RESEARCH ARTICLE

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Gender issues in fundamental physics:
A bibliometric analysis

Alessandro Strumia

Physicist, Fauglia (Italien)

Keine offenen Zugänge

Tagebuch

Schlüsselwörter: sociology of physics, gender gap in STEM, faculty hiring, bibliometrics, gender differences,
sex differences

Zitat: Strumia, A. (2021). Gender
issues in fundamental physics: A
bibliometric analysis. Quantitative
Science Studies, 2(1), 225–253. https://
doi.org/10.1162/qss_a_00114

DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/qss_a_00114

zusätzliche Informationen:
https://doi.org/10.1162/qss_a_00114

Erhalten: 22 Januar 2019
Akzeptiert: 5 Oktober 2019

Korrespondierender Autor:
Alessandro Strumia
astrumia@icloud.com

Handling-Editor:
Ludo Waltman

Urheberrechte ©: © 2021 Alessandro Strumia.
Veröffentlicht unter Creative Commons
Namensnennung 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0)
Lizenz.

Die MIT-Presse

ABSTRAKT

I analyze bibliometric data about fundamental physics worldwide from 1970 to now, extracting
quantitative data about gender issues. I do not find significant gender differences in hiring rates,
hiring timing, career gaps and slowdowns, abandonment rates, Zitat, and self-citation
patterns. Außerdem, various bibliometric indicators (number of fractionally counted papers,
citations, usw.) exhibit a productivity gap at hiring moments, at career level, and without
integrating over careers. The gap persists after accounting for confounding factors and manifests
as an increasing fraction of male authors going from average to top authors in terms of
bibliometric indices, with a quantitative shape that can be fitted by higher male variability.

1.

EINFÜHRUNG

This paper originates from an observational opportunity: For the first time sociological issues in
fundamental physics can be studied using the public InSpire database, which has accumulated
bibliometric data about fundamental physics worldwide from around 1970 to now (InSpire,
2010). Fundamental physics is a subdiscipline of physics that deals with the fundamental aspects
of the field and that presently focuses mostly on particle physics, cosmology, and astrophysics,
from an experimental and theoretical point of view.

Such bibliometric data are being used to study various aspects of the field. Like other science,
Technologie, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields, physics exhibits persistent gender
differences, which I try to characterize and understand in the present paper. The bibliometric
approach relies on large amounts of objective quantitative data about papers, authors, citations,
and hires. Having a large amount of new data I will follow a data-driven approach. Enough
statistics is sometimes needed to reveal effects, and to go beyond simple counting by devising
dedicated analyses that target specific questions. I can do this, as I have the full database, not just
access to some predefined metrics.

While a vast literature has studied gender differences in STEM, no previous studies have spe-
cifically focused on fundamental physics: The present study will fill this gap.1 A main theme is

1 Various studies focused on discrimination as a possible source of gender differences. Small samples of female
physics students were interviewed by Barthelemy, McCormick, and Henderson (2016) and Aycock, Hazari
et al. (2019). The NASEM Report (2018) focused on the University of Texas System, finding that 17% (13%) von
weiblich (männlich) students in science reported “sexist hostility.” However, the NASEM Report (2018) also found
that a higher 45% rate of “sexist hostility” is reported by female students in medicine, a field with a negligible
gender gap in participation. Only a few percent of male and female STEM students reported more serious
problems (NASEM Report 2018, Figur 3.3). As “evidence of direct discrimination is limited,” alternative in-
terpretations of the gender representation difference in STEM have been considered and “many scholars now
emphasize the role of gender differences in preferences, self-concept and attitudes” (Breda & Napp, 2019).

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

understanding why women remain underrepresented in STEM fields, a worldwide phenomenon
that has persisted for decades, despite interventions on its alleged social causes (Stoet & Geary,
2018). A limitation of bibliometric analyses is that authors start being scientifically active roughly
at PhD level: In physics (as in other STEM fields) low female representation is already present
at this entry level of bibliometric data. Earlier phases need to be explored with other tools.

An important clue is that a similar gender difference already appears in surveys of occupational
plans and first choices of high-school students (Ceci, Ginther et al., 2014; Xie & Shauman, 2003).
This is possibly mainly due to gender differences in interests (Ceci et al., 2014; Hyde, 2014; Lippa,
2010; Stoet & Geary, 2018; Su & Rounds, 2015; Su, Rounds, & Armstrong, 2009; Thelwall, Bailey
et al., 2019). Gender differences in relative attitudes (girls with high mathematical ability tend to
also have high verbal ability) also contribute to student choices (Ceci et al., 2014; Stoet & Geary,
2018; Wang, Eccles, & Kenney, 2013): Most of the gender gap in student intentions to study math
disappears after taking into account their mathematics versus reading difference in PISA scores,
while absolute results (boys outperform girls in mathematics, girls outperform boys in reading) Sind
much less able to explain the gender gap (Breda & Napp, 2019).

Coming to the later phase that can be studied by experiments and by bibliometrics, initial
small-scale experiments and anecdotal reports suggested biases against hypothetical female ap-
plicants (sehen, Zum Beispiel, Moss-Racusin, Dovidio et al. [2012] and Wennerås and Wold
[1997]); see also Eaton, Saunders et al. [2019]. These findings have not been supported by more
recent larger-scale experiments (see the review in Ceci and Williams [2011] and also Ceci and
Williams [2015] and Williams and Ceci [2017]). Milkman, Akinola, and Chugh (2015) sent let-
ters by fictional students seeking research opportunities to professors and measured their re-
Rücklaufquote. The result of this social experiment performed in the United States was that,
looking at gender in isolation (rather than at “women and minorities”), female students received
slightly more responses from public schools (the majority of the sample) with respect to men in
the same racial group.

Experiments about hypothetical applicants often miss key elements of real applications,
mostly based on scientific results reported in publications and evaluated by scientists who work
in the same subfield. No significant biases have been found in examined real grant evaluations
(Ceci et al., 2014; Ley & Hamilton, 2008; Marsh, Jayasinghe, & Bond, 2011; Mutz, Bornmann, &
Daniel, 2012) and referee reports of journals (Borsuk, Aarssen et al., 2009; Ceci et al., 2014;
Edwards, Schroeder, & Dugdale, 2018); the gender composition of applicants (Way,
Larremore, & Clauset, 2016) and panels (Abramo, D’Angelo, & Rosati, 2015) has little effect.
Real hires show a higher success rate among women (Ceci et al., 2014; Glass & Minnotte,
2010; National Research Council, 2009; Wolfinger, Mason, & Goulden, 2008), especially in
those STEM fields where women are less represented (Ceci et al., 2014). Bibliometric attempts
to recognize higher merit (Ceci et al., 2014) found that male faculty members write more papers
(Abramo, D’Angelo, & Caprasecca, 2009A; Fuchs, 2005; Larivière, Ni et al., 2013; Levin &
Stephan, 1998; Way, Larremore, & Clauset, 2016; Xie & Shauman, 1998) (see also Holman,
Stuart-Fox et al. [2018] and Thelwall [2018]), predominate among first and last authors (presti-
gious in some fields) and in single-authored papers (Jagsi, Guancial et al., 2006; Westen, Jacquet
et al., 2013). Such gender productivity gap persists after accounting for confounding factors such
as seniority (Caplar, Tacchella, & Birrer, 2017; Ceci et al., 2014; Moldwin & Liemohn, 2018).
Consistent with these results, Witteman, Hendricks et al. (2019) found that female grant appli-
cations in Canada are less successful when evaluations involve career-level elements. Some
studies observed a small group of extremely productive, mostly male, “star authors” (Abramo,
D’Angelo, & Caprasecca, 2009B; Abramo, Cicero, & D’Angelo, 2015; Bordons, Morillo et al.,
2003). A smaller “leaky pipeline” rate of female authors is observed in STEM fields than in other

Quantitative Science Studies

226

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

fields with higher female representation (Ceci et al., 2014). Looking at PLOS medical journals,
where each author declares his or her role (Analyse, Design, Material, perform, schreiben), Macaluso,
Larivière et al. (2016) found that women were more involved in performing experiments, Und
men more involved in the other roles.

This paper is structured as follows.

In Section 2 I describe how I identify the gender of authors; how I obtain lists of hires; how I
combine citations to define bibliometric indicators that can be used as reliable proxies for sci-
entific merit, being significantly correlated to human evaluations such as scientific prizes; Und
how I deal with the confounding factor due to the gender evolution of the field.

In Section 3 I present findings that exhibit interesting gender differences. New authors today
appear with roughly 4:1 männlich:female proportion, with order one variations in different countries.
I find that this entry difference in representation is negligibly affected by hiring, consistent with
Ceci et al. (2014). As I use citations, in Section 3.1 I first verify that male (M ) and female (F )
authors cite in the same way. I achieve this by defining a gender asymmetry in citations sensitive
only to a differential gender bias, not to gender differences in number or productivity of authors.
In Section 3.2 I then compare reliable bibliometric indices based on citations finding that F
authors are hired with indices that are, on average, not higher than those of M authors.
Abschnitt 3.3 finds a productivity gap consistent with previous studies. This new difference is quan-
titatively studied in Section 3.4 finding that the M fraction progressively grows going from aver-
age to top authors, consistent with Abramo et al. (2009B, 2015), and Bordons et al. (2003). In
Abschnitt 3.5 I consider self-references, finding no new gender differences. Data are made avail-
able as discussed in the “Data availability” section. As bibliometric data are influenced by a com-
plicated background of social and historical accidents in the supplementary Section S1 I show
that the results above persist after taking confounding variables into account. Statistical details
are presented in the supplementary Section S2. Interpretations of the data are discussed in
Abschnitt 4.

2. METHODEN

The public InSpire database (InSpire, 2010) maintained by CERN and other institutions offers a
picture of fundamental physics worldwide from around 1970 to now. InSpire gives data on about
1.3 million scientific papers, 30 million references, 71,104 identified authors in over 7,000 In-
stitutes and over 6,000 collaborations. InSpire individually identified all authors (except occa-
sional authors), solving the problem of name disambiguation. I expect that the database is
negligibly affected by direct or indirect gender bias. In der Tat, the database provides essentially
full coverage of the scientific literature within “fundamental physics.” For decades this has been
a self-contained, highly specialized subject (Sinatra, Deville et al., 2005), so that the “bound-
aries” of the database play a minor role. Papers in InSpire include all those published in some
categories of the preprint bulletin arXiv and in some journals with topics considered relevant for
fundamental physics. Human intervention is minor, such as adding extra papers considered rel-
evant. Only a minor fraction of authors occasionally work in other fields or arrive from other
fields. A possible gender difference in multidisciplinary attitudes is thereby expected to have
negligible impact on the subsequent discussion. Andererseits, various authors work on
multiple topics within the field, which cannot be sharply subdivided.

InSpire does not provide gender information: In Section 2.1 I describe the procedure to infer
gender from full names and nationality of the authors. In Section 2.2 I describe how I obtain lists
of hires in fundamental physics worldwide. Abschnitt 2.3 motivates the bibliometric index that I
will use to indicate scientific merit.

Quantitative Science Studies

227

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

2.1. Name–Gender Association

I need to infer gender from names in an accurate and complete way.2 Three main problems are
encountered. Erste, the InSpire database provides only name initials for about 13% of the authors.
These are mostly authors with little impact, as defined by any index. Zweite, some names such
as Nicola are “ambiguous”: They correspond to different genders in different countries. Dritte,
some authors have unusual names. The Mathematica machine learning function Classify
(Wolfram Mathematica, n.d.) uses information about the first name only and leaves about 40%
of authors with unclassified gender.

I tested two approaches to determine their strengths and to choose the best combination:

1. Erste, I run the online Ethnea (Torvik & Agarwal, 2016) tool, which uses the full name (Erste
and family name) to infer both gender and ethnicity. Ethnea leaves 26% of the authors
with unclassified gender.

2. Zweite, for each author I extract a “guessed” nationality from the earlier affiliations in his or
her papers and use it to disambiguate “ambiguous” names. The obtained list of first names
and nationalities is matched to a database of names and countries from the Worldwide
Gender-Name Dictionary (WGND) (Raffo, 2016). This database contains 175,917 Namen
with their associated countries. About 70% of authors have “unambiguous” names that are
present in the WGND. Authors with “ambiguous” names present in the WGND are
matched using the nationality inferred from their earliest affiliations. The size of this subset
of authors is approximately 3% of the total, and the uncertainty induced by this procedure is
below the per cent level. About 0.1% of the authors have “ambiguous” names and no
nationality information: I match them to the most common gender corresponding to their
name, defined as the one used in the largest number of countries. Twenty-three percent
of the authors remain unclassified.

The results discussed in the following are affected in a minor way using the Ethnea or
WGND classifications. By comparing them it can be seen that the Ethnea classification is less
complete, leaving unclassified more authors with unusual names. Andererseits, Die
WGND classification leads to some authors with misidentified gender, typically arising due
to a misidentification of their nationality. Different genders are found for 1.8% of all identified
authors; the percentage rises to 5% among Chinese,3 Indian and Korean authors, and falls to
1% among European authors.

As a best choice, I adopt the Ethnea classification whenever available, and the WGND clas-
sification otherwise. Außerdem, I selected 1,000 of the top-cited authors in different time pe-
riods and systematically verified and correctly assigned their gender with no errors, verwenden
information available on the internet.

2.2. Hiring

InSpire is integrated with HepNames, a database with biographical information about the var-
ious authors, including papers, affiliation history, experiments they participated in, PhD advisor,
and graduate students. Als Beispiel, the internet page http://inspirehep.net/author/profile/A

2 Some U.S. astronomers “discourage” adopting this “quantitative methodology,” seen as “epistemically vio-

lent” and “discriminatory” (Rasmussen, Maier et al., 2019).

3 Gender can be reliably extracted from Chinese names only when they are written in Chinese characters: Das

information is not always provided by InSpire.

Quantitative Science Studies

228

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

.Strumia.1 shows the profile of the present author. A user interface allows researchers to create
and update HepNames records for themselves and for other authors, providing precise career
information on a voluntary basis (to be validated by the InSpire team). Außerdem, groß
collaborations systematically provide complete author information upon submission of
documents through a dedicated format.

From HepNames I obtained a database of about 10,000 first hires in fundamental physics
worldwide, including dates and disambiguated institutions. Such InSpire hires might be biased
if those F and M authors who need to self-report their data tend to do this differently. Während
InSpire is a widely used tool in the community, integrated with a job announcement system,
funded by multiple official institutions and endorsed in Reviews of Particle Physics, occasional
authors might not use it.

I therefore complement InSpire hires by computing unbiased “pseudohires,” defined as
follows. For each paper, there is a list of disambiguated affiliations of each author. I consider
an author as pyr-hired when he or she starts writing papers with the same affiliation for at least
p years. Using this definition I obtain a database of about 40,000/19,000 5/10yr-first hires from
1960 Zu 2013/2008 (64,000/23,000, including multiple hires for the same author). Jedoch, In
this way it is not possible to obtain a precise hiring date for the subset of authors hired by the
same institution to which they were previously affiliated.

daher, InSpire hires will be used when a precise hiring date is more important than
increased statistics, and pseudohires in the opposite situation, when full coverage is more
important than precise timing. In each case, the other sample will be used as a control sample.

2.3. Bibliometrics

I here motivate the use of appropriate bibliometric indicators as a proxy for what is commonly
considered as scientific merit.

Various authors have studied what citation counts do measure.

At theoretical level, two main models have been proposed. According to the normative inter-
pretation, scientists primarily cite to give credit. A bibliometric index then provides a valid proxy of
scientific merit, especially when highly correlated with scientific prizes or other human evalua-
tions of scientific merit. According to the social-constructivist interpretation, citations are instead
primarily a social persuasion tool; the concept of scientific merit itself is questioned: as reviewed in
Bornmann and Daniel (2008) “scientific knowledge is socially constructed through the manipu-
lation of political and financial resources and the use of rhetorical devices.” According to this point
of view, citation counts could be correlated to prizes simply because both reflect social status.
Some prizes in physics require established scientific results; others are awarded following rules
that leave more space for sociological distortions.

Observational works supported the normative interpretation at high aggregation level
(Bornmann & Daniel, 2008; Tahamtan & Bornmann, 2019); personal factors important at indi-
vidual level average out when considering many authors. My data provides extra evidence in
this direction: Zum Beispiel, authors of top-cited papers tend to be younger, rather than powerful
senior scientists.

Citation counts are surely influenced by some confounding factors (Bornmann & Daniel,
2008): the citation intensity depends on time and field (my indicator will compensate for this),
on language (essentially all physics literature is in English), on accessibility (physics literature has
been freely available on the preprint bulletin arXiv since 1995), and on collaboration size.

Quantitative Science Studies

229

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

Collaboration size is a big issue in fundamental physics, due to the presence of very large (hoch
Zu 3,000 authors) and very productive (up to 6,000 Papiere) collaborations, mostly in high-energy
experimental physics. Because of this main reason, traditional metrics (such as citation counts,
h-index, paper counts) now fail to provide reasonable proxies for scientific merit in fundamental
Physik (Strumia & Torre, 2019). Signing more papers that one can read stretches the concept of
authorship (Birnholtz, 2006). At a quantitative level, the problem is that the contribution of one
big collaboration overwhelms the database (1.3 million papers) Wenn 6,000 papers are counted as
3,000 × 6,000 = 1.8 Million.

This situation can be corrected by “fractional counting” (Hooydonk, 1997; Leydesdorff & Park,
2016; Perianes-Rodriguez, Waltman, & van Eck, 2016): A fraction 1/Naut of each paper (eher
than the full paper) is equally attributed to its Naut authors, as appropriate for an intensive quantity.
All authors, including first and last authors, are treated on equal footing because authors are
usually sorted alphabetically in fundamental physics, unlike in other fields.4 Thereby there is no
way of telling who contributed what to multiauthored papers. When huge collaborations are
involved there is no warranty that each author contributed to each paper. Despite this, the data
show that the total fractionally counted bibliometric output of collaborations scales, on average, als
their number of authors (Rossi, Strumia, & Torre, 2019), suggesting that large collaborations form
when scientifically needed and that gift authorship does not play a large role.

Fractional counting of citations already provides one simple acceptable indicator. I improve

on it by using the closely related number of “individual citations”

Nicit ¼ Ncit=NautNref

(1)

(summed over all citing papers, as precisely defined in Strumia and Torre (2019), which gives
reduced weight to citations coming from papers with a larger number Nref of references. Das
refinement addresses the issue of normalization between different fields and times (Zitt and
Small (2008); see Waltman (2016) for a review and extra references): Papers in sectors with a
higher rate of publication (such as phenomenology in fundamental physics) tend to receive more
citations; for the same reason these papers also tend to have more references. Thereby, dividing
by the number of references tends to give a common normalization to different fields, without
needing a field classification system. In der Tat, the average number of individual citations re-
ceived by papers in any field disconnected from other fields is 1. As a test that this concept works
in practice in the InSpire database, I computed the average number of citations, of references,
and of individual citations of papers within the main theoretical fields defined by arXiv (hep-th,
hep-ph, gr-qc, nucl-th, astro-ph, hep-lat5), finding that the dispersion in Nicit among different
fields is reduced to 8%, more than twice as small as the dispersion in Ncit or Nref. Similar results
are found considering different times: The field grows with time, such that newer papers receive
more citations and have more references, in roughly proportional amounts.

Individual citations have the following meaning: An author who wrote Nicit fractionally
counted papers of average impact in his or her field received Nicit individual citations. Tisch 7
of Strumia and Torre (2019) lists the 50 physicists who received the most individual citations,
together with their scientific prizes. Physicists can read their names and consider whether Nicit is
dominantly influenced by scientific achievements or by social constructivism. For practical

4 The first author is not alphabetically sorted in 6% of the multiauthored papers in the hep-th arXiv bulletin,
13% in hep-ph and hep-ex, 18% in hep-lat, 25% in gr-qc, Und 44% in astro-ph. In more papers the author
highlighted as first might accidentally be also alphabetically first.

5 Experimental papers form a separate category, as they tend to have many coauthors and to receive more

citations.

Quantitative Science Studies

230

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

Zwecke, an indicator provides an acceptable proxy of scientific merit if scientific merit posi-
tively affects the index more than confounding variables. A full or large correlation with scien-
tific merit improves the sensitivity of the analysis, but some effects can be large enough that fine
sensitivity is not needed to reveal them.

The use of bibliometric indices based on citation counts as a proxy for scientific merit comes
with limitations and dangers. Some citations are given for negative reasons. On short timescales
citations are more influenced by visibility, and some authors engage in boosting their citation
counts in various ways: large collaborations, many references, self-references, citation net-
funktioniert, salami slicing into minimum publishable units, usw. Individual citations are not boosted
by the first two strategies. As this paper is concerned with gender differences, it is reassuring that
Abschnitt 3.5 will find no extra significant gender differences in self-referencing.

As “when a measure becomes a target, it ceases to be a good measure,” in the supplementary
Section S1 I consider a metric based on citations more different from common targets, welches ist
not enhanced by the latter three strategies. The CitationCoin C== is defined as the difference
between the number of received and given individual citations (up to a correction factor that
prevents systematically negative contributions from recent papers), such that it is not affected
by self-citations or by networks of circular citations (Strumia & Torre, 2019). Authors who write
too many poorly cited papers can even have a negative C== score.

Bibliometric indicators measure the average opinion of the community: While all opinions
can be wrong, a better possibility could be to rely on the opinion of top authors. This is done by
metrics based on the PageRank algorithm (such as those discussed in Chen, Xie et al. (2007), Ma,
Guan, and Zhao (2008), Pinski and Narin (1976), Radicchi, Fortunato et al. (2009), Strumia and
Torre (2019), and West, Jensen et al. (2014)). This is studied in the supplementary Section S1,
where for completeness I also consider the widely used but naive bibliometric indicators based
on paper counting and on the average number of citations per paper.

In der Praxis, the differences in bibliometric indices among authors are so large that log-scale
plots will be appropriate and refined metrics only make minor differences. Individual citations
Nicit are used because this metric is simpler and closer to the commonly used number of citations
Ncit, while allowing us to meaningfully deal with experimentalists, theorists, and astrophysicists
by compensating for the vastly different typical number of co-authors Naut of papers produced
by these communities.

2.4. The Age Confounder

The fraction of female authors in fundamental physics has significantly increased with time, Profi-
ducing demographic gender differences (female authors are on average younger than male
authors) that act as a confounding factor to my later analyses. Apparent gender differences
can just be age differences. Career-integrated indices tend to favor senior authors, and indices
based on single papers tend to favor younger authors. As age is a significant confounder, Ich werde
compensate for the different time evolution Nstart
F;M (T) (number of F and M authors that produced
their first paper during year t) by assigning to each author A a weight proportional to

Nstart
F

tAð
2Nstart
G

M tAð
Þ þ Nstart
Þ
tAð

Þ

;

(2)

where tA is the date of his or her first paper and G is his or her gender. This is equivalent to selecting
every year a random subset of new authors (respecting the time evolution of the total number of
authors) such that M and F authors are numerically equal, and averaging over the possible choices.

Quantitative Science Studies

231

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

3. ERGEBNISSE

Among the 71,104 authors in fundamental physics listed in the InSpire database, 49,860 männlich
Und 9,205 female authors were identified. 16% of authors with identified gender are classified as
weiblich, and wrote 10% of the fractionally counted papers receiving 7% of the individual cita-
tionen. These raw numbers, meant only to give a first rough idea of the field, are affected by a
variety of historical accidents.

As documented in Strumia and Torre (2019), the field has expanded significantly: Due to in-
creased publication intensity, about half of citations have been given after 2000, so that metrics
based on citations favor recent authors (the metric Nicit automatically compensates for publica-
tion intensity). Außerdem, the F percentage grew with time, as shown by the raw data in the left
Tafel mit Figuren 1.

The right panel shows that, within the countries that most contributed to fundamental physics,
the female fractions range between 7% Und 23%. It is interesting to explore if the female fraction
is correlated with the Global Gender Gap Index (GGGI) of the countries (World Economic
Forum, 2016), which measures the gap between women and men in education, Politik, Gesundheit,
and economy, as this is a possible cause of the low female representation. The GGGI ranges
zwischen 0 Und 1, mit 1 indicating parity or a gap in favor of women (as the GGGI ignores
imbalances to the advantage of women). The right panel of Figure 1 shows that the female frac-
tion is not positively correlated with the GGGI, as similarly observed among students in STEM
(Stoet & Geary, 2018).

Figur 2 shows that the female percentage is a factor of 2 higher in subfields dominated by

large experimental collaborations than in theoretical fields.

Clearly, the field and its gender composition have evolved in the past 50 Jahre. While describ-
ing such changes from a bibliometric point of view is an interesting subject, I try to focus on the
general features that emerge from the complicated background of social factors. This will need to
take into account possible confounding variables, by studying subperiods and subtopics or by
trying to compensate for the above variations.

Figur 1.
Left panel: Percentage female contribution to the number of new authors, of authors that wrote at least a paper during the year, Zu
the number of fractionally counted papers, and to the number of received individual citations, considering the papers written each year.
Citations are counted based on the year of the cited publication. Right panel: The percentage of F authors in fundamental theory is not
positively correlated with the Global Gender Gap Index of the country (World Economic Forum, 2016).

Quantitative Science Studies

232

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

Figur 2. As in Figure 1, showing after 1995 the result within the main arXiv categories, plotted as colored curves: Experimental categories
include hep-ex (high-energy experiments) and nucl-ex (nuclear experiments). Theoretical categories include hep-ph (high-energy phenome-
nology), hep-th (high-energy theory), hep-lat (lattice), and nucl-th (nuclear theory); gr-qc (general relativity and quantum cosmology) is mostly
theoretical, although it includes some experiments. Endlich, astro-ph contains astrophysics and cosmology.

3.1. Citations

I want to investigate if citations are influenced by the gender of the cited authors, searching
for a possible different tendency of the two genders to cite a given gender more often.

In principle, complete information could be extracted by comparing “how citations are” with
“how citations would be” in the absence of gender discrimination. In der Praxis, this strategy
needs a theoretical model of citations, but such models are affected by questionable systematic
issues. One can try controlling for the main factors (such as different numbers of M and F authors,
different average seniorities, regional differences), but reality can contain more complicated
Effekte, such as different scientific qualities. Zum Beispiel, Caplar, Tacchella, and Birrer (2017)
claim (consistent with my later findings) that papers in astronomy written by F authors are less
cited than papers written by M authors, even after trying to correct for some social factors. Nach
considering attributing the remaining difference to gender bias, Caplar et al. (2017) conclude
“of course we cannot claim that we have actually measured gender bias.”

I will follow a different strategy, which is often more useful in the presence of backgrounds
that cannot be reliably modeled: I construct an asymmetry such that it is not affected by the
backgrounds. The extracted information encoded in the asymmetry is reliable but partial, as I
give up on the attempt to model the full citation process.

To start, I restrict my inquiry to the subsample of single-author papers with identified gender
G!G0, Die
. ICH

G, as these would likely be more strongly affected by a possible gender bias. I count Ncit
number of single-author papers with gender G citing single-author papers with gender G
G! = (cid:1)
compute the proportions fG!G0 = Ncit
Ncit

G!G0 , Also 0 ≤ fG!G0 ≤ 1. From this I define the gender asymmetry as

G! dividing by the total numbers Ncit

G!G0 /Ncit

G0

0

A ¼ fM!M − fF!M ¼ fF!F − fM!F ¼

(cid:1)

1
M!Ncit
F!

Ncit

det

M!M Ncit
Ncit
F!M Ncit
Ncit

M!F
F!F

(cid:3)

:

(3)

The first formula means that A is the proportion in which solo men cite solo male research more
than solo women cite solo male research. The second formula means that A is also the proportion in
which solo women cite solo female research more than solo men cite solo female research. So the
gender asymmetry ranges between −1 ≤ A ≤ 1. The final formula shows that A is symmetric under
M $ F permutations, with a property that makes it useful: A vanishes whenever citations are given
without considering gender. A > 0 (A < 0) signals same-gender (opposite-gender) preference, althought more complicated patterns are possible: Only one gender might have a particular pref- erence for citing a given gender, or both might have a preference for opposite genders, or both might Quantitative Science Studies 233 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Figure 3. Time evolution of the gender asymmetry defined in Eq. 3; A > 0 (A < 0) signals same-gender (opposite-gender) preference. Left panel: As a function of the publication year of the cited single-author papers. Right panel: As a function of the publication year of the citing single-author papers. After 1995 I also show the asymmetry in different sectors of fundamental physics, based on their arXiv categories: theory (hep-ph, hep-th, hep-lat, nucl-th, and gr-qc), experiment (hep-ex and nucl-ex), and astrophysics (astra-ph). The bin 2018–20 only uses data available up to mid-2018. have a preference for the same gender, in different amounts. On the other hand, A is insensitive to a difference in the total number and in the average scientific quality of M and F authors (as quantified by the chosen indicator), as well as to a possible collective equal bias of both genders towards one gender, which corresponds to multiplying one column of the matrix above by a fixed constant.6 To better understand what the asymmetry measures, it is useful to compute its predicted value in a toy model of citations where Naut G authors of gender G cite with gender-dependent rates pG!G0 (for simplicity I ignore that some authors are more active than others, so that an effective number would be directly relevant). In this model Ncit G0 pG!G0 and the asymmetry equals G Naut G!G0 / Naut (cid:1) (cid:3) A ’ Naut (cid:4) Naut M M Naut F þ Naut F (cid:5) 2 det pM!M pM!F pF!F pF!M (4) in the limit where all pG!G0 are close to a common value (otherwise a slightly more cumbersome expression applies). I extract A from the data by removing self-citations, which introduce a background of same- gender preference not due to an actual gender preference. This removal is done exactly, as I have a list of all references where all authors are identified with a unique code. The removal of self- citations reduces same-gender citations, introducing a small gender discrimination of order 1/ Naut G in the asymmetry: When considering many authors this bias is negligibly smaller than the .7 Figure 3 shows the time evolution of the statistical uncertainty of A, which scales as 1/ gender asymmetry, found to be compatible with zero at all times.8 Restricting to papers after 2010 gives the results shown in Table 1.9 The uncertainty is shown as one standard deviation after the ± symbol. A hint of an asymmetry, Aother = (4.8 ± 1.2)%, is observed among about 104 ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi Naut G p 6 The subsample of “ambiguous” authors (whose name is associated with different genders in different countries) does not show anomalous features that would support the hypothesis of a collective gender bias. 7 More precisely, the uncertainty on A equals [Ncit gation of statistical fluctuations on each counts, F!M/Ncit3 F!FNcit ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi p Ncit . G!G0 8 An analysis performed along the same lines but replacing genders with countries shows an order one prefer- ence for citing authors of the same country, especially in some countries. This can be a manifestation of the stronger contacts between nearby authors. M!]1/2 using the usual propa- M!M/Ncit3 F! + Ncit M!FNcit 9 Our results have been reproduced by Hossenfelder (2018), who also tried to go beyond the asymmetry by G ). As such models introduce questionable assuming a model similar to my Eq. 4 (but with Naut systematic uncertainties, I restrict my attention to the model-independent gender asymmetry. G replaced by Npap Quantitative Science Studies 234 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Table 1. Gender asymmetry A defined in Eq. 3 computed restricting to single-author papers after 2010, in the arXiv categories defined in the caption of Figure 3. The counts are the number of single-author papers in a given arXiv category cited by any single-author papers, not necessarily in the same category Category Counts A in % hep-ex 2,755 hep-ph 14,627 hep-th 15,370 hep-lat 1,762 nucl-ex 1,673 nucl-th 1,258 gr-qc 6,706 astro-ph 6,733 −1.0 ± 1.7 0.5 ± 0.6 0.0 ± 0.7 −0.3 ± 2.2 6.0 ± 2.4 0.7 ± 2.2 0.5 ± 1.2 0.5 ± 1.1 other papers (mostly unpublished) not included in the eight major arXiv categories relevant for fundamental physics. As a result, combining all single-author papers citing single-author papers gives an asymmetry Apublished = (1.0 ± 0.5)% when restricting to published papers, or Aall = (1.9 ± 0.4)% when including all papers. The definition of the gender asymmetry could be extended to multiauthored papers knowing how a hypothetical gender bias would depend on the relative amount of F and M authors. One simple possibility is just to generalize the definition of Ncit G) is the fraction of authors with gender G in each citing (cited) paper. All self-citations, now defined as whenever the cited and citing paper have at least one author in common, are now dropped. With the new Ncit G!G0 I find the results in Table 2. Uncertainties (not shown) are there about five times smaller than in the single-author sample, if propagation of errors is naively applied to fractional counts. G where fG (f 0 G!G0 into (cid:1) citations fGf 0 Taking into account the definition of the asymmetry A and the relative number of F and M authors in my data, I conclude that A is so close to zero that a nonzero gender asymmetry in citations within its measured range would not significantly distort the bibliometric indices based on citations discussed in the following. 3.2. Hiring The lack of a gender asymmetry in citations means that there is no fracture along gender lines in the community about which research in fundamental physics is more relevant/used/visible. In Section 2.3 it was shown that appropriate bibliometric indices based on citations are useful prox- ies for scientific merit. I here use such indices to search for a possible gender difference in hiring. For each hired or pseudohired author I compute his or her bibliometric indices at the hiring moment, defined as in Section 2.2. From this I extract the mean bibliometric indices of hired F and M authors. The left and right panels of Figure 4 show the mean number of fractionally counted papers and of individual citations Nicit, respectively, of authors at their hiring date as reported by InSpire. For the sake of clarity I use traditional color codes: blue for male and pink for female authors. It can be seen that hired F authors do not have, on average, bibliometric indicators above those of hired M authors. Rather, a tendency in the opposite direction seems present at all times, across the main subfields10 and most countries (statistical uncertainties become significant when restricting to some countries with not enough authors). This result persists after taking into ac- count the possible confounding variables considered in the supplementary Section S1.1. I next provide extra information. 10 Experimentalists who work in large collaborations tend to have similar bibliometric indicators. The average Nicit at hiring can be lower for F authors if they are hired younger than M authors. Quantitative Science Studies 235 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Table 2. As in Table 1, considering all papers after 2010 Category Counts/1,000 A in % hep-ex 115 0.0 hep-ph 421 0.3 hep-th 270 0.5 hep-lat 42 1.0 nucl-ex 22 1.0 nucl-th 44 0.5 gr-qc 90 −0.1 astro-ph 285 0.4 Figure 5 shows the cumulative distribution of hired physicists as a function of their scientific age at hiring. It exhibits no significant gender difference. A gender difference could have been produced in various ways: 1. Some hiring committees might take into account career gaps due to maternity (about which no information is available): This would tend to increase the average scientific age of female hired scientists. 2. A gender discrimination in hiring would tend to reduce the average scientific age at hiring of scientists with the favored gender. 3. A gender difference in abandonment rates would tend to reduce the average scientific age at hiring of scientists with the higher abandonment rate.11 A warning is necessary about the two next plots, which extend the analysis to authors who have not been hired. My analysis is restricted to InSpire authors listed in HepNames (described in Section 2.2), which misses many authors who leave the field after writing a few papers. This generates an extra systematic issue, which presumably tends to be gender neutral, such that gender ratios presumably are more reliable than absolute rates. Indeed, information for M and F authors presumably is similarly incomplete, as InSpire does not collect data about gender, especially of unknown authors. Figure 6 shows the fraction of hired authors among those who started writing papers in given time periods. Significant gender differences are not seen. I used 10-year hiring because coverage is more important here than timing. Therefore the plot stops 10 years ago, and absolute numbers would be different using incomplete InSpire hiring. Furthermore, as warned above, extra un- hired authors not in InSpire would lower the hired fraction. Figure 7 shows the abandonment rate per year as a function of scientific age. I considered departures during 2000–2015, counting as having left those authors who wrote no further papers up to 2018. Older authors started when the M fraction was higher and the abandonment rate was lower (as hinted by Figure 6): This confounder generates an apparently lower abandonment rate among M authors. I thereby compensate for gender history as described in Eq. 2. I find that the abandonment rate is maximal among older authors who retire, minimal among senior authors, 11 The temporal distribution of 245 hires of astronomers in the United States after 2010 was studied in Flaherty (2018), finding that F authors are hired on average 1.1 ± 0.6 years earlier than M authors (considering the time after receiving their PhD; astronomers are hired on average five years later). I find a difference of 0.95 ± 0.5 yr restricting to astro/cosmo authors (considering the time after the first paper; authors are hired on average nine years later). According to Flaherty (2018), the hiring time distribution is better fitted assuming a three to four times higher F abandonment rate, rather than assuming a 10:1 bias in favor of F astronomers. However this claim is only based on a very simplified model of hiring that neglects important effects (some authors are better than others; quotas would not be overfilled, etc). I do not attempt to model hiring, as I do not see how models can be made realistic. Rather, I have extra data about papers and citations that support neither a 10:1 bias (see Figure 4) nor a 4:1 difference in abandonment rates (see Figure 7). Flaherty’s (2018) results have been “firmly ruled out” by Perley (2019), who ruled out gender differences larger than 40% in hiring and abandonment rates. Quantitative Science Studies 236 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Figure 4. The left and right panels show the mean number of fractionally counted papers Nipap and of individual citations Nicit, respectively, of authors in fundamental physics at the moment of their first hiring, as a function of the hiring year. Data are shown separately for male (blue) and female (pink) authors, and compensated for gender history as described in Eq. 2. and intermediate among junior authors (as warned above, the abandonment rate of very young authors who leave the field after writing just a few papers is underestimated). Abandonment rates show no significant gender difference, in agreement with the null result by Perley (2019) and in disagreement with Flaherty (2018) (these authors only considered astrophysics). In conclusion, the gender gap in representation at the entrance level of research is negligibly affected by “leaky pipeline” effects consistent with Ceci et al. (2014) that finds large gender differences at PhD level in STEM, and mild differences in the subsequent progress; see also Allen-Hermanson (2017) and Miller and Wai (2015). l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . 3.3. Productivity In this section I study scientific productivity as quantified through bibliometric indices. Of course, such indices say nothing about other activities of researchers that do not result in pub- lications, such as teaching, mentoring, and outreach. Figures 1, 2, 8, and 11 show a possible gender gap in the fractionally counted number of papers: Male authors write, on average, 10% more papers. The gap is consistent with earlier findings in the literature (see, for example, f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 5. Among all authors first hired after 2000, the cumulative fraction of hired authors as a function of their scientific age is shown, for male (blue) and female (pink) authors in experiment (left), theory (middle), astro/cosmo (right). InSpire hires are used and gender history is compensated for as described in Eq. 2. Quantitative Science Studies 237 Gender issues in fundamental physics Figure 6. warnings. Fraction of authors hired up to now as function of the date of the first paper. Only the statistical uncertainty is shown; see the text for Table 2 of Ceci et al. (2014), Abramo et al. (2009a, 2015). A slightly larger gap is found in the number of received individual citations. Is such a gap due to the different average scientific age of M and F authors? To check this possibility, the left panel of Figure 8 shows the mean number of fractionally counted papers written by M and F authors as a function of their scientific age (time since their earliest paper). The gap persists. The right panel of Figure 8 similarly shows the mean number of received individual cita- tions. In both cases it can be seen that junior M and F authors have similar productivity, and that a gap develops with their scientific age. A higher scientific age means going backwards in time to authors who started earlier, when the field was different and when the F percentage was smaller. The averages in Figure 8 are shown separately for hired and not-hired authors, using 10-yr-hires in order to have more complete coverage. It can be seen that hiring is not the reason for the gap. Furthermore, in Figure 8 only scientifically active authors are considered (those who wrote at least one paper after 2013), such that these results would not be affected by a gap in abandon- ment rates. Figure 8 does not compensate for possible career gaps, as such gaps do not exhibit significant gender differences. This is shown in Figure 9, where for each author I computed the longest time gap between consecutive papers, using arXiv dates to have precise information about publica- tion dates. The distribution of longest gaps among M and F authors shown in Figure 9 does not exhibit significant gender differences. A similar null result is found when restricting to hired au- thors. Stopping writing papers might, however, be the extremum of a tendency towards reduced productivity (possibly due to maternity issues). I therefore searched for consecutive years of reduced publication intensity: Some authors are more regular, other experience periods of relatively lower productivity, but again the distributions show no significant gender differences (see Figure 10). No significant gender differences are found looking at periods of relatively higher productivity. Figure 7. Fraction of active authors that leave research each year, as a function of their scientific age. Departures during 2000–2015 are considered, counting as having left those authors who had written no further papers up to 2018 and gender history is compensated for as described in Eq. 2. See the text for warnings. Quantitative Science Studies 238 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d . / f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Figure 8. Mean number of fractionally counted papers (left) and of individual citations (right) as a function of scientific age (time after the first paper) of scientifically active authors now. A gender difference in abandonment rates or career gaps or periods of lower productivity would reduce the cumulative number of papers and of received citations of authors at career level. It is thereby interesting to test whether a gap persists in noncumulative productivity indices that avoid summing over author careers. The procedure is as follows: For each year the subset of scientifically active authors that produced papers is selected, and Figure 11 shows their average productivity, separately for M and F authors. I find that active F authors produce on average roughly 30% fewer papers than active M authors, and receive roughly half the number of citations.12 Furthermore, Figure 12 (left panel) analyzes the gap at the level of papers, finding a smaller F percentage among authors of top-cited papers, even when restricting to single-author papers (see also Jagsi et al., 2006; West et al., 2013). The right panel of Figure 12 shows that F authors tend to work in larger collaborations. The supplementary Section S1.2 discusses other possible confounding variables, without finding anything that can remove the gender gap in productivity. I now discuss some possible causes of such a gap. In various countries F authors have earlier retirement ages. But gender differences show up before retirement in Figure 8. Furthermore, many physicists tend to remain scientifically active after retirement (although the productivity of most physicists tends to decline before retirement). A possible reason for the gender gap observed in various fields is children and maternity. See Ceci et al. (2014) for a recent summary of the literature, which is not univocal. Some studies find no or small effect (Cole & Zuckerman, 1987; Sax, Hagedorn et al., 2002; Stack, 2004; Xie & Shauman, 2003). Other studies find a negative impact on women (Fox, 1995; Ginther & Kahn, 2009), and on men and women equally (Hargens, McCann, & Reskin, 1978), while some studies found a positive impact on men (Ceci et al., 2014), possibly due to selection effects. 12 Fractional counting is used. Using full counting, hyperauthored publications would lead to a recent boom, roughly equal for M and F authors, due to the appearance of experimental collaborations with thousands of authors. Alternatively, the productivity gap can be seen using full counting and restricting to theorists. Quantitative Science Studies 239 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d . / f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Figure 9. papers. Gender history is compensated for as described in Eq. 2. Fraction of authors active between 2000 and now (divided by their main topic) as a function of the longest time break among their Results vary depending on field (with physical sciences sometimes being an outlier, possibly a fluctuation) and are mostly focused on the situation in the United States and on the number of papers produced or worked hours. Ceci et al. (2014) conclude: “the presence of children cannot explain the overall gender productivity gaps.” While maternity would deserve a dedicated study, the InSpire data do not provide any personal information, so it is only possible to proceed in- directly. As has already been described, timing of publications does not show gender differences in periods of null or reduced productivity. Figure 8 indicates that the productivity gap opens at an age roughly consistent with maternity (but also consistent with the transition to scientific inde- pendence), and that it does not close at older ages. A similar situation is found when analyzing the salaries of physicists in the United States: no gender gap just after graduation; a 10% gap after 10–15 years according to Porter and Ivie (2019), who report large differences in personal life choices, in particular that women are four times more likely to have a career break. As maternity laws are different in different countries, an alternative possible strategy is looking for national differences in the M/F gap, which seems stronger in Germany, the United Kingdom, and Italy; weaker in the United States and France, and null in Japan. However single-country statistics are poor and many other national differences can act as confounding factors. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . 3.4. Distribution of Individual Citations In the previous section a productivity gap was found. I here characterize its statistical properties. Figure 13 shows the distributions in the number of individual citations Nicit received by female and male authors in fundamental physics, considering the whole InSpire database. The bell- shaped distributions spread through a few orders of magnitude in Nicit. The dotted curves show that each bell is well approximated, at least on its upper side, by a log-normal as a function of Nicit (namely, by a Gaussian as function of log Nicit, the variable used on the horizontal axis of f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 10. For each author the minimal number of papers he or she produced in a consecutive three-year period is computed. This number is divided by the author average publication rate, obtaining a number 0 ≤ r ≤ 1 normalized such that r = 1 indicates an author who published in a regular way, while r = 0 indicates an author with a three-year period of null productivity. As a function of r the fraction of authors active between 2000 and now is plotted, divided by their main topic. Gender history is compensated for as described in Eq. 2. Quantitative Science Studies 240 Gender issues in fundamental physics Left panel: Mean number of fractionally counted papers produced each year by M and F authors active that year. Right panel: Figure 11. Mean number of received individual citations divided by mean number of fractionally counted papers. The shading reminds us that citation counts are incomplete for recent papers. The continuous curves show the result compensated for gender history as described in Eq. 2, and the negligibly different dashed curves show raw data. Figure 13; log is the logarithm base 10). A log-normal, already observed in bibliometrics (Thelwall & Wilson, 2014), arises when many positive independent random variables contribute multiplicatively.13 The difference between the F and M distributions in Figure 13 is statistically significant. This is better shown by the black curve in Figure 13, that shows the ratio NF/NM of female versus male authors (left axis) as function of the number of received individual citations (horizontal axis).14 The NF/NM gender ratio is not constant: The M fraction progressively grows when going from average to top authors in terms of individual citations. Again it is necessary to study whether such a difference can be a byproduct of confounding factors that affect my composite sample of data. This issue is discussed in supplementary Section S1.3: I do not find any confounder that washes away the trend. As discussed in Section 2.4 I compensate for one significant confounder: Male authors are presently on average more senior than female authors. This is relevant here because senior authors had more time to receive citations (and because younger authors contribute more to top-cited papers). This confounder does not remove the gender difference in Nicit, given that it is observed within sub- samples of authors with same scientific age (see supplementary Figure S11). Correcting for the age confounder is, however, needed to precisely quantify the difference. I proceed as described in Eq. 2. The left panel of Figure 13 shows age-corrected results, while the right panel shows raw data. Both averages and variances differ in raw distributions. The difference in variances persists among the upper sides of the age-corrected distributions, while no significant gender differences are seen on the lower sides of age-corrected distributions, more affected by social phenomena (lower sides would be mostly removed by restricting to hired authors). Different ages can be accounted for by using a complementary strategy: I consider a new A that approximatively compensates for the scientific age ΔtA = − tA of each author. In order to achieve the compensation I choose p ≈ 1.8, because Nicit bibliometric index Nicit /Δtp tnow 13 The following observations suggest that the dominant random variables are unlikely to be of social type (e.g., the possibility that some authors get more visibility and funds that boost their citation counts (Ruocco, Daraio et al., 2017): I find that the number of citations received by authors in physics tends to grow linearly or qua- dratically rather than exponentially with their scientific age. Furthermore, when looking at single papers, rather than at author careers, a significant excess of young authors (scientific age below approximatively 15 years) is observed among authors of top-cited papers. 14 In precise mathematical notation this is dNF/dNM, and the bells are N−1 G dNG/d log Ncit. Quantitative Science Studies 241 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics Left panel: Fractional contribution of all F authors as function of the number of individual citations received by the paper. The F Figure 12. fraction of each paper is determined assuming that each author contributed equally to collaboration papers and compensating for gender history. The same result is also computed restricting to solo papers and to papers with fewer than 10 authors. Right panel: Fractional contri- bution of all F authors to papers with Naut authors. averaged over authors approximatively scales as Δt1:8 A within all main subfields. The result of such a correction is shown in the left panel of Figure 14: Both M and F distributions become narrower, having removed one source of their variability. I again find a higher male variance among the upper sides. The right panel of Figure 14 shows that applying both age corrections has negligible extra effect. The difference in variances on the upper side is seen in any case. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d / . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 13. The bells show the probability distributions of authors in fundamental physics as a function of their number of individual citations, separately for Male (blue) and Female (pink) authors and plotted with a common normalization (right axis). The numbers in the figure show the logarithmic average and standard deviation; and the dots below the bells in the right panel show individual authors separately for M and F authors; the dotted curves show how well a log-normal approximates the upper side of the bells. The black smoothed curve (left axis) shows the ratio NF/NM between the absolute numbers of female and male authors who received the amount Nicit of individual citations indicated on the horizontal axis. Error bars on data points are one standard deviation statistical uncertainties. Left panel: Data after compensating for the higher average age of male authors, as described in Eq. 2. Right panel: Raw data, biased by the different age distribution. Quantitative Science Studies 242 Gender issues in fundamental physics Figure 14. As in Figure 13, adopting a measure that, on average, does not depend on the scientific age of authors. The M distribution still has a longer upper tail. The M fraction is largest among top authors, as clear from the dots below the bells in the right panel of Figure 13, which show the individual authors. It is useful to focus on the subset of top authors. A physicist might read their names and conclude that no sociological confounder can wash away most of them. It is thereby interesting to show that the gender difference is statistically significant, even when restricting to top authors. This is done by considering the hypothesis of no gender difference: While being sociologically implausible it has precisely computable conse- quences that can be compared to data about top authors. Starting from raw data, the F author with most individual citations is in position F1 = 69. Under the hypothesis of no gender difference one −6, where can mathematically compute that the probability of observing F1 m = 1 − f ≈ 0.83 is the male fraction of the large sample (or m ≈ 0.87 restricting to theorists). Under the same assumption, the kth F author should be on average position hFk i = k/f, with probability distribution f kmFk−k( ) (Knapp, 2010). The observed positions are F2 = 147 (the probability of 1 = mF1−1 ≈ 3 10 ≥ 69 is } −1 Fk k−1 3 ≈ 3 10 ≈ 2 10 −13), etc., roughly fitted by Fk −11), F3 = 191 (} being in this or lower position is } 2 69k. Such low probabilities mean that gender differences are needed to account for data about top authors. Of course, it is already known that the time evolution of f = NF/(NM + NF) is a significant confounder. I again find that such a confounder is not enough to remove the difference, as the female fraction f is larger than 1/69 at all relevant times. More precisely, after performing the age correction as in Eq. 2, the kth top female authors shift to Fk = {36, 82, 114, 126, …} (cid:2) 33k position. Considering the age-corrected metric Nicit /Δt1:8 A the positions are Fk = {20, 57, 109, 180, …} (cid:2) 38k. After correcting for the age confounder, }-values remain small because the female fraction (cid:2)1/33 or (cid:2)1/38 found among top authors remains smaller than the fraction f of female authors in the full sample. An excess of male top authors is found even after correcting for the age confounder.15 ≈ The fact that gender differences are maximal among those top authors who receive up to 3,000 more individual citations than average authors indicates that differences do not predominantly result from sociological factors that could give factors of 2 differences at individual level (such as harder working vs. career gaps, more research vs. more teaching, specialization on physics vs. wider interests, etc). 15 A varying gender fraction that culminates in a small group of extremely productive, mostly male, “star authors” has been observed in Abramo et al. (2009b, 2015) and Bordons et al. (2003) (see also Kwiek, 2016). Ioannidis, Baas et al. (2019) used Scopus data about 6.9 million scientists in all disciplines to compute a “composite” bibliometric indicator, producing a list of top authors. When restricted to fundamental physics, their list (despite minor problematic aspects) is significantly correlated to my list. In their all-fields list the female authors are found in positions Fk = {133, 146, 160, …}. While this naively seems to extend my findings, I cannot correct their list for the age confounder nor for other confounders. Quantitative Science Studies 243 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / e d u q s s / a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 2 1 2 2 5 1 9 0 6 6 0 5 q s s _ a _ 0 0 1 1 4 p d . / f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Gender issues in fundamental physics In the left panel the points with Gaussian 1(cid:2) statistical errors are data about the NF /NM female/male ratio as a function of the Figure 15. number of individual citations received. Points with NM > NF = 0 are shown as down arrows. The blue points are raw data, the red points are
corrected compensating for the different time evolution of the overall number of M and F authors. In the right panel a different bibliometric
index is considered, which approximatively compensates also for the scientific age of each author. Data are not well fitted by a linear function in a
log-log scale (which corresponds to p = 1 in the supplementary Eq. S2) and can be fitted by a quadratic function (which corresponds to p = 2). Das
can be interpreted as different male and female variances, as in Eq. 6, rather than as different averages, as in Eq. 5.

As the variation of NF /NM survives to confounding variables, I try to better understand this

effect by investigating its quantitative shape.

A look at Figures 13 oder 14 suggests that the M bell has a longer tail of top authors (the raw data
also show a difference in averages, mostly due to confounders, that makes the difference in
variances less easily visible). The supplementary Section S2 shows that the difference in upper
variances is statistically significant. I here provide a simpler, more intuitive, argument through
analytical approximations based on the observation that the distributions of individual citations
received by M and F authors separately are approximatively log-normal (Gaussian as a function
of ℓ = ln Nicit) on their upper side. Log-normal distributions with a common standard deviation (cid:2)
and different averages μ

F for M and F authors would produce a NF /NM of the form

≠ μ

M

NF
NM

D

/ e− ‘ − μ
D
e− ‘ − μ

Þ2=2σ2

F

Þ2=2σ2

M

(cid:9)
¼ exp − Rσ
2

(cid:7)

‘ −

M

M

þ μ
F

2

(cid:10)

(cid:8)

;

Rσ ¼ 2

M

M

− μ
F
σ2

:

Different standard deviations (cid:2)

(cid:2)

M

F would produce

NF
NM

D

/ e− ‘ − μ
D
e− ‘ − μ

Þ2=2σ2
F

Þ2=2σ2
M

(cid:9)
¼ exp − Rσ
2

(cid:10)

D

‘ − μ

Þ2

;

Rσ ¼ 1
σ2
F

− 1
σ2
M

:

(5)

(6)

Thereby a dominant difference in averages (standard deviations) would produce a line (a parabola)
when NF/NM is plotted as a function of Nicit on a log-log scale. Such a plot is shown in Figure 15,
again performing the usual corrections for the age confounder. The important point is that, in all
Fälle, NF/NM exhibits a parabolic shape along the upper sides of the bells, where the log-normal
approximation is accurate enough.16 In conclusion, the data exhibit a gender difference in the
upper variances.

16 Adding higher-order terms in the exponent would not change the above conclusion, because fits to the ob-

served distributions find small higher-order terms.

Quantitative Science Studies

244

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

Is this statistically strong preference an artefact of the complexity of the full data sample?
Um zu antworten, the analysis is repeated within the independent subsamples of supplementary
Figure S11: Plotted on a log-log scale they independently tend to show a parabolic (rather than
linear) trend in NF/NM. The dotted curves in Figure S11 show how well each subsample can be
fitted in terms of R(cid:2) 2 and p ≈ 2. Außerdem, the probabilities }
i that test the hypothesis of
no gender difference restricting to top authors are small within the subsamples of Figure S11
(where the top authors are plotted as points), consistent with Abramo et al. (2009B) Und
Bordons et al. (2003).

A similar difference in upper variances is also found when looking at different bibliometric

indicators; see the supplementary Section S1.3.

3.5. Self-References

Gender differences in self-references are an interesting topic on its own, as as well as a possible
confounding factor to previous analyses. I verified that my previous results persist when drop-
ping self-references, and I next justify why there is no need to drop self-references.

Bibliometric studies have found that men cite their own papers more than women: Cameron,
White, and Gray (2016) focused on six ecology journals; Ghiasi, Larivière, and Sugimoto (2016)
on the Web of Science database; King, Bergstrom et al. (2017) on the JSTOR database; Und
Hossenfelder (2018) on single-authored papers in arXiv. In agreement with such studies, restricting
to solo papers, I find a (cid:2)20–30% higher fraction of self-references among the papers written by
M authors (7.3% versus 5.9%).

Jedoch, King et al. (2017) mention the possibility that this gender difference in self-
references is just a reflection of the fact that male authors tend to write more solo papers, Und
thereby have more scientific reasons for self-references. King et al. (2017) could not check if this
is a significant confounding factor, because authors are not disambiguated in their database. Als
authors are disambiguated in the InSpire database, I can perform this check finding that this
confounding factor removes the gender difference in self-references: A similar fraction of
self-references is found when comparing male and female authors who wrote the same num-
ber of papers Npap. In other terms, the fraction of self-references can be a misleading indicator
because the average number of self-references grows with the number of solo papers following
pap with power p > 1. My data suggest p ≈ 1.3.
a scale law Nself
cit

/ Np

As solo papers are a relatively small subsample that might be not representative of the full
database, I extend the analysis to multiauthored papers. Um dies zu tun, it is necessary to dis-
tinguish self-references from self-citations. I count a citation as self-citation whenever the citing
paper has at least one author in common with the cited paper. I count it as a self-reference only
for those authors who wrote both the cited and the citing paper. I clarify this with an example.
One paper by authors A and B cites a paper by authors B and C: B gives a self-reference; both B
and C receive a self-citation (B directly and C indirectly).

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

As usual, fractional counting is used. For each author I compute the total number of received
, of given individual self-

individual citations Nicit, of received individual self-citations Nreceived
references Ngiven
Verweise).

icit

icit

, and of fractionally counted papers Npap (equal to the number of given individual

I consider the mean fraction of given individual self-references Ngiven
/Nicit (left panels of
Figur 16) and the mean fraction of received individual self-citations Nreceived
/Nicit (right panels
of Figure 16). The upper row shows some gender difference: Male authors tend to give

icit

icit

Quantitative Science Studies

245

Gender issues in fundamental physics

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Figur 16.
Left panel: Distribution of authors as a function of their self-reference rate. Right panel: Distribution of authors as a function of their
self-citation rate. Bottom row: Mean fraction of given self-references (links) and of received self-citations (Rechts) among M (Blau) and F (pink)
authors with the number of fractionally counted number of papers indicated on the horizontal axis.

themselves a higher fraction of self-references (13.2% instead than 12.0%); female authors tend
to receive a higher fraction of indirect self-citations (23.5% instead t hen 19.8%). Such differ-
ences again disappear in the lower row of Figure 16, where the self fractions are computed as a
function of the number Npap of fractionally counted papers written by each author. Similarly to
what we found in the solo sample, male authors tend to cite themselves and their collaborators
more just because male authors tend to have more past papers. Similar results are found re-
stricting within the main subfields.

4. CONCLUSIONS

I performed a bibliometric analysis of gender issues in fundamental physics worldwide from
etwa 1970 to now. Bibliometrics gives quantitative data on activities of researchers
that result in publications and tells nothing about other possible activities, such as teaching,
mentoring, and outreach. Trotzdem, research is an interesting area in which individual talent
can be expressed, as confirmed by the large differences between authors found in data.
Concerning gender I find the following results:

1. Erste, the well-known initial gender difference in representation is seen: There are roughly
four males for each female among new authors that appear at PhD-level. The initial
female fraction is not positively correlated with the Global Gender Gap Index of the
Länder,17 and negligibly evolves in the subsequent career stages (Figur 7).

17 An anticorrelation known as “gender equity paradox” (Stoet & Geary, 2018) has been observed among stu-

dents, who are less mobile than researchers.

Quantitative Science Studies

246

Gender issues in fundamental physics

2. When citing works by others, authors exhibit no or small gender difference: Male and
female authors have the same average opinion about which research in fundamental
physics deserves to be cited (Figur 3).18 Außerdem, M and F authors give to their
own papers a similar fraction of self-references (Figur 16), taking into account that M
authors tend to write more papers, especially solo papers (Figur 12).

3. Female authors do not have, at hiring moments, higher average bibliometric indicators
based on individual citations or fractionally counted papers than male authors (Figuren 4
Und 5).

4. Among authors identified by InSpire HepNames profiles (which misses authors who
write very few papers), I do not find a gender difference in hired percentages (Figur 6),
in abandonment rates (Figur 7), in longest breaks between papers (Figur 9), in periods
of reduced activity relatively to their average (Figur 10).

The above results are in line with the literature, as summarized in Ceci et al. (2014): “the
overall picture is one of gender neutrality”; “no evidence of women having harder time getting
tenure”; postbachelor gender differences in attrition rates are significantly smaller in STEM than
in life science, Psychologie, and social science. The literature finds a second gender difference, In
productivity: “women on average publish fewer papers than men”; “there are no sex differences
in citations per article” (Ceci et al., 2014). I find:

5. A productivity gap both in the fractionally counted number of publications and in their
citational impact (Figur 8), which does not appear to be predominantly concentrated in
specific countries, topics, periods, bibliometric indicators, journals (supplementary Figure S1),
usw. The gap is also found without integrating over careers (see Figures 11 Und 12).

6. A gradually increasing male fraction when going from average to top authors in terms of
individual citations (or other indices, such as fractionally counted publications). The quan-
titative shape of this trend appears predominantly due to a higher variance on the upper
side of the M distributions (siehe Abbildung 15 and Eqs. 5 Und 6), rather than due to a difference
in averages.

I verified that my results still hold when ignoring hyperauthored papers (possibly affected by

guest authorship) or restricting to single-authored papers.

While many social phenomena could produce different averages, producing different vari-
ances would need something that specifically disadvantages research by top female authors. Nur
to take one example of a social nature, a gender gap in research productivity could arise if better
female authors receive more honours and leadership positions that drive them away from re-
suchen. Jedoch, data also show an excess of young authors among those who produced
top-cited papers: The excess is observed among both M and F authors. This suggests extending
my considerations from possible sociological issues to possible biological issues.

18 Some authors discuss the possibility that physicists are collectively affected by an unconscious gender bias—a
concept that has received recent attention in the United States following the development of Implicit
Association Tests (IAT) that claimed to reveal such biases. Even if such tests were scientifically valid (sehen, Wie-
immer, Oswald, Mitchell et al. (2013) for a metareview), reading a scientific paper involves different mental
processes than those probed by IAT. Some of my results are based on citations: The same conclusions are
reached when removing from my analysis citations to single-authored research, which would be more affected
by a hypothetical collective gender bias. Außerdem, fields that study bias might have their own biases:
Stewart-Williams, Thomas et al. (2019) and Winegard, Clark et al. (2018) found that scientific results exhibiting
male-favoring differences are perceived as less credible and more offensive. Handley, Brown et al. (2015)
found that men (especially among STEM faculty) evaluate gender bias research less favorably than women.

Quantitative Science Studies

247

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

It is interesting to point out that the gender differences in representation and productivity
observed in bibliometric data can be explained at face value (one does not need to assume that
confounders make things different from what they seem), relying on the combination of two effects
documented in the scientific literature: differences in interests (Diekman, Johnston, & Clark, 2010;
Lippa, 2010; Su, Rounds, & Armstrong, 2009; Su & Rounds, 2015; Thelwall, Bailey et al., 2019)
and in variability (Deary, Irwing et al., 2007; Halpern, Benbow et al., 2007; Hyde, 2014; Stevens &
Haidt, 2017; Wang et al., 2013).

Greater male interest in things and greater female interest in people is observed consistently
across cultures and time and is large (d ≈ 1, d.h., distributions differ by about one standard devi-
ation): Such a difference in interests predominantly accounts for the initial difference in repre-
sentation.19 Difference in variability accounts for the difference in productivity. This is consistent
with O’Dea, Lagisz et al. (2018), which confirms the difference in variabilities looking at grades,
and observes that this difference alone cannot reproduce the representation gap.

The amount of higher male variability suggested by bibliometric data in fundamental physics
is at the 10% Ebene, roughly consistent with independent observations of presumably relevant
traits. While such psychometrics observations predominantly probe the central, most populated,
part of the distributions, I reasonably expect that physicists probe the upper tail20 and that top-
cited physicists reach the far-end upper tail. I estimate to reach about five standard deviations
above the mean, given that this is the maximal deviation statistically expected from a pool of
(cid:2)109 persons in a Gaussian approximation.

When dealing with complex systems, any simple interpretation can easily be incomplete,
including a hypothetical gender discrimination. In any case, it is interesting that data can be
explained without invoking such a hypothesis.

I conclude by addressing ethical and social values, given that a gender difference in variances
is seen by some as offensive, like other ideas originally proposed by Darwin (Hill, 2017) (mod-
estly keeping things in proportion in this comparison). The interpretation in terms of different
variabilities implies that one should keep giving gender-neutral equal opportunities to every-
body by considering each person based on his or her individual qualities, not as member of a
demographic group (Geschlecht, nationality, or whatever). The refusal to consider population level
differences in distributions when trying to understand gaps in representation can lead to discrim-
ination allegedly aimed at establishing equal outcomes (sehen, Zum Beispiel, Strumia (2019) for a
more extensive discussion of such issues).

19 Large gender differences along the people/things dimension are observed in occupational choices and in ac-
ademic fields: Such differences are reproduced within subfields (Thelwall et al., 2019). Insbesondere, weiblich
participation is lower in subfields closer to physics, even within fields with their own cultures, such as “physical
and theoretical chemistry” within chemistry (Thelwall et al., 2019). This suggests that the people/things dimen-
sion plays a more relevant role than the different cultures of different fields.

Außerdem, psychology finds that females value careers with positive societal benefits more than do
Männer: Some authors propose that women tend more to opt out of STEM because “women tend to endorse
communal goals more than men” (Diekman et al., 2010; Evans & Diekman, 2009). Indeed Gibney (2007) finds
that women in UK academia report dedicating 10% less time than men to research and 4% more time to
teaching and outreach, and Guarino and Borden (2017) finds that women in U.S. non-STEM fields do more
academic service than men. Concerning fundamental physics, old discoveries gave huge societal benefits, Aber
no practical applications are resulting from contemporary explorations of very small and very large scales (solch
−25 seconds, or cosmological observations of objects
as production at colliders of particles that decay in 10
billions of light years far away).

20 Physics attracted students with high average grades; sehen, Zum Beispiel, Figur 9 of Ceci et al. (2014) and Figure 1 von
Ginther and Kahn (2015). Looking at career-integrated citations, Physik (and especially fundamental physics)
shows the largest ratio between high (90th) and low (25th) Perzentil (Tables 1 and S3 of Ioannidis et al. (2019)).

Quantitative Science Studies

248

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank the referees for their comments; the InSpire team for clarifications; Guy Madison for
discussions and suggestions; Riccardo Torre for (among many things) having implemented the
WGND name-gender association; Sabine Hossenfelder for having independently replicated the
results in Figure 8b and Section 3.1 using arXiv data (Hossenfelder, 2018); and more colleagues
who prefer not to be mentioned.

COMPETING INTERESTS

The author has no competing interests.

FUNDING INFORMATION

No funding has been used for this research, performed outside working time.

DATA AVAILABILITY

This study is based on bibliometric data extracted from the InSpire database around mid-2018.
While data are already public from the InSpire web site (InSpire, 2010), I selected the key data
and converted them into user-friendly Mathematica format. The code used to extract data is
available at GitHub (https://github.com/RiccardoTorre/InSpireTools) and the resulting data at
Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3482884). The data consist of a table with more than
one million entries, one for each paper. Each entry contains the following information:

1. an integer number identifying the paper;
2. publication date;
3. publication date on arXiv when available;
4. number of authors;
5. number of references;
6. number of citations;
7. PageRank with self-citations;
8. PageRank without self-citations;
9.
10.
11.
12.
13. arXiv main category when available;
14. 1 if published, 0 ansonsten;
15.

list of authors with each author identified by an integer number;
list of references, with each paper identified by the integer in item 1);
list of citations;
title;

list of lists of affiliations of each author, with each institute identified by an integer
number;

16. arXiv subcategory when available;
17. arXiv number when available;
18.
19. collaboration(S), with each collaboration identified by an integer number;
20. number of individual citations.

journal, with each journal identified by an integer number;

The correspondence between integer numbers and papers, authors, institutes, journals, Und
collaborations is available from InSpire (for privacy reasons I avoid making my tables public).

Quantitative Science Studies

249

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

.

/

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

VERWEISE

Abramo, G., Cicero, T., & D’Angelo, C. A. (2015). Should the research
performance of scientists be distinguished by gender? Zeitschrift für
Informetrics, 9, 25–38. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joi.2014
.11.002

Abramo, G., D’Angelo, C. A., & Caprasecca, A. (2009A). Gender
differences in research productivity: A bibliometric analysis of
the Italian academic system. Scientometrics, 79, 517–539. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-007-2046-8

Abramo, G., D’Angelo, C. A., & Caprasecca, A. (2009B). The contri-
bution of star scientists to sex differences in research productivity.
Scientometrics, 81, 137. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192
-008-2131-7

Abramo, G., D’Angelo, C. A., & Rosati, F. (2015). Selection com-
mittees for academic recruitment: Does gender matter? Forschung
Evaluation, 24(4), 392–404. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/reseval
/rvv019

Allen-Hermanson, S. (2017). Leaky pipeline myths: In search of
gender effects on the job market and early career publishing in
philosophy. Grenzen in der Psychologie, 8, 953. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00953, PMID: 28659843, PMCID:
PMC5469890

Aycock, L. M., Hazari, Z., Brewe, E., Clancy, K. B. H., Hodapp, T.,
& Goertzen, R. M. (2019). Sexual harassment reported by under-
graduate female physicists. Physical Review Physics Education
R e s e a r c h , 1 5 , 0 1 0 1 2 1 . D O I : h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 1 0 3
/PhysRevPhysEducRes.15.010121

Barthelemy, R. S., McCormick, M., & Henderson, C. (2016).
Gender discrimination in physics and astronomy: Graduate
student experiences of sexism and gender microaggressions.
Physical Review Physics Education Research, 12, 020119.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.12.020119
Birnholtz, J. P. (2006). What does it mean to be an author? The in-
tersection of credit, contribution, and collaboration in science.
Journal of the American Society for Information Science and
Technologie, 57(13), 1758–1770. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002
/asi.20380

Bordons, M., Morillo, F., Fernandez, M. T., & Gomez, ICH. (2003).
One step further in the production of bibliometric indicators at
the micro level: Differences by gender and professional category
of scientists. Scientometrics, 57(2), 159–173. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1023/A:1024181400646

Bornmann, L., & Daniel, H. D. (2008). What do citation counts
messen? A review of studies on citing behavior. Zeitschrift für
Documentation, 64(1), 45–80. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1108
/00220410810844150

Borsuk, R., Aarssen, L. W., Budden, A. E., Koricheva, J., Leimu, R.,
… Lortie, C. J. (2009). To name or not to name: The effect of
changing author gender on peer review. BioScience, 59(11),
985–989. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/bio.2009.59.11.10

Breda, T., & Napp, C. (2019) Girls’ comparative advantage in read-
ing can largely explain the gender gap in math-related fields.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 116(31), 15435–15440. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1905779116, PMID: 31308226, PMCID:
PMC6681723

Cameron, E. Z., White, A. M., & Gray, M. E. (2016). Solving the
productivity and impact puzzle: Do men outperform women,
or are metrics biased? BioScience, 66(3), 245–252. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biv173

Caplar, N., Tacchella, S., & Birrer, S. (2017). Quantitative evalua-
tion of gender bias in astronomical publications from citation

counts. Nature Astronomy, 1, 0141. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038
/s41550-017-0141

Ceci, S. J., Ginther, D. K., Kahn, S., & Williams, W. M. (2014). Frauen
in academic science: A changing landscape. Psychological Science
in the Public Interest, 15(3), 75–141. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177
/1529100614541236, PMID: 26172066

Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2011) Understanding current causes
of women’s underrepresentation in science. Verfahren der
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
108(8), 3157–3162. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas
.1014871108, PMID: 21300892, PMCID: PMC3044353

Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2015). Women have substantial ad-
vantage in STEM faculty hiring, except when competing against
more-accomplished men. Grenzen in der Psychologie, 6, 1532. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01532, PMID: 26539132,
PMCID: PMC4611984

Chen, P., Xie, H., Maslov, S., & Redner, S. (2007). Finding scientific
gems with Google’s PageRank algorithm. Journal of Informetrics,
1(1), 8-15. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joi.2006.06.001

Cole, J. R., & Zuckerman, H. (1987). Hochzeit, motherhood, Und
research performance in science. Scientific American, 256(2),
119–125. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0287
-119, PMID: 3810117

Deary, ICH. J., Irwing, P., Der, G., & Bates, T. C. (2007). Brother-sister
differences in the g factor in intelligence: Analysis of full, opposite-
sex siblings from the NLSY1979. Intelligence, 35(5), 451–456.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.003

Diekman, A. B., Johnston, A. M., & Clark, E. K. (2010). Seeking
congruity between goals and roles: A new look at why women
opt out of science, Technologie, engineering, and mathematics
careers. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1051–1057. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.1177/0956797610377342, PMID: 20631322

Eaton, A. A., Saunders, J. F., Jacobson, R. K., & Westen, K. (2019).
How gender and race stereotypes impact the advancement of
scholars in STEM: Professors’ biased evaluations of physics and
biology post-doctoral candidates. Sex Roles, 82, 127–141. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01052-w

Edwards, H. A., Schroeder, J., & Dugdale, H. L. (2018). Gender differ-
ences in authorships are not associated with publication bias in an
evolutionary journal. PLOS ONE, 29, e0201725. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201725, PMID: 30157231, PMCID:
PMC6114708

Evans, C. D., & Diekman, A. B. (2009). On motivated role selection:
Gender beliefs, distant goals, and career interest. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 33(2), 235–249. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111
/j.1471-6402.2009.01493.x

Flaherty, K. (2018). The leaky pipeline for postdocs: A study of the
time between receiving a PhD and securing a faculty job for
male and female astronomers. arXiv, arXiv:1810.01511.

Fuchs, M. F. (1995). Women in scientific careers. In S. Jasanoff, G. E.
Markle, J. C. Peterson, & T. Pinch (Hrsg.), Handbook of science
and technology studies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Fuchs, M. F. (2005). Gender, family characteristics, and publication
productivity among scientists. Social Studies of Science, 35(1),
131–150. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0306312705046630
Ghiasi, G., Larivière, V., & Sugimoto, C. R. (2016). Gender differences
in synchronous and diachronous self-citations. 21st International
Conference on Science and Technology Indicators, Valencia.
Gibney, E. (2007). Teaching load could put female scientists at career
disadvantage. Nature Trend Watch. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038
/nature.2017.21839

Quantitative Science Studies

250

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

Ginther, D. K., & Kahn, S. (2009). Does science promote women?
Evidence from Academia 1973–2001. In R. B. Freeman & D. L.
Goroff (Hrsg.), Science and engineering careers in the United States.
National Bureau of Economic Research Conference Report.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Ginther, D. K., & Kahn, S. (2015). Comment on expectations of bril-
liance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines.
Wissenschaft, 349(6246), 391. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science
.aaa9632, PMID: 26206926

Glass, C., & Minnotte, K. (2010). Recruiting and hiring women in
STEM fields. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 3(4), 218–229.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020581

Guarino, C. M., & Borden, V. M. H. (2017). Faculty service loads
and gender: Are women taking care of the academic family?
Research in Higher Education, 58(6), 672–694. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1007/s11162-017-9454-2

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., &
Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science
and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(1),
1–51. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x,
PMID: 25530726, PMCID: PMC4270278

Handley, M., Braun, E. R., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Schmied, J. L.
(2015). Quality of evidence revealing subtle gender biases in sci-
ence is in the eye of the beholder. Verfahren des Nationalen
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(43),
13201–13206. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1510649112,
PMID: 26460001, PMCID: PMC4629390

Hargens, L. L., McCann, J. C., & Reskin, B. F. (1978). Productivity
and reproductivity: Fertility and professional achievement among
research scientists. Social Forces, 57(1), 154–163. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.2307/2577631

Hill, T. P. (2017). An evolutionary theory for the variability hypoth-

esis. ArXiv, arXiv:1703.04184.

Holman, L., Stuart-Fox, D., Hauser, C. E., & Sugimoto, C. (2018).
The gender gap in science: How long until women are equally
represented? PLOS Biology, 16, e2004956. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1371/journal.pbio.2004956, PMID: 29672508, PMCID:
PMC5908072

Hooydonk, G. V. (1997). Fractional counting of multiauthored publi-
Kationen: Consequences for the impact of authors. Journal of the
American Society for Information Science, 48(10), 944–945. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1002/(WISSENSCHAFT)1097-4571(199710)48:10<944:: AID-ASI8>3.0.CO;2-1

Hossenfelder, S. (2018). Do women get fewer citations than men?

Talk at Chapman University, November 11, 2018.

Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review
of Psychology, 65, 373–398. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev
-psych-010213-115057, PMID: 23808917

InSpire. (2010). High-energy physics literature INSPIRE database
(A. Holtkamp, S. Mele, T. Simko, and T. Schmied (INSPIRE
collaboration)).

Ioannidis, J. P. A., Baas, J., Klavans, R., & Boyack, K. W. (2019). A
standardized citation metrics author database annotated for
scientific field. PLOS Biology, 17, e3000384. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000384, PMID: 31404057, PMCID:
PMC6699798

Jagsi, R., Guancial, E. A., Worobey, C. C., Henault, L. E., Chang, Y.,
… Hyek, E. M. (2006). The gender gap in authorship of academic
medical literature—A 35-year perspective. New England Journal
of Medicine, 355(3), 281–287. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1056
/NEJMsa053910, PMID: 16855268

King, M. M., Bergstrom, C. T., Correll, S. J., Jacquet, J., & Westen, J. D.
(2017). Men set their own cites high: Gender and self-citation

across fields and over time. Socius. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177
/2378023117738903

Knapp, M. (2010). Are participation rates sufficient to explain gen-
der differences in chess performance? Proceedings of the Royal
Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1692), 2269–2270. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.2257, PMID: 20335203,
PMCID: PMC2894898

Kwiek, M. (2016). The European research elite: A cross-national study
of highly productive academics in 11 Länder. Higher Education,
71, 379–397. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-015-9910-x
Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., Cronin, B., & Sugimoto, C. R.
(2013). Bibliometrics: Global gender disparities in science.
Natur, 504, 211–213. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/504211a,
PMID: 24350369

Levin, S. G., & Stephan, P. E. (1998). Gender differences in the rewards
to publishing in academe: Science in the 1970s. Sex Roles, 38,
1049–1064. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018882711314
Ley, T., & Hamilton, B. (2008). The gender gap in NIH grant appli-
Kationen. Wissenschaft, 322(5907), 1472–1474. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1126/science.1165878, PMID: 19056961

Leydesdorff, L., & Park, H. W. (2016). Full and fractional counting

in bibliometric networks. arXiv, arXiv:1611.06943.

Lippa, R. A. (2010). Gender differences in personality and interests:
Wann, Wo, and why? Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
4(11), 1098–1110. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004
.2010.00320.X

Ma, N., Guan, J., & Zhao, Y. (2008). Bringing PageRank to the ci-
tation analysis. Information Processing & Management, 44(2),
800–810. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2007.06.006

Macaluso, B., Larivière, V., Sugimoto, T., & Sugimoto, C. R. (2016).
Is science built on the shoulders of women? A study of gender
differences in contributorship. Academic Medicine, 91(8),
1 1 3 6 – 1 1 4 2 . D O I : h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 9 7 / A C M
.0000000000001261, PMID: 27276004

Marsh, H. W., Jayasinghe, U. W., & Bond, N. W. (2011). Gender
differences in peer reviews of grant applications: A substantive-
methodological synergy in support of the null hypothesis model.
Journal of Informetrics, 5(1), 167–180. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.joi.2010.10.004

Milkman, K. L., Akinola, M., & Chugh, D. (2015). What happens
Vor? A field experiment exploring how pay and representation
differentially shape bias on the pathway into organizations.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(6), 1678–1712. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000022, PMID: 25867167

Müller, D. ICH., & Wai, J. (2015). The bachelor’s to Ph.D. STEM pipe-
line no longer leaks more women than men: A 30-year analysis.
Grenzen in der Psychologie, 6, 37. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389
/fpsyg.2015.00037, PMID: 25741293, PMCID: PMC4331608
Moldwin, M. B., & Liemohn, M. W. (2018). High-citation papers in
space physics: Examination of gender, country, and paper char-
acteristics. Journal of Geophysical Research, 123(4), 2557–2565.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/2018JA025291

Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., &
Handelsman, J. (2012). Science faculty subtle gender biases favor
male students. Verfahren der Nationalen Akademie der Wissenschaften
of the United States of America, 109(41), 16474–16479. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211286109, PMID: 22988126,
PMCID: PMC3478626

Mutz, R., Bornmann, L., & Daniel, H. D. (2012). Does gender
matter in grant peer review? An empirical investigation using the
example of the Austrian Science Fund. Zeitschrift für Psychologie,
220(2), 121–129. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604
/a000103, PMID: 23480982, PMCID: PMC3414231

Quantitative Science Studies

251

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

NASEM Report. (2018). Sexual harassment of women. US National

Academies of Science, Engineering and Medicine.

National Research Council. (2009). Gender differences at critical
transitions in the careers of science, engineering and mathematics
faculty. National Academies Press.

O’Dea, R. E., Lagisz, M., Jennions, M. D., & Nakagawa, S. (2018).
Gender differences in individual variation in academic grades
fail to fit expected patterns for STEM. Nature Communications,
9(1), 3777. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-06292-0,
PMID: 30254267, PMCID: PMC6156605

Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., & Tetlock, P.
(2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis
of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychologie, 105(2), 171–192. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037
/a0032734, PMID: 23773046

Perianes-Rodriguez, A., Waltman, L., & van Eck, N. J. (2016).
Constructing bibliometric networks: A comparison between full
and fractional counting. Journal of Informetrics, 10(4), 1178–1195.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joi.2016.10.006

Perley, D. A. (2019). Gender and the career outcomes of PhD
astronomers in the United States. Publications of the Astronomical
Society of the Pacific, 131(1005), 114502. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1088/1538-3873/ab0cc4

Pinski, G., & Narin, F. (1976). Citation influence for journal aggre-
gates of scientific publications: Theory, with application to the
literature of physics. Information Processing & Management,
12(5), 297–312. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4573(76)
90048-0

Porter, A. M., & Ivie, R. (2019). Women in physics and astronomy.

American Institute of Physics.

Radicchi, F., Fortunato, S., Markines, B., & Vespignani, A. (2009).
Diffusion of scientific credits and the ranking of scientists.
Physical Review, E80, 056103. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1103
/PhysRevE.80.056103, PMID: 20365039

Raffo, J. (2016). Worldwide gender-name dictionary. WIPO Economics
& Statistics Related Resources 10, World Intellectual Property
Organization.

Rasmussen, K. C., Maier, E., Strauss, B. E., Durbin, M., Riesbeck, L.,
… Erena, A. (2019). The nonbinary fraction: Looking towards the
future of gender equity in astronomy. arXiv, arXiv:1907.04893.
Rossi, P., & Strumia, A., & Torre, A. (2019). Bibliometrics for col-

laboration works. arXiv, arXiv:1902.01693.

Ruocco, G., Daraio, C., Folli, V., & Leonetti, M. (2017).
Bibliometric indicators: The origin of their log-normal distribu-
tion and why they are not a reliable proxy for an individual
scholar’s talent. Palgrave Communications, 3, 17064. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1057/palcomms.2017.64

Sax, L. J., Hagedorn, L. S., Arredondo, M., & Dicrisi, F. A. (2002).
Faculty research productivity: Exploring the role of gender and
family-related factors. Research in Higher Education, 43, 423–446.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015575616285

Sinatra, R., Deville, P., Szell, M., Wang, D., & Barabasi, A. L. (2005).
A century of physics. Nature Physics, 11, 791–796. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.1038/nphys3494

Stack, S. (2004). Gender, children and research productivity. Forschung
in Higher Education, 45, 891–920. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007
/s11162-004-5953-z

Stevens, S., & Haidt, J. (2017). The Google memo: What does the
research say about gender differences? Heterodox: The Blog,
August 10. https://heterodoxacademy.org/blog/the-google-
memo-what-does-the-research-say-about-gender-differences/.
Stewart-Williams, S., Thomas, A. G., Blackburn, J. D., & Chan, C. Y. M.
(2019). Reactions to male-favoring vs. female-favoring sex differences:

A preregistered experiment. British Journal of Psychology. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12463, PMID: 32701171

Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2018). The gender-equality paradox in
Wissenschaft, Technologie, engineering, and mathematics education.
Psychological Science, 29(4), 581–593. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1177/0956797617741719, PMID: 29442575

Strumia, A. (2019). Why are women under-represented in physics?
Quillette, April 16. https://quillette.com/2019/04/16/why-are
-women-under-represented-in-physics/.

Strumia, A., & Torre, R. (2019). Biblioranking fundamental physics.
Journal of Informetrics, 13(2), 515–539. DOI: https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.joi.2019.01.011

Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. ICH. (2009). Men and things, Frauen
and people: A meta-analysis of sex differences in interests.
Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 859–884. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1037/a0017364, PMID: 19883140

Su, R., & Rounds, J. (2015). All STEM fields are not created equal:
People and things interests explain gender disparities across
STEM fields. Grenzen in der Psychologie, 6, 189. DOI: https://doi
.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00189, PMID: 25762964, PMCID:
PMC4340183

Tahamtan, ICH., & Bornmann, L. (2019). What do citation counts measure?
An updated review of studies on citations in scientific documents
published between 2006 Und 2018. arXiv, arXiv:1906.04588. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-019-03243-4

Thelwall, M. (2018). Do females create higher impact research?
Scopus citations and Mendeley readers for articles from five
Länder. Journal of Informetrics, 12(4), 1031–1041. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joi.2018.08.005

Thelwall, M., Bailey, C., Tobin, C., & Bradshaw, N.-A. (2019).
Gender differences in research areas, methods and topics: Can
people and thing orientations explain the results? Zeitschrift für
Informetrics, 13(1), 149–169. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joi
.2018.12.002

Thelwall, M., & Wilson, P. (2014). Regression for citation data: Ein
evaluation of different methods. Journal of Informetrics, 8, 963.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joi.2014.09.011

Torvik, V. ICH., & Agarwal, S. (2016). ETHNEA—An instance-based
ethnicity classifier based on geo-coded author names in a
large-scale bibliographic database. International Symposium on
Science of Science, March 22–23.

Waltman, L. (2016). A review of the literature on citation impact
indicators. Journal of Informetrics, 10(2), 365–391. DOI: https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.joi.2016.02.007

Wang, M. T., Eccles, J., & Kenney, S. (2013). Not lack of ability but
more choice: Individual and gender differences in choice of
careers in science, Technologie, engineering, and mathematics.
Psychological Science, 24(5), 770–775. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1177/0956797612458937, PMID: 23508740

Way, S. F., Larremore, D. B., & Clauset, A. (2016). Gender, Prod-
tivity, and prestige in computer science faculty hiring networks.
Proceedings of the 25th International Conference on World Wide
Netz (S. 1169–1179). New York: Association for Computing
Machinery. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/2872427.2883073
Wennerås, C., & Wold, A. (1997). Nepotism and sexism in peer-
Rezension. Natur, 387, 341–343. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038
/387341a0, PMID: 9163412

Westen, J. D., Jacquet, J., King, M. M., Correll, S. J., & Bergstrom, C. T.
(2013). The role of gender in scholarly authorship. PLOS ONE, 8,
e66212. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066212,
PMID: 23894278, PMCID: PMC3718784

Westen, J. D., Jensen, M. C., Dandrea, R. J., Gordon, G. J., &
Bergstrom, C. T. (2014). Author-level eigenfactor metrics:

Quantitative Science Studies

252

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Gender issues in fundamental physics

Evaluating the influence of authors, institutions, and countries
within the social science research network community. Zeitschrift
of the American Society for Information Science and Technology,
64(4), 787–801. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.22790

Williams, W. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2017). National hiring experiments
aufdecken 2:1 faculty preference for women on STEM tenure track.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 112(17), 5360–5365. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1073/pnas.1418878112, PMID: 25870272, PMCID:
PMC4418903

Winegard, B., Clark, C. J., Hasty, C., & Baumeister, R. (2018).
Equalitarianism: A source of liberal bias. SSRN Electronic Journal.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3175680

Witteman, H. O., Hendricks, M., Straus, S., & Tannenbaum, C.
(2019). Female grant applicants are equally successful when peer
reviewers assess the science, but not when they assess the scien-
tist. Lancet, 393(10171), 531–540. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016
/S0140-6736(18)32611-4

Wolfinger, N. H., Mason, M. A., & Goulden, M. (2008). Problems
in the pipeline: Gender, Hochzeit, and fertility in the ivory tower.
Journal of Higher Education, 79(4), 388–405. DOI: https://doi.org
/10.1080/00221546.2008.11772108

Wolfram Mathematica. (n.d.). Determine the gender of given name.
https://www.wolfram.com/mathematica/new-in-10/built-in
-classifier-collection/determine-the-gender-of-given-name.html.
World Economic Forum. (2016). The global gender gap report.

World Economic Freedom.

Xie, Y., & Shauman, K. A. (1998). Sex differences in research produc-
tivity: New evidence about an old puzzle. American Sociological
Rezension, 63(6), 847–870. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/2657505
Xie, Y., & Shauman, K. A. (2003). Women in science: Career pro-
cesses and outcomes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zitt, M., & Small, H. (2008). Modifying the journal impact factor by
fractional citation weighting: The audience factor. Journal of the
American Society for Information Science and Technology, 59(11),
1856–1860. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.20880

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

/

e
D
u
Q
S
S
/
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

2
1
2
2
5
1
9
0
6
6
0
5
Q
S
S
_
A
_
0
0
1
1
4
P
D

/

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Quantitative Science Studies

253RESEARCH ARTICLE image
RESEARCH ARTICLE image

PDF Herunterladen