T
5.32

3.36

4.14

3.88

3.78

3.58

3.48

p
p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 Cluster size 701 83 1637 198 2046 167 Neurobiology of Language Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain Table 9. Whole-brain results areas that are more sensitive to DSlrels as compared to PSxps (DSlrels > PSxps)

Area
left AG

right caudate

left SFG

right posterior CC

MNI coord.
−40 −58 22

6 14 8

−12 52 26

12 −22 38

T
5.00

4.56

3.12

3.32

p
p < 0.001 p < 0.001 0.002 p < 0.001 Cluster size 9207 216 139 On the other hand, contrasting DSlrels and PSxps in the other direction (Dslrels > PSxps, 9)

indicates an involvement of the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and the right caudate.

DISCUSSION

The goal of our experiment was to investigate whether parts of the brain that have previously been
implicated in syntax are sensitive to different types of syntactic operations involved in parsing
sentences. We investigated whether brain activity of six left hemispheric regions was better explained
by DG or by PSG. The two grammars were not meant to represent contrasting hypotheses, and our
aim was not to prove that one is a better formalism than the other. Our results suggest that both
grammars can explain variance in cortical areas supposedly involved in syntactic processing,
and that they appear to do so for different areas of the brain.

These results partially differ from those reported by Li and Hale (2019). Their structural dis-
tance metric explains activity in the right anterior and left posterior temporal cortex.
Considering that the PSxps instead explained activation in the left STG, whereas the DSlrels
explained activation in the left ATP, their observation that structural distance shows activation
in both anterior and posterior superior temporal areas might be due to the fact that it implicitly
contains information conflating the dependency and phrasal structure of the stimuli. We be-
lieve that these differences are caused by a different use of information from dependency and
phrase-structure grammars in the two studies. On the other hand, Li and Hale’s context-free
grammar-based measure (comparable to our PSxps) does explain activity in the left posterior
temporal lobe, which we believe is in line with our results showing the PSxps explaining activity
in the left STG.

A Syntactic Division of Labour

The results of a series of ROI analyses show that dependency structure measures significantly
explain activity in the left ATP, and that phrase-structure measures seem instead to explain activ-
ity in the left STG. These results overall seem to point towards a general division of labour be-
tween anterior temporal areas (responsible for the computation of dependency representations of
the sentence) and more posterior temporal areas involved, instead, in phrase-structure analysis.

The Role of the Anterior Temporal Lobe

Our results indicate a relation between ATP activity and the number of left-hand side dependency
relations at the word level, which was adopted as a quantification of the operation the human
brain is supposed to carry out in order to integrate each word into the dependency parse of the
sentence that it is embedded in. This therefore seems to indicate that the ATP acts as a combina-
torial hub that binds together words according to relations similar to the ones characterizing a
dependency parse. These results are in line with previous studies that describe this area as a

Neurobiology of Language

168

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

hub for composition during sentence comprehension (Friederici et al., 2000; Humphries et al.,
2006, 2007; Mazoyer et al., 1993; Stowe et al., 1998).

Dependency structure, composed by binary typified relations, might also be close to the two-
word stimuli that were found to elicit activation in the ATP by Baron et al. (2010) and by
Westerlund et al. (2015). Baron et al. (2010) observed a modulation of activity in this region when
adjective–noun pairs were presented. The relations of adjectives and nouns are directly captured
by the modification relations in the type of dependency parses we adopted in this study. Similarly,
an interesting parallelism between the typified relations that constitute dependency graphs and
the types of two-word stimuli presented in Westerlund et al. (2015) might help in understanding
why dependency structure might be a correct way of characterizing the type of job performed
by the ATP. Dependency relations directly link pairs of words according to the type of role they
play in a sentential–semantic relationship. They can be grouped—broadly speaking—in verb-
argument relations (i.e., the relations established between a predicate and its subject, object,
or complement, or modifier relations).

Modifier-type dependency relations can be exemplified by the link between an adjective and
a noun, an adverb and a verb, a determiner and a noun, and so on. Westerlund et al. (2015)
demonstrated that a wide range of “composition modes” affect the activity of the left ATP.
These modes consist of two-word sequences classified either as modification (Adjective-Noun
Adverb-Verb Adverb-Adjective) or argument saturation ( Verb-Noun Preposition-Noun
Determiner-Noun). These modes (aside from the fact that they involve word pairs) resemble
the classification of the dependency relation types. Therefore it seems natural to suggest that,
on the basis of our results and the results found in the literature, DG offers a reasonable formal-
ization of the type of structure employed (or constructed) in the left ATP.

Our results therefore confirm that the left ATP can serve as the locus where sentence-level
semantic representations are computed, and that these representations might be produced by
combining the sentence constituent words following the structure specified in a fashion compa-
rable to a dependency parse.

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

The Role of the Left IFG

The ROI analyses show a small effect of dependency structure measures on the activity in the left
IFG (pars opercularis). This area plays center stage in several studies on language and syntactic
processing and it is often associated with the activity in the left pSTG (Caramazza & Zurif, 1976;
Friederici et al., 2005; Pallier et al., 2011; Snijders et al., 2008; Tyler et al., 2008; Zaccarella &
Friederici, 2015; Zaccarella et al., 2015). Our results seem to indicate that this area might work in
concert with the left ATP in building sentence-level representations that follow the structure de-
scribed by word-word dependency relations. This is potentially compatible with the memory,
unification, and control framework (Hagoort, 2013) that predicates a role for the IFG in integrating
words into their sentential and discourse context.

Besides the ROI analyses, the PPI and whole-brain analyses provide a somewhat more complex
picture. Activity in part of the left pars triangularis is explained by the activity of the left STG mod-
ulated by the phrase structure, whereas activity in another portion of the same subregion is linked
to the activity in the pars opercularis modulated by the dependency structure. The whole-brain
analyses instead indicate an involvement of the left pars orbitalis in phrase-structure processing.

In light of these observations, it is possible that different subregions of the left IFG support the
analysis of different syntactic structures, in concert with either the left ATP or the left STG. In
particular, the pars opercularis might work in concert with the left ATP in building sentence-level

Neurobiology of Language

169

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

dependency representations, whereas the pars orbitalis performs operations related to the ones
carried on in the left STG, having to do with hierarchical phrasal representations of the sentence.
The pars triangularis, in different ways linked to the computation of dependency structure in the
ATP and the phrasal analysis in the STG, might—it is very tentative to say—act as a buffer
between these two areas and their syntactic operations.

These explanations are still at the level of speculation, and we defer to further investigations

before drawing any stronger conclusions.

Additional Areas Involved in Dependency Parsing

Whole-brain analyses highlighted an additional set of regions that are more sensitive to depen-
dency structure as compared to phrase-structure measures. In addition to the ROI results, we
observed an involvement of other brain structures: the left AG, the right posterior cingulate
cortex, and the left superior frontal gyrus.

These patterns of activation might indicate that dependency structures correlate with working
memory mechanisms subserving syntactic parsing. The PFC has been mentioned as a central
player in working memory studies, including in the domain of language processing and sentence
comprehension (D’Esposito & Postle, 2007; Nee & D’Esposito, 2016). In addition, Bonhage et al.
(2014) reported the involvement of also the inferior parietal cortex (including the AG) and areas
bordering the cingulate cortex and the precuneus during the encoding in working memory of
short sentence fragments (4 or 6 words).

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

One may argue that the number of DSlrels governing each word in the stimulus is simply
modeling the load on working memory resources required for word-by-word sentence processing.
In other words, given a sentence, the brain has to store in memory each word incrementally until
the recipient of a dependency relation with each word is presented—and eventually integrated. In
this sense, DSlrels only capture the number of words to keep in mind until a suitable dependent or
head is read or heard.

Nonetheless, this interpretation does not explain the whole picture with regards to dependency
structure processing. As pointed out above, there is a significant relation between DSlrels and the
left ATP, an area that is not traditionally considered part of the working memory network.
Therefore, rather than interpreting these results as suggesting that dependency measures simply
capture working memory loads imposed by the number of words to integrate into the parse, it
might be more accurate to claim that, while dependency-related activity in the left ATP indeed
computes sentence-level structural analyses, activity in areas such as the cingulate and the frontal
and inferior parietal cortex might be well explained in terms of processing memory support to the
activity in the anterior temporal and the inferior frontal regions. Further work is required to shed
light on this possibility.

Interaction Between Areas

The PPI analyses were conducted in order to see what type of interaction exists between these
syntax processing areas (left ATP, IFG, and STG) and the rest of the brain.

The fact that activity in the left STG, modulated by PSxps, seems to drive the activation in a
small portion of the left IFG, besides a large network of bilateral central and precentral regions,
might indicate that there is an interaction between phrase-structure and dependency-structure
processing areas. This might be supported also by the observation that IFG activity, modulated
by DSlrels, explains activity in the left posterior perisylvian cortex (i.e., AG), and in a small

Neurobiology of Language

170

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

portion of the right middle STG. Nonetheless these results cannot allow us to strongly claim a
causal interaction between these sets of areas.

Difference Between ROI and Voxel-wise Results

Part of the discrepancies between the ROI analyses, on the one hand, and the voxel-wise anal-
yses, on the other, are likely due to statistical thresholding in the latter, which is, in many ways, a
statistically insensitive method (as compared to ROI analyses). In the ROI analyses, we assess
whether adding a syntactic predictor improves the linear model fit on the average activity in
an anatomically defined brain area. The whole-brain analyses test whether the beta-coefficient
of a regressor fits the activity of a single voxel in a statistically significant manner. Given that this is
done on many voxels at once, there is a substantial risk of false negatives. The PPI analyses
investigate the coefficients of the syntactic regressor convolved with the BOLD activity of a
seed area while controlling for the BOLD activity of the area itself. Both in spirit and implemen-
tation, this is complementary to the activation-based approach taken in the ROI and whole-brain
analyses. Differences are, therefore, to be expected.

Conclusions

In this article we investigated whether different brain regions are sensitive to different kinds of
syntactic operations. In order to do so, we assessed dependency and phrase-structure descrip-
tors of sentences as predictors of brain activity in the left ATP, the left IFG, and the left STG—
areas engaged during language processing.

We found that activity in the left ATP is better explained by dependency measures as com-
pared to phrase-structure measures. These results differ from previous studies adopting phrase
structures as the formalism of choice to characterize natural language syntax (Nelson et al.,
2017). Our results are related to the ones presented by Brennan et al. (2016). They predicted
fMRI data in both the left anterior and posterior portions of the temporal cortex during narrative
listening using syntactic metrics derived from the phrase structure of the stimuli. In a more recent
study, Li and Hale (2019) observed that a measure combining dependency and phrasal informa-
tion significantly explains activity in the right anterior and left posterior temporal cortex.
Nonetheless, two aspects distinguish our study from the ones by Brennan et al. (2016) and Li
and Hale (2019). First, in our study we deliberately decided to keep phrasal and dependency
measures apart under the assumption that their structural differences might explain activity in
different areas composing the language network in the brain. As a matter of fact, our results show
how the left ATP and the left STG are selective for one or the other. Second, our results, obtained
on Dutch instead of English, may be taken to support the cross-linguistic validity of these obser-
vations, keeping in mind, nonetheless, that Dutch and English are closely related.

Our observations regarding the role of the left ATP are also in line with studies such as
Westerlund et al. (2015) that show how this area is sensitive to a wide range of compositional
structures, including verb-argument and preposition-argument pairs. We think that our results
support the idea that ATP works as a hub for sentential-level semantic composition where words
are combined according to the argument structure of the sentences, as captured by its depen-
dency parse.

We also conducted PPI analyses investigating how the interaction between each of our ROIs
and the rest of the brain is modulated by its preferred structural description as from the previous
lme-analysis. We observed that the activity in the left STG, modulated by the number of closed
phrase structures, might drive the activity in the left ATP. These results, while confirming a

Neurobiology of Language

171

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

division of labour between brain regions, seems to point to an ancillary role of the STG and phrase-
structure building subserving the dependency-style analysis that an area such ATP seems to per-
form. Further research is needed in order to investigate this interaction. A crucial point would be to
assess whether the left STG sensitivity to PSG manifests at earlier latencies as compared to the
sensitivity of the ATP to DG.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The work presented here was funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research
(NWO) Gravitation Grant 024.001.006 to the Language in Interaction Consortium.

FUNDING INFORMATION

Antal van den Bosch, Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (http://dx
.doi.org/10.13039/501100003246), Award ID: 024.001.006.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Alessandro Lopopolo: Conceptualization: lead; Data curation: equal; Formal analysis: lead;
Investigation: lead; Methodology: lead; Software: lead; Visualization: lead; Writing – original draft:
lead; Writing – review & editing: lead. Antal van den Bosch: Formal analysis: equal; Funding
acquisition: equal; Supervision: equal; Writing – review & editing: equal. Karl-Magnus Petersson:
Supervision: equal; Writing – review & editing: equal. Roel M. Willems: Conceptualization: equal;
Investigation: supporting; Methodology: supporting; Resources: equal; Supervision: equal; Writing –
review & editing: equal.

REFERENCES

Baron, S. G., & Osherson, D. N. (2011). Evidence for conceptual
combination in the left anterior temporal lobe. NeuroImage,
55, 1847–1852. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2011.01.066, PMID: 21281723

Baron, S. G., Thompson-Schill, S. L., Weber, M., & Osherson, D. (2010).
An early stage of conceptual combination: Superimposition of
constituent concepts in left anterolateral temporal lobe. Cognitive
Neuroscience, 1(1), 44–51. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080
/17588920903548751, PMID: 24168244

Bastiaanse, R., Bouma, G., & Post, W. (2009). Linguistic complexity and
frequency in agrammatic speech production. Brain and Language,
109(1), 18–28. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.12.004,
PMID: 19217151

Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magne-
toencephalography investigation into the comprehension of mini-
mal linguistic phrases. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801–2814.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5003-10.2011, PMID:
21414902, PMCID: PMC6623787

Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Basic linguistic composition
recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus
during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, 23(8),
1859–1873. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/ bhs170,
PMID: 22735156

Bonhage, C. E., Fiebach, C. J., Bahlmann, J., & Mueller, J. L. (2014). Brain
signature of working memory for sentence structure: Enriched encoding
and facilitated maintenance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26,
1654–1671. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00566, PMID:
24405186

Borsley, R. D. (1998). Syntactic theory: A unified approach (2nd ed.).

(Previous ed.: 1991.) E. Arnold.

Brennan, J. R., Stabler, E. P., Van Wagenen, S. E., Luh, W.-M., &
Hale, J. T. (2016). Abstract linguistic structure correlates with
temporal activity during naturalistic comprehension. Brain and
Language, 157–158, 81–94. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.bandl.2016.04.008, PMID: 27208858, PMCID: PMC4893969
Bright, P., Moss, H. E., Stamatakis, E. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2005). The
anatomy of object processing: The role of anteromedial temporal
cortex. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B,
5 8 ( 3 – 4 b) , 3 6 1 – 3 7 7 . D O I : h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0
/02724990544000013, PMID: 16194974

Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. C. J. (2010). Modeling the noun
phrase versus sentence coordination ambiguity in Dutch: Evidence
from surprisal theory. In J. T. Hale (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2010
workshop on cognitive modeling and computational linguistics
(pp. 72–80). Association for Computational Linguistics.

Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic
and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence
from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3(4), 572–582. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(76)90048-1

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Mouton and Co. DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1515/9783112316009

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. The MIT Press.
http://www.amazon.com/Aspects-Theory-Syntax-Noam-Chomsky
/dp/0262530074. DOI: https://doi.org/10.21236/AD0616323

D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience
of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142.

Neurobiology of Language

172

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031,
PMID: 25251486, PMCID: PMC4374359

Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D. J., Redfern, B. B., &
Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in
language comprehension. Cognition, 92(1–2), 145–177. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002, PMID: 15037129
Frank, S. L., Bod, R., & Christiansen, M. H. (2012). How hierarchical
is language use? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences, 279. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1741,
PMID: 22977157, PMCID: PMC3479729

Friederici, A. D., Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I., &
von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). Processing linguistic complexity and
grammaticality in the left frontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12),
1709–1717. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj106, PMID:
16400163

Friederici, A. D., Meyer, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Auditory
language comprehension: An event-related fMRI study on the
processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and
Language, 74, 289–300. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/ brln
.2000.2313, PMID: 10950920

Friston, K. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: A review.
Brain Connectivity, 1, 13–36. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1089/brain
.2011.0008, PMID: 22432952

Friston, K. J., Büchel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., & Dolan, R. J.
(1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neu-
roimaging. NeuroImage, 6, 218–229. DOI: https://doi.org/10
.1006/nimg.1997.0291, PMID: 9344826

Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Poline, J.-B., Grasby, P. J., Williams, S.
C. R., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Turner, R. (1995). Analysis of fMRI
time-series revisited. NeuroImage, 2(1), 45–53. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1006/nimg.1995.1007, PMID: 9343589

Gauthier, I., Anderson, A. W., Tarr, M. J., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J. C.
(1997). Levels of categorization in visual recognition studied using
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Current Biology, 7(9),
645–651. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(06)00291-0
Hagoort, P. (2005). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 416–423. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004, PMID: 16054419

Hagoort, P. (2013). MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond.
Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 416. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389
/fpsyg.2013.00416, PMID: 23874313, PMCID: PMC3709422

Hays, D. G. (1964). Dependency theory: A formalism and some
observations (Memorandum RM-4087-PR). United States Air Force
Project Rand. https://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs
/research_memoranda/2008/ RM4087.pdf. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.2307/411934

Hodges, J. R., Graham, N., & Patterson, K. (1995). Charting the pro-
gression in semantic dementia: Implications for the organisation
of semantic memory. Memory, 3(3–4), 463–495. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1080/09658219508253161, PMID: 8574874

Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992).
Semantic dementia: Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal
lobe atrophy. Brain, 115(6), 1783–1806. DOI: https://doi.org/10
.1093/brain/115.6.1783, PMID: 1486461

Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E. (2006).
Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during audi-
tory sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
18(4), 665–679. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006
.18.4.665, PMID: 16768368, PMCID: PMC1635792

Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E. (2007).
Time course of semantic processes during sentence comprehen-
sion: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 36, 924–932. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.059, PMID: 17500009,
PMCID: PMC1941617

Jung, W.-Y. (1998). Syntaktische Relationen im Rahmen der

Dependenzgrammatik. Buske.

Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX-NL: A new
measure for Dutch word frequency based on film subtitles.
Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 643–650. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.643, PMID: 20805586

Kos, M., van den Brink, D., & Hagoort, P. (2012). Individual variation
in the late positive complex to semantic anomalies. Frontiers in
Psychology, 3, 318. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012
.00318, PMID: 22973249, PMCID: PMC3434872

Kos, M., van den Brink, D., Snijders, T. M., Rijpkema, M., Franke, B.,
Fernandez, G., & Hagoort, P. (2012). CNTNAP2 and language
processing in healthy individuals as measured with ERPs. PLOS
ONE, 7, 1–8. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone
.0046995, PMID: 23115634, PMCID: PMC3480372

Kos, M., Vosse, T., van Den Brink, D., & Hagoort, P. (2010). About
edible restaurants: Conflicts between syntax and semantics as
revealed by ERPs. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 222. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00222, PMID: 21833277, PMCID:
PMC3153827

Kübler, S., McDonald, R., & Nivre, J. (2009). Dependency parsing:
Synthesis lectures on human language technologies. Morgan &
C l a y p o o l P u b l i s h e r s . D O I : h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 2 2 0 0
/S00169ED1V01Y200901HLT002

Li, J., & Hale, J. (2019). Grammatical predictors for fMRI time-
courses. In R. C. Berwick & E. P. Stabler (Eds.), Minimalist parsing
(pp. 159–173). Oxford Scholarship. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093
/oso/9780198795087.003.0007

Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F., & Saffran, E. M. (1983).
Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic
aphasics. Cognition, 13(3), 361–392. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016
/0010-0277(83)90015-X

Lopopolo, A., Frank, S. L., van den Bosch, A., Nijhof, A., & Willems,
R. M. (2018). The narrative brain dataset (NBD), an fMRI dataset
for the study of natural language processing in the brain. In B.
Devereux, E. Shutova, & C.-R. Huang (Eds.), Proceedings of
LREC 2018 Workshop “Linguistic and Neuro-Cognitive
Resources (LiNCR)” (pp. 8–11). European Language Resources
Association.

Lopopolo, A., Frank, S. L., van den Bosch, A., & Willems, R. M.
(2017). Using stochastic language models (SLM) to map lexical,
syntactic, and phonological information processing in the brain.
PLOS ONE, 12(5), e0177794. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371
/journal.pone.0177794, PMID: 28542396, PMCID: PMC5436813
Lopopolo, A., Frank, S. L., van den Bosch, A., & Willems, R. (2019).
Dependency parsing with your eyes: Dependency structure
predicts eye regressions during reading. In Proceedings of the
workshop on cognitive modeling and computational linguistics
(pp. 77–85). Association for Computational Linguistics. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/ W19-2909

Matchin, W., Hammerly, C., & Lau, E. (2017). The role of the IFG
and pSTS in syntactic prediction: Evidence from a parametric
study of hierarchical structure in fMRI. Cortex, 88, 106–123.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.010, PMID:
28088041

Mazoyer, B. M., Tzourio, N., Frak, V., Syrota, A., Murayama, N.,
Levrier, O., Salamon, G., Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., & Mehler, J.
(1993). The cortical representation of speech. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 5(4), 467–479. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn
.1993.5.4.467, PMID: 23964919

Mel’c(cid:1)uk, I. (1988). Dependency syntax: Theory and practice. State

University of New York Press.

Moss, H., Rodd, J., Stamatakis, E., Bright, P., & Tyler, L. K. (2004).
Anteromedial temporal cortex supports fine-grained differentiation

Neurobiology of Language

173

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

among objects. Cerebral Cortex, 15(5), 616–627. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh163, PMID: 15342435

Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Veltman, D. J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak,
R. S., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). A voxel-based morphometry study of
semantic dementia: Relationship between temporal lobe atrophy
and semantic memory. Annals of Neurology, 47(1), 36–45. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1002/1531-8249(200001)47:1<36::AID -ANA8>3.0.CO;2-L

Nee, D. E., & D’Esposito, M. (2016). The hierarchical organization
of the lateral prefrontal cortex. eLife, 5. DOI: https://doi.org/10
.7554/eLife.12112, PMID: 26999822, PMCID: PMC4811776
Nelson, M. J., El Karoui, I., Giber, K., Yang, X., Cohen, L., Koopman,
H., Cash, S. S., Naccache, L., Hale, J. T., Pallier, C., & Dehaene, S.
(2017). Neurophysiological dynamics of phrase-structure building
during sentence processing. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, 114(18). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas
.1701590114, PMID: 28416691, PMCID: PMC5422821

Oostdijk, N. (2000). The spoken Dutch corpus. Overview and first
evaluation. In Proceedings of the Second International Conference
on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’00). European
Language Resources Association (ELRA). Retrieved from http://
www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2000/pdf/110.pdf

O’Reilly, J. X., Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S. M., &
Johansen-Berg, H. (2012). Tools of the trade: Psychophysiological
interactions and functional connectivity. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 7(5), 604–609. DOI: https://doi.org/10
.1093/scan/nss055, PMID: 22569188, PMCID: PMC3375893
Pallier, C., Devauchelle, A.-D., & Dehaene, S. (2011). Cortical repre-
sentation of the constituent structure of sentences. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, 108(6), 2522–2527. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1018711108, PMID: 21224415,
PMCID: PMC3038732

Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know
what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the
human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 976–987. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277, PMID: 18026167

Poser, B., Koopmans, P., Witzel, T., Wald, L., & Barth, M. (2010).
Three dimensional echo-planar imaging at 7 Tesla. NeuroImage,
51(1), 261–266. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2010.01.108, PMID: 20139009, PMCID: PMC2853246

Rambow, O. (2010). The simple truth about dependency and
phrase structure representations: An opinion piece. In Human
language technologies: The 2010 annual conference of the
North American Chapter of the Association for Computational
Linguistics (pp. 337–340). Association for Computational
Linguistics.

Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2008). Selective attention to semantic
and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks
in anterior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(4), 786–796.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn126, PMID: 18669589,
PMCID: PMC2651476

Rogers, T. T., Hocking, J., Noppeney, U., Mechelli, A., Gorno-
Tempini, M. L., Patterson, K., & Price, C. J. (2006). Anterior
temporal cortex and semantic memory: Reconciling findings from
neuropsychology and functional imaging. Cognitive, Affective, &
Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 201–213. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.3758/CABN.6.3.201, PMID: 17243356

Rogers, T. T., Ralph, M. A. L., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., McClelland, J. L.,
Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2004). Structure and deterioration of
semantic memory: A neuropsychological and computational inves-
tigation. Psychological Review, 111(1), 205–235. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.205, PMID: 14756594

Schäfer, R., & Bildhauer, F. (2012). Building large corpora from the
web using a new efficient tool chain. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T.

Declerck, M. U. Dogan, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, & S.
Piperidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the eighth international conference
on language resources and evaluation, LREC 2012 (pp. 486–493).
European Language Resources Association (ELRA). Retrieved from
https://dblp.uni-trier.de/db/conf/lrec/lrec2012.html#SchaferB12
Schneider, G. (1998). A linguistic comparison of constituency, depen-
dency and link grammar. [Master’s thesis] University of Zurich.
Slotnick, S. D., Moo, L. R., Segal, J. B., & Hart, J. J. (2003). Distinct
prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source
memory for visual shapes. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(1), 75–82.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00082-X

Snijders, T. M., Vosse, T., Kempen, G., Van Berkum, J. J., Petersson,
K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). Retrieval and unification of syntactic
structure in sentence comprehension: An fMRI study using word-
category ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1493–1503. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn187, PMID: 19001084

Stolcke, A. (2002). SRILM—An extensible language modeling
toolkit. In Proceedings of the 7th international conference on
spoken language processing (ICSLP 2002) (pp. 901–904).

Stowe, L. A., Broere, C. A. J., Paans, A. M. J., Wijers, A. A., Mulder,
G., Vaalburg, W., & Zwarts, F. (1998). Localizing components of
a complex task: Sentence processing and working memory.
NeuroReport, 9(13), 2995–2999. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097
/00001756-199809140-00014, PMID: 9804304

Tesnière, L. (2015). Elements of structural syntax (T. Osborne & S.
Kahane, Trans). John Benjamins Publishing Company. Retrieved
from https://books.google.nl/books?id=FNjooAEACAAJ. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1075/z.185

Thothathiri, M., Kimberg, D. Y., & Schwartz, M. F. (2012). The neural
basis of reversible sentence comprehension: Evidence from voxel-
based lesion symptom mapping in aphasia. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 24(1), 212–222. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn
_a_00118, PMID: 21861679, PMCID: PMC3389786

Tyler, L. K., Randall, B., & Stamatakis, E. A. (2008). Cortical differ-
entiation for nouns and verbs depends on grammatical markers.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(8), 1381–1389. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20095, PMID: 18303983

Tyler, L. K., Stamatakis, E. A., Bright, P., Acres, K., Abdallah, S.,
Rodd, J. M., & Moss, H. E. (2004). Processing objects at different
levels of specificity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16,
351–362. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904322926692,
PMID: 15072671

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello,
F., & Joliot, M. (2001). Automated anatomical labeling of activa-
tions in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the
MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15, 273–289. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978, PMID: 11771995

van der Beek, L., Bouma, G., Malouf, R., & van Noord, G. (2002).
The Alpino Dependency Treebank. In Computational linguistics
in the Netherlands 2001. Brill. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1163
/9789004334038_003

van Noord, G. J. M. (2006). At last parsing is now operational. In P.
Mertens, C. Fairon, A. Dister, & P. Watrin (Eds.), TALN06. Verbum
Ex Machina. Actes de la 13e conference sur le traitement auto-
matique des langues naturelles (pp. 20–42). Leuven University
Press.

Westerlund, M., Kastner, I., Kaabi, M. A., & Pylkkänen, L. (2015).
The LATL as locus of composition: MEG evidence from English
and Arabic. Brain and Language, 141, 124–134. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.12.003, PMID: 25585277

Westerlund, M., & Pylkkänen, L. (2014). The role of the left anterior
temporal lobe in semantic composition vs. semantic memory.
Neuropsychologia, 57, 59–70. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuropsychologia.2014.03.001, PMID: 24631260

Neurobiology of Language

174

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

/

.

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Distinguishing syntactic operations in the brain

Willems, R. M., Frank, S. L., Nijhof, A. D., Hagoort, P., & Bosch,
A. V. D. (2016). Prediction during natural language comprehen-
sion. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6), 2506–2516. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1093/cercor/bhv075, PMID: 25903464

Wulfeck, B., & Bates, E. (1991). Differential sensitivity to errors of
agreement and word order in Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 3(3), 258–272. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn
.1991.3.3.258, PMID: 23964841

Zaccarella, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2015). Merge in the human brain: A
sub-region based functional investigation in the left pars opercularis.
Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1818. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389
/fpsyg.2015.01818, PMID: 26640453, PMCID: PMC4661288

Zaccarella, E., Meyer, L., Makuuchi, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2015).
Building by syntax: The neural basis of minimal linguistic struc-
tures. Cerebral Cortex, 27(1), 411–421. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1093/cercor/bhv234, PMID: 26464476

l

D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d

f
r
o
m
h

t
t

p

:
/
/

d
i
r
e
c
t
.

m

i
t
.

e
d
u
n
o

/

l
/

l

a
r
t
i
c
e

p
d

f
/

/

/

/

2
1
1
5
2
1
9
0
8
8
7
9
n
o
_
a
_
0
0
0
2
9
p
d

.

/

l

f

b
y
g
u
e
s
t

t

o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3

Neurobiology of Language

175RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image

Download pdf