Lifetime Learning as a Factor in

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in
Life History Evolution

John A. Bullinaria*
University of Birmingham

Schlüsselwörter
Artificial life, life history, evolution,
learning, neural networks

Abstract An artificial life approach is taken to explore the effect
that lifetime learning can have on the evolution of certain life history
traits, in particular the periods of protection that parents offer
their young, and the age at first reproduction of those young. Der
study begins by simulating the evolution of simple artificial neural
network systems that must learn quickly to perform well on simple
classification tasks, and determining if and when extended periods
of parental protection emerge. It is concluded that longer periods of
parental protection of children do offer clear learning advantages and
better adult performance, but only if procreation is not allowed during
the protection period. In this case, a compromise protection period
evolves that balances the improved learning performance against
reduced procreation period. The crucial properties of the neural
learning processes are then abstracted out to explore the possibility
of studying the effect of learning more generally and with better
computational efficiency. Hindurch, the implications of these
simulations for more realistic scenarios are discussed.

1 Einführung

The term ‘‘life history’’ refers to the sequence of events and changes that take place during an organ-
ism’s lifetime. This covers issues such as stages of growth, reaching reproductive maturity, Prod-
tion and nurturing of offspring, menopause, and death. There are numerous tradeoffs involved in
their timing, with widely different patterns of such events evolving for different species and different
environmental conditions (z.B., [17, 27, 30, 31]). Classic examples include the tradeoff between
reproduction and growth [35], and between nursing and survival [15]. There also appear to be cor-
relations between various aspects of life history, Zum Beispiel, fixed ratios within lineages of life span
to age of maturity [13, 18]. This study takes an artificial life approach to look specifically at one factor
that has received relatively little attention in the past, nämlich, how the ability to learn during an
individual’s lifetime affects the optimal period of protection that parents should provide for their
Kinder, and how that should affect the children’s age at first reproduction. The general idea that
slow growth and extended infant dependency could be associated with learning is not new (z.B., sehen
the discussions in [5, 21, 22, 24]), but the study presented here appears to be the first to simulate
explicitly the interaction of learning and life history evolution.

In nature, parental protection varies enormously, from precocial species in which the young are born
well developed and require virtually no protection, to altricial species in which the young are born
helpless and require long periods of parental care before they are capable of surviving on their own.

* School of Computer Science, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, B15 2TT, Vereinigtes Königreich. Email: j.a.bullinaria@cs.bham.ac.uk

N 2009 Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Artificial Life 15: 389 – 409 (2009)

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

There is also some variation in how the age of first reproduction relates to the period of protection. Es
might seem obvious that early first reproduction is advantageous because it increases the chance that
individuals will survive to reproduce, and decreases the time wasted not reproducing [16]. Jedoch, Wenn
parents protect their young, the first advantage will be less relevant, and the second may be balanced if
avoiding the costs of early reproduction leads to improved performance later in life, with increased re-
production overall. Tatsächlich, there are many factors involving growth, Größe, fecundity, Umfeld, Und
bald, that are known to affect the age at maturity and first reproduction (z.B., [26, 31]). One major
advantage of simplified artificial life simulations is that they render it possible to explore the individual
factors without all the confounds inherent in empirical measurements of existing biological populations.
This investigation was prompted by the observation that human infants are particularly altricial,
even compared with other primates, and require extended periods of parental protection and support
(z.B., [23]). There are numerous factors that could have led to this (z.B., [21, 28]), but there are two
particularly important processes that are known to take place during the protection stage of altricial
species—the infants are growing, and they are learning. Human infants clearly do need to grow
considerably after birth, and consequently survival without parental protection would be virtually
unmöglich. Learning is also crucial for humans, and for other species for which relatively complex
behaviors are required, since encoding all the necessary skills genetically is likely to be difficult, Und
even if that were evolutionarily possible, adaptation would still be needed to cope with their rapid
growth processes and the changing and unpredictable nature of their environment. Ansonsten, innate
behavior would be adequate [7], obwohl, where possible, the costs of learning will tend to result in
genetic assimilation of the learned behaviors [3, 36]. This article aims to explore the extent to which
such learning processes alone could be responsible the evolution of long protection periods. Studies of
the effect of brain growth and adult brain sizes on life history variables [24], and arguments that the
human’s unique pattern of early neurological development is responsible for the substantially earlier
weaning of humans than of the great apes [22], already lend weight to the idea that neural processes
play an important role in determining the details of human life history. Striedter [34] provides a good
overview of the factors involved in brain evolution and the differences observed across species.

This study begins by simulating the evolution of populations of learning individuals using artificial
neural networks that must learn quickly how to perform well on simplified classification tasks. Mit
individuals of all ages competing to survive and procreate based on their learned performance, Die
effect of different extended periods of parental protection can be explored. Allowing the protection
period to evolve demonstrates how the advantages of protection trade off against any associated
disadvantages so that a particular period of protection emerges through evolution. Varying the
details of the simulations (insbesondere, how the typical life span of the simulated species affects the
protection period, and how the age of first reproduction might be affected by the parental protec-
tion) renders it possible to determine how these various factors interact, and how different patterns
of behavior could arise from other species-specific factors. Dann, by abstracting out the crucial prop-
erties of the neural learning process, it is possible to explore the effect of learning more generally.
The next section describes the approach taken to simulate the evolution of populations of neural
network learners. Abschnitt 3 then establishes baseline performance results for populations that evolve
for a selection of fixed protection periods, before the protection period itself is allowed to evolve as
discussed in Section 4. The properties of the evolved neural networks are analyzed in Section 5, Die
robustness of the simulation results is checked in Section 6, and the associated tradeoffs are explored
in Section 7. The potential for extending the results to more abstract general learning processes is
considered in Section 8. Endlich, the article ends in Section 9 with some conclusions and a discussion
of the implications for more realistic scenarios.

2 Evolving Neural Network Learners

To maintain as wide a relevance as possible, this study aims to adopt a fairly abstract approach for
simulating the crucial features of the evolution of most animal populations, with particular emphasis

390

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

on the aspects of fitness associated with lifetime learning. Since it is the brains of animals that are
largely responsible for their ability to learn complex behaviors, it is natural to begin by representing
the animal populations by simple artificial neural network models. The extent to which it really is
necessary to include explicit neural learning, rather than simpler approximations to such learning
processes, will be explored later, in Section 8.

The artificial life simulations here will involve populations of individual neural networks, each spec-
ified by a set of innate parameters, that must learn from a continuous stream of input patterns how to
classify future input patterns. The inputs could, Zum Beispiel, correspond to relevant observable
features of plants or other animals, and the desired output classes (categories) could correspond to
important properties of them, such as being edible or dangerous. The fitness of each individual will be
measured by how well it classifies the new inputs before discovering their correct classes and learning
von ihnen. By forcing the individuals to compete to survive and procreate, according to their relative
fitness, populations of increasing fitness can be expected to emerge. Darüber hinaus, in order to compete
effectively in a population consisting of individuals of all ages, each individual must not only learn how
to perform well, but also be able to learn quickly how to achieve that good performance, or at least
quickly enough that it can survive after its parents have withdrawn their protection.

Proceeding with such simulations obviously requires the choice of a specific concrete learning
system and training data set(S), and it makes sense to employ a setup that has already been explored
in some detail and proved instructive elsewhere [6, 8 –10]. Folglich, standard fully connected
multi-layer perceptron neural networks were used, with one hidden layer, sigmoidal processing units,
and training by gradient descent weight updates using the cross-entropy error function on simple
classification (d.h., categorization) tasks [4]. Since most real-world classification tasks involve learning
nonlinear decision boundaries in a space of real-valued inputs, the set of tasks was chosen to have
continuous two-dimensional input spaces, conveniently normalized to a unit square, with particular
circular classification boundaries. Each input to a neural network was thus represented by a position
in the unit square input space, and the network output represented the predicted probability that the
input corresponded to a particular class or category. The aim was to learn from past experience (d.h.,
binary target outputs) the class of each new input, mit anderen Worten, to discover where the boundaries
between classes lay in the input space. This setup was sufficiently simple to allow extensive
simulations, yet involved the crucial features and difficulties of real-world problems. It was important
that the networks did not evolve to cope only with a particular single data set, so each individual
network was assigned a randomly chosen classification boundary of the specified type, and had to
learn from a stream of randomly drawn data points from the input space. The individual fitness at
each stage could then be defined as the generalization ability, das ist, the average number of inputs
correctly classified (z.B., defined as having the network outputs —i.e., class probabilities — within 0.2
of the binary targets) before training on them.

An additional factor that complicates this learning process for many tasks is that humans and
some other animal species have critical periods for learning, and outside that period the learning is
more difficult [2, 20]. It is not clear how, or even if, that will interact with a period of parental
protection. It has certainly been demonstrated in previous computational studies that evolving neural
network learning rates that vary during the learning process does lead to improved learning
Leistung, and that the evolved time (T ) dependences are qualitatively similar to humanlike age
dependences [7, 9, 10]. Those previous studies indicate that, for current purposes, such time-
dependent learning rates DL(T ) can be conveniently approximated by introducing a simple two-
parameter exponential scale factor s(T ) to multiply innate initial learning rates DL(0):

gLðtÞ ¼ sðtÞ gLð0Þ;

sðtÞ ¼ b þ ð1 (cid:5) bÞe-t=s

in which t is the age of the individual in simulated years, the baseline h specifies the ratio of the final
to initial learning rates, and the time constant H determines how quickly the learning rate rises or falls
toward the final value. Both h and H are evolved to take on the positive values that result in the best

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

391

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

performance under the given circumstances. Clearly, if time dependence of this type proves not to be
beneficial, the parameters will be able to evolve so that s(T ) = 1. Note also that the subscript L has
been introduced because earlier studies [6, 8, 10] have shown empirically that better performance can
be obtained by evolving separate learning rates DL and random initial weight distributions [(cid:5)rL, +rL]
for each of the four distinct network components L (the input-to-hidden weights IH, the hidden unit
biases HB, the hidden-to-output weights HO, and the output unit biases OB), rather than having
identical parameters across the whole network. Wieder, if this proves not to be beneficial in this
particular case, the parameters will be able to evolve so that they are equal across any combination of
components for which that proves useful.

The four initial weight parameters rL, four learning rates DL, and two variable plasticity param-
eters H and h, together with a standard momentum parameter a and weight decay regularization
parameter E [4], give a total of 12 real-valued evolvable innate parameters for each network. In
principle, the number of hidden units could also be evolved, but doing that invariably results in it
rising quickly to whatever limit is placed on it, considerably slowing down the simulations in the
Verfahren, so this is usually best kept fixed at some appropriate value [10]. For the current study it was
fixed at 20 hidden units for all networks, which was found to be more than enough for successful
learning of the given tasks.

After specifying the details of the learning individuals, they then needed to be integrated into the
evolutionary process. This required the neural network learning time scales to be aligned with the
lifetime and evolutionary time scales. This was conveniently done by defining a simulated year of
experience to be 1,200 training data samples, and computing the fitness of each individual at the end of
each simulated year as the average classification performance over that year. This simulated year
could then be used as a common unit of time across all the learning, lifetime, and evolutionary pro-
Prozesse. A computationally feasible fixed population size of 200 was maintained throughout (con-
sistent with the idea that there are fixed total food resources available to support the population),
achieved by replacing the individuals that died with children of the fittest individuals. Deaths
occurred by losing a fitness comparison ‘‘fight’’ against other individuals, or randomly due to old age
beyond a natural life span. The details of all these evolutionary factors needed to be fixed to en-
courage evolutionary change and preserve a reasonably diverse population, but fortunately the pre-
cise specifications proved not to affect the qualitative results a great deal, so convenient round
numbers were chosen for the associated parameter values.

The time scale was chosen so that learning the given tasks typically took a humanlike 10 Zu
15 Jahre, and the natural life span was defined to be 30 Jahre, beyond which a random 20% of older
individuals died each year. This allowed individuals enough training samples to learn their given tasks
and have a reasonable period during which they were fit enough to reproduce, yet prevented the
populations from becoming dominated by a few very old and very fit individuals. Zusätzlich, jeder von
the unprotected individuals was forced to compete each year with another randomly chosen eligible
individual, and would die if its fitness proved to be lower than its competitor’s. To preserve a rea-
sonable population age distribution, deaths in this way were limited to 10% of the population each
Jahr, though for populations with relatively large parental protection periods, this limit was rarely
reached. The children were generated by crossover and mutation from two parents chosen each year
by pairwise fitness comparisons of the eligible individuals. This was conveniently implemented by
having each child inherit innate parameters chosen randomly from the corresponding ranges spanned
by its two parents, plus a random mutation (from a Gaussian distribution) that gave it a reasonable
chance of falling outside that range. A litter size of one was chosen, since that appears to be optimal
for large primates [25]. Clearly, these details are gross oversimplifications of real biological processes,
but they constitute a manageable starting point that includes approximations of all the key processes.
This simple nature-inspired algorithm, tracking an evolving population of aging individuals over
Zeit, with the oldest and least fit individuals tending to die and be replaced by children of the fittest
individuals at the end of each simulated year, has already been shown to work well in practice [9, 10].
The crucial additional feature here is that the children can be protected by their parents until they
reach a certain age, and cannot be killed by competitors before then. This introduces an implicit cost

392

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

to the parents in that the more children are protected, the higher the chance they stand of being in
the maximum of 10% of the population that die through competition each year, and the higher the
chance they have to compete against another adult rather than a less experienced child. A cost that
the children can, and will, be forced to bear is that they are prevented from having any children of
their own before they leave the protection of their parents. The effect of this cost is something that
will be considered in some detail later.

The following sections will present simulation results that explore how the protection period
affects the performance of the evolved individuals, and what protection period emerges if it is left
free to evolve in the same way as all the other innate parameters. Erste, Jedoch, es ist wichtig
note some of the simplifications inherent in the definition of the protection process: The parents are
assumed to be perfect protectors, whereas in reality their own fitness will normally affect their
protection abilities, as will the number of children they are protecting at any given time. Darüber hinaus,
real parental protection will usually influence the training data available to the infants, but this
complication has been ignored and the nature of the training data kept constant across all cases.
Obviously, these design choices are oversimplifications, but wherever possible, for each such factor
in the simulations, care was taken to make the design decision in such a way that the parental
protection effect was least likely to emerge. Dann, if it did, there could be some confidence that it
really was an important and robust effect. One factor likely to reduce the parental protection relates
to the fact that there will usually be more direct costs to parents protecting young (z.B., [14]), but this
proved difficult to quantify within the current framework. This complication was therefore ignored
for the present purposes, though a better account of the reproductive and protection costs will
certainly be required for more realistic models.

3 Simulation Results for Fixed Protection Periods

The natural starting point was to perform the evolutionary neural network simulations described
above for a carefully selected range of fixed protection periods to determine if there are any
differences between the populations that evolve. The learning time scale was set so that evolved
individuals were typically able to learn the given task in 10 Zu 20 simulated years, and after 30
simulated years they start dying of old age, so protection periods of 1, 10, Und 20 years formed a
suitable representative sample. The evolution of the initial learning rates DL(0) for these three cases
are shown in Figure 1, with means and variances over 10 runs (which proved sufficient to establish
statistically significant results). In each case, the pattern of evolved parameters and relatively low
variances across runs are similar to those found in earlier studies [9, 10], but subtle differences can
be seen between the final parameter values, and the evolutionary process is noticeably slower to
settle down for the longer protection periods.

The means of generalization error performance across populations during evolution for each
protection period are compared in the bottom-right graph of Figure 1. This appears to show that the
two longer protection periods do have a clear advantage in this respect. Jedoch, such simple
population averages are built up from complex age-dependent error distributions, and it is inevitable
that the population age distributions will depend considerably on the protection period. Insbesondere,
longer protection periods will tend to result in populations with more older, and hence fitter,
individuals, so observing improved average population fitness alone is not sufficient to demonstrate
that extended protection periods really do result in improved individual performance. To show that
they do provide a real evolutionary advantage, the protection period needs to be allowed to evolve
alongside all the other parameters.

4 Simulation Results for Evolved Protection Periods

For simulations in which the protection period is allowed to evolve, as another real-valued
parameter, the evolution of that period and the associated initial learning rates DL(0) are as shown

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

393

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 1. Evolution of the average initial learning rates DL(0) and variances over 10 runs for fixed protection periods of 1,
10, Und 20 Jahre, and comparison of the corresponding performance error rates, where the subscript refers to the relevant
network connections (IH, HO) and biases (HB, OB). It is clear that the protection period does affect what evolves.

in Abbildung 2, again with means and variances over 10 runs. During the early stages of evolution, while
all the individuals are still performing relatively poorly, the protection period rises rapidly to about
17 Jahre, but then falls slightly, settling down at around 15 Jahre.

The length of the protection period, whether it is fixed (bei 1, 10, 20 Jahre) or evolved (leading to
Ev c 15 Jahre), has significant consequences for the evolved learning processes. The effect on the
initial learning rates DL(0), as seen in Figures 1 Und 2, is barely noticeable, but the effect on the age
dependences of the learning rates is very clear. Figur 3 shows how the scale factor time constant H
and baseline h of the evolved learning rate vary with protection period, and the corresponding scale

Figur 2. Evolution of the average initial learning rates DL(0) and variances over 10 runs when the protection period is
allowed to evolve (links), and the evolution of the protection period (Rechts). A well-defined optimal protection period of
um 15 years emerges.

394

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 3. The effect of different protection periods on the evolved age dependence of the learning rates. Plotted are the
evolved baseline h and time scale H (links), and the resultant learning rate scale factors s (Rechts), for the representative
fixed protection periods (1, 10, 20) and evolved period (Ev c 15). Longer protection periods lead to the evolution of
longer learning periods.

factors s as a function of age. Increased protection periods mean there is less urgency to learn, Und
this results in a lengthening of the period during which significant learning takes place, und ein
consequent reduction of learning rates needed during that period. This will provide an evolutionary
advantage if the extended learning period results in better performance after the period of protection
has ended, and could thus provide a reason why extended protection periods should evolve.

Wie oben beschrieben, Jedoch, the protection period will also affect the age distribution of the
Bevölkerung, and this can also affect its evolution. The averages and variances of the crucial factors
are compared for the various protection periods in the four graphs of Figure 4. An increased pro-
tection period will reduce the number of deaths per year due to competition from the maximum of
20 (top left). This will result in more individuals living longer and increase the average age of the
Bevölkerung (top right), and lead to more individuals living long enough to start dying of old age (top
links). Gesamt, there is still a net reduction in the number of deaths per year, and so, given the fixed
population size, the average number of children per individual at any given time decreases with
increasing protection period (bottom-left), without any introduced direct cost for parents protecting
more children. Together these factors result in rather different age distributions for the various
Populationen (bottom right). Each age distribution is fairly flat during the protection period, and then
falls off due to competition until the individuals start dying of old age from the age of 30, at which
point there is an exponential fall toward zero.

The various trends observed in Figures 3 Und 4 are all monotonic with respect to the protection
Zeitraum, and the evolved-protection-period population results are consistent with what would be
expected from their evolved period of around 15 Jahre. The question arises as to what causes the
evolved protection period to stabilize at the value it does, rather than at some higher or lower value.
It is unlikely that longer protection periods could result in inferior learning, because the evolution can
easily adjust the learning process to ignore any final part of the protection period that worsens the
Leistung. Jedoch, even if increased protection periods result in better individual performance,
there will still be an associated reduction in the number of children per individual, and that will place
those individuals at an evolutionary disadvantage, which will tend to drive a reduction in the
protection period. There will be a tradeoff between improved individual fitness and reduced poten-
tial for procreation. To explore this tradeoff, the next section investigates more carefully the per-
formance of the evolved populations.

5 Analysis of the Evolved Performance

Understanding the evolved performance fully requires going beyond the population averages of
the previous two sections. Figur 5 shows the means and variances of the individual performance

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

395

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Figur 4. The effect of different protection periods on the evolved populations: the average death rates (top left), Die
average age of individuals (top right), the average number of children per individual (bottom left), and the resultant age
distributions within the population (bottom right).

error rates (d.h., inverse fitness) during learning. There do appear to be major reductions in the mean
error rates resulting from increased protection periods, with associated delays in reaching those
lower error rates, as permitted by the delayed need to compete. Jedoch, the variances in the error
rates are extremely large, mainly due to long tails at the high end of the distribution of errors, and this
renders it difficult to analyze the significance of the mean performance differences. This rather
skewed distribution of errors has been observed before, and is a consequence of the evolutionary

Figur 5. The mean individual performance error rates (links) and variances (Rechts) during learning for the evolved pop-
ulations with different protection periods. Longer protection periods result in slower learning but better final performance.

396

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 6. The median performance error rates during learning (links) and the distribution of errors for all individuals
zwischen dem Alter von 50 Und 60 (Rechts), for the different protection periods. The median performance is perfect by about
12 Jahre alt, but the population has many large errors persisting into old age.

pressure to learn quickly resulting in individuals adopting risky learning strategies, which lead occa-
sionally to very poor performance [9, 10]. Since extended protection periods reduce the pressure to
learn quickly, it is easy to understand the observed performance improvements.

The median learning performance error rates, which are not affected by the long tails of the error
distributions, are shown on the left of Figure 6, and indicate essentially perfect performance by
Alter 12 for all protection periods, though with the expected slower initial learning for longer pro-
tection periods. The distributions of errors for older individuals (aged between 50 Und 60), shown on
the right of Figure 6, give an indication of the extent of the small numbers of cases of very large
error that persist even for long protection periods. There is certainly a massive peak around zero
Fehler, as would be expected at those ages, but there remain a significant number of very large errors.
The upper and lower quartile performance error rates, shown in Figure 7, are consistent with the
expectation that longer protection periods will slow the learning, and also confirm that they improve
the learned performance at the poorer performance end of the spectrum.

To reinforce the above understanding, all the evolutionary simulations were repeated without
allowing age dependences in the learning rates [12]. In this case, there is no easy mechanism for
longer protection periods to lead to slowing and extending of the learning process, as was achieved
before by modifying the age-dependent learning rate scale factor as seen in Figure 3. Jedoch,
evolution of other learning parameter differences still allows longer protection periods to result in
significant improvements in the error distribution for old individuals and in lower-quartile learning

Figur 7. The upper- and lower-quartile performance error rates during learning (left and right respectively), für die
different protection periods. Longer protection periods massively improve the chances of achieving perfect performance
later in life.

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

397

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Leistung, with relatively modest slowing of the upper quartile and median learning, as shown in
Figur 8. In this case, the evolved protection period is about 16 Jahre, only slightly longer than found
Vor. The differences and improvements afforded by the possibility of age-dependent learning
rates confirms the importance of including in the simulations as many as possible of the known
features of real learning systems.

6 Robustness of the Evolutionary Simulations

The above simulations and analyses have established that longer protection periods do offer clear
learning advantages, and relatively few disadvantages, whether or not age-dependent learning rates
are allowed. Evolving the protection period suggests that there is a tradeoff between this learning
advantage and the cost of reduced procreation opportunities. Jedoch, evolutionary simulations can
easily stall in less than optimal configurations, particularly if there is a loss of diversity in the evolving
Populationen, or a poor choice of initial population [10]. A check is therefore required to verify that
the evolved protection periods are not simply some artifact of the chosen evolutionary process,
perhaps corresponding to a local optimum of fitness and/or lack of population diversity. This can be
done by allowing the protection period in the three fixed-period evolved populations to evolve away
from its previously fixed values. The results of this are shown on the left of Figure 9, with means and
variances across 10 runs. For each case, there is a relatively fast rise or fall to the same evolved period
of around 15 years that emerged before. A qualitatively identical pattern of results emerge for
populations evolved without age-dependent learning rates [12].

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Figur 8. The upper-quartile, median, and lower-quartile performance error rates during learning (top left, top right,
bottom left) and the distribution of errors for all individuals between the ages of 50 Und 60 (bottom right), für die
different protection periods, when age-dependent learning rates are not allowed. Comparison with Figures 6 Und 7
reveals a clear reduction in performance levels.

398

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 9. Allowing the protection periods to evolve starting from the fixed-period populations (links), and natural selection
working on the combined evolved populations leading to the dominance of the evolved-protection-period individuals
(Rechts). These confirm the robustness of the evolved protection period of about 15 Jahre.

An additional test involves combining the evolved populations from all four cases (the three
different fixed period runs, and one evolved period run) into one big population, and allowing natural
selection to take its course. Since the initial constituent populations had already been optimized by
evolution, the children inherited characteristics from just their fittest parent, with no further cross-
over or mutation allowed. The results of this process are shown on the right of Figure 9, with means
and variances across 20 runs. Individuals with virtually no parental protection are wiped out almost
immediately, and eventually individuals with the evolved protection period come to dominate the
whole population. Wieder, a qualitatively identical pattern of results emerges for populations evolved
without age dependences in the learning rates [12]. It seems that the evolved protection periods are
quite robust.

7 Exploration of the Tradeoffs

The simulations described above have demonstrated that, although there are clear learning
advantages to having longer protection periods, extending those protection periods into effectively
fixed life spans restricts the available procreation opportunities, and that in turn places those
populations at a serious evolutionary disadvantage. The evolving populations manage this tradeoff,
and establish a suitable compromise value for their protection period, appropriate for individuals
that start dying of old age after 30 Jahre. Two issues relevant to this tradeoff need further inves-
tigation: How does the overall individual life span affect the balance, and how important is the pre-
vention of procreation while being protected?

Repeating the above evolutionary simulations with the onset of old age at different ages, corre-
sponding to different natural life spans, will show how that affects the emergent protection period
and associated performance levels. Figur 10 presents the results of doing that. Beyond what might
be termed the natural learning time scale of about 10 Jahre, there is a fairly linear relation between the
protection period and life span. There is also a clear improvement in the individual adult perfor-
mance levels as the life span becomes longer. These demonstrate that the effect of learning is of
sufficient importance that the amount of time devoted to it continues to increase as the life spans
become longer, rather than using all the extra time for procreation. Future studies might consider the
extent to which such learning-driven extended protection periods will lead to the coevolution of
longer growing periods, zu. There are known to be correlations between age of maturity and life
spans in biological populations, though there are certainly other factors involved there besides learn-
ing (z.B., [13, 18, 32, 33]). Interessant, after controlling for body size, there is also a significant cor-
relation between brain weight and life span among higher primates [1]. It certainly seems plausible
that a correlation should exist between the weight of the brain and the protection period that would
lead to its optimal learning performance.

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

399

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 10. The effect of the age of onset of old age, and hence the life span, on the evolved protection period (links), Und
learning performance levels (Rechts). These demonstrate the importance of an
the corresponding average individual
extended protection period for learning, relative to a potential increase in procreation period.

A further set of simulations have demonstrated that if the life span is also allowed to evolve freely
within the current setup, it keeps on increasing indefinitely. In biological populations, Natürlich, Dort
are many other factors that act to restrict life spans (z.B., [21, 31]), but simulating such tradeoffs is
beyond the scope of this study. Ähnlich, simulating factors that might result in reproduction
stopping early within the typical life span, tendencies for increased reproductive problems at older
Alter, the effects of grandmothering, and such like [19, 21] are also left for future studies.

In all the above simulations, it has been decreed that procreation is not allowed while being
protected, and it has been argued that this is behind the tradeoff that has restricted the protection
Zeitraum. This restriction on procreation is clearly more true of some natural species than others.
Jedoch, the aim here is not to tie the simulations to particular natural species, but rather to explore
the extreme behaviors with a view to understanding the general principles involved. To check the
relation between restricted procreation and restricted protection period, the evolutionary simulations
were repeated with procreation allowed irrespective of whether the individual concerned was being
protected. Allowing procreation from the first fitness comparison point (at age one year) results in
the protection period evolving quickly to be safely beyond the normal life span of the individuals, als
seen on the left of Figure 11. This means that all individuals are protected all their lives from deaths
due to competition, and only die because of old age. Natürlich, this ignores the fact that most
parents will not live long enough to protect their children for that long, but it does show the
underlying trend that is likely to persist in more realistic simulations. Rather than simply assuming

Figur 11. Evolution of the protection period for zero non-procreation period (links), and simultaneous evolution of the
protection and non-procreation periods (Rechts), if procreation while protected is allowed. In both cases the protection
period evolves to be more than the natural life span.

400

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

that someone will be around to protect each child for the evolved period, as has been done thus far,
future simulations will need to involve much larger populations, which will allow the consideration of
explicit families and the patterns of protection they can offer.

If the non-procreation period (oder, gleichwertig, the age of first reproduction) is also allowed to
evolve as an independent parameter, that period falls quickly, to finish before the first fitness com-
parison point at age one, and the protection period again rises to beyond the normal life span, als
seen on the right of Figure 11. What both the scenarios of Figure 11 reintroduce is the need to
compete at all ages to procreate, and this encourages faster learning again and the return of the
unwanted associated side effects, such as the evolution of risky learning strategies that sometimes
result in persistent very poor performance at all ages. This can be seen in the return of a shorter
learning-rate age-dependence time scale H and increased mean error rates at age 60, as shown in
Figur 12. If the need to compete to procreate is removed (d.h., fitness is not used to choose parents),
the protection period again rises to beyond the normal life span, leaving no fitness pressure at all in
the evolutionary process, and so no performance improvement takes place.

Es scheint, Dann, that there are individual performance advantages that emerge as a result of
preventing offspring from reproducing while being protected, but this feature does not evolve in the
current simulations. In these simulations, the tradeoff between reproductive opportunity and im-
proved adult performance favors early reproduction, even though allowing the protected children to
reproduce will compromise the parents’ own reproductive success rate. Interessant, a significant cor-
relation is found in higher primates between brain weight and age of first female reproduction [1],
which makes sense if delayed reproduction really can improve learning performance for complex
tasks. Natürlich, correlation does not imply causation, but such correlations are certainly consistent
with the various factors coevolving in these species to provide better overall performances. Clearly,
delayed reproduction fails to emerge in the current simulations because there are other important
factors that have not been included in them. Zum Beispiel, if the adult performance affects the
population size that can be supported by the environment, or affects some absolute ability of
individuals to survive (z.B., associated with crucial food gathering skills or competition with other
Spezies), then behaviors that increase that performance will be more likely to emerge in simulations
that include such factors. There is certainly much scope for future work to explore these issues.

8 Evolution with Abstracted Learning Processes

To render the above artificial life simulations as reliable as possible, the learning task was taken to be
something that is likely to be a component of real animal learning, and the learning process was

Figur 12. Comparison of the evolved learning-rate age-dependence time scales H (links) and mean error rates at age 60
(Rechts), for the case of procreation while protected (PWP) and for the earlier simulations. It is clear that the restricted
procreation is crucial for the improved performance.

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

401

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

implemented in the form of a traditional artificial neural network model. Such evolutionary neural
network simulations, Jedoch, are extremely computationally expensive, and it makes sense to con-
sider now the extent to which the various issues can be explored by abstracting out the key prop-
erties of the learning process, and evolving only the life history factors. The problem with attempting
this is that the error distributions and associated fitness levels during neural network learning depend
in a complex manner on the learning algorithm and its evolved parameters, and these depend in a
nontrivial way on the evolutionary pressures and population age distributions, which in turn are
affected by the protection periods that the study is attempting to determine. With so many un-
knowns, and the complexity of their interactions, it is not possible to predict reliably in advance what
distributions of all these things will emerge across the evolving populations. Jedoch, having run all
the above simulations, a good idea of the patterns that emerge is now available, and it becomes
feasible to abstract out the key issues and attempt to run the simulations again without the full neural
learning processes.

The simplest conceivable approximation to the full

learning process would be to have each
individual’s cost (d.h., abstracted classification error, or inverse fitness) fall linearly with increasing age
aus 100 down to 0. Figur 13 shows the results of evolving the protection period in this case, for a
range of learning rates y, with means and variances over 20 runs. The deterministic case, which has
the individual costs reduced by y each year down to zero, has similar results to a stochastic version in
which the reductions are drawn randomly from the range [0, 2j]. The population mean cost is
approximately linear in the expected learning time (ELT ) 100/y for both the deterministic and
stochastic versions. The evolved protection periods also start close to linear in 100/y, but level off
around 30 Jahre, the point at which the individuals start dying of old age. Below around 20 Jahre
there is a slight, but significant, increase (von 1 Zu 2 Jahre) in the protection periods for the stochastic
case over the deterministic case, which reflects the uncertainty in reaching perfect performance by the
expected time (100/j). Predictably, the best mean performance is achieved with very high learning
rates y, for which all individuals reach perfect performance before their first round of competition to
survive or procreate at the end of their first year. Folglich, if the learning rate y is evolved along
with the protection period, it quickly achieves very high levels, and the protection period takes on the
associated very low levels as indicated by Figure 13. Natürlich, with neural networks one cannot
keep on increasing the learning rate and expect the learning time to decrease with it. Eventually, bei
some task-dependent point, the approximation to true gradient descent breaks down, und das
learning performance deteriorates. As was observed in the evolving neural network simulations
über, evolution can be used to find the best values for the learning parameters, and having slower
learning with longer protection periods does provide a clear evolutionary advantage.

Figur 13. Emergent population mean costs (links) and protection periods (Rechts) when the individual costs (d.h.,
abstracted performance errors) decrease linearly with age at learning rate y. Deterministic and stochastic versions are
shown as a function of the expected learning time 100/y.

402

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

To provide a better approximation to the observed neural learning process, with faster learning
leading to riskier learning strategies that increasingly lead to persistent poor performance, one can
consider having the learning process stop at some random point in the performance range [0, 100]
with a probability Uy that increases linearly with the learning rate y and an associated parameter U.
The top two graphs of Figure 14 show how the stochastic version’s performance then depends on y
and 100/y for four representative values of U. The higher U is, the lower the value of y at which
significant deviations from the earlier U = 0 case arise. Eventually, in all U > 0 Fälle, the cost begins
to increase again with y until Uy = 1, at which point it begins to fall slowly along a U-independent
curve. The bottom-left graph shows that the relation between the protection period and 100/y is not
much affected by the size of U. The bottom-right graph shows how the cost varies with the
protection period for the different values of U.

The cost plots in Figure 14 show clear minima for each value of U, and successful evolutionary
processes are likely to result in the emergence of optimal learning rates y and protection periods that
follow similar trends. The top two graphs of Figure 15 show the ELTs 100/y and protection pe-
riods that actually emerge through evolution as a function of the parameter U. As U increases, Die
best possible learning time 100/y also increases, and the best protection period follows suit. Im
top-right graph of Figure 14 it can be seen that around U = 0.08 the global cost minimum shifts
from the local minimum at fairly high 100/y to the point at zero 100/y. The bottom-left graph of
Figur 15, showing cost against U, indicates that the evolutionary process gets trapped in the local
minima at the U = 0.1 Punkt, but for higher U the global optima are found, as is clear from the
sudden drops in the top two graphs of Figure 15. The bottom-right graph shows that the mean
evolved protection period is always slightly longer than the mean evolved ELT 100/y. Das ist ein

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Figur 14. Results when the linear individual cost improvement with age stops with probability Uy at a random cost in the
range [0,100], for U a {0, 0.02, 0.05, 0.1}: mean population cost as a function of learning rate y (top left) and as a function
of expected learning time 100/y (top right), protection period as a function of ELT 100/y (bottom left), and cost as a
function of protection period (bottom right). There are clear cost minima that vary with the value of U.

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

403

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 15. Evolutionary simulation results when the linear individual cost improvement with age stops with probability Uy
at a random cost in the range [0,100]: mean evolved expected learning time 100/y as a function of parameter U (top left),
protection period as a function of U (top right), population-mean cost as a function of U (bottom left), and protection
period as a function of 100/y (bottom right). The parameter U acts as an abstract measure of learning difficulty.

reflection of the fact that mutations always lead to distributions of learning times and protection
periods, and that the protection period should always be long enough to accommodate a reasonable
number of individuals that are slower than average. For the same reason, the protection periods for
the stochastic case are always slightly longer than those for the deterministic case, as previously
observed in Figure 13.

The parameter U is an abstract measure of learning difficulty; it can be regarded as an approx-
imate representation of the difficulty a neural network learning algorithm has with its given task. Es ist
certainly a rough approximation, but it does have the required properties. Easy tasks will correspond
to low U, be learned quickly, and have short protection periods. Harder, or more complex, tasks
will correspond to higher values of U, take longer to learn, and benefit from longer protection
periods. The individual performance levels that emerge in the abstracted learning models of Fig-
ures 14 Und 15 can be compared directly with those from the full evolutionary neural network
simulations of Figures 5, 6, Und 7. Figur 16 shows the mean, median, upper quartile, and lower
quartile costs as a function of age for the abstracted processes with U = 0.04. In this case, der Mittelwert
evolved protection period is around 14 Jahre, and the mean evolved ELT 100/y is around 10 Jahre.
As in the full neural simulations, the results for the evolved protection period (Ev) are compared
with those for representative fixed protection periods (1, 10, 20). Clearly, the linear learning
approximation and flat distribution of residual errors are gross approximations of the real neural
learning processes, but the broad pattern of results is seen to be the same: Longer protection periods
allow slower learning and result in better adult performance, but not allowing procreation while
being protected prevents the evolved protection periods from becoming excessively long. The effects
of changing the onset of old age, and of allowing procreation while protected, are shown in Figure 17,

404

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

Figur 16. Mean, median, upper quartile, and lower quartile individual performance as a function of age for the abstracted
learning processes of Figures 14 Und 15 with U = 0.04. Each plot compares the results for the evolved protection period
(Ev) with those for representative fixed protection periods (1, 10, 20). These results are in broad agreement with the full
neural simulations of Figures 5, 6, Und 7.

and are also found to be in line with those of the full evolving neural network simulations presented
in Figures 10 Und 11.

9 Conclusions and Discussion

This artificial life study has shown how evolutionary neural network simulations can begin to explore
the effects of lifetime learning on life history evolution. It has established that clear learning

Figur 17. Further tests of how well the results on abstracted learning processes match those of the full neural
simulations. The effect of life span on protection period (links) for comparison with Figure 10, and evolved periods for the
case of procreation while protected (Rechts) for comparison with Figure 11, both show broad agreement.

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

405

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

advantages and better adult performance are possible if children receive longer periods of parental
protection, but only if the children are not allowed to reproduce during their period of protection. Wenn
procreation is not prevented while being protected, the competition to reproduce leads to learning
strategies that result in worse adult performance. When procreation is prevented during protection, A
compromise protection period evolves that balances the improved learning performance against the
reduced procreation period. It was also found that the evolved protection period increases with life
Spanne, rather than remaining at a fixed duration determined by the learning task complexity, illus-
trating the tradeoff involved and confirming the importance of learning well.

influence on one individual

in which the environmental

Applying these findings to the understanding of real animal populations is not straightforward.
Indirect genetic effects,
is affected by a
different individual (such as a parent ), are known to have important and sometimes nonintuitive
consequences for the associated evolutionary processes [37], and there are an enormous number of
potentially important, species-dependent factors that have been glossed over in this study. Das Konzept
of protection period studied here is certainly a considerably simplified form of the altruistic behaviors
known to be common in nature [29]. Darüber hinaus, the relative importance of the complexities of social
life versus diet and food gathering for the coevolution of brain size, learning, and intelligence remains
controversial [21]. Both of these factors are clearly important for primates in general, and humans in
besondere. Jedoch, the simulations presented in this article bypass the distinction, showing that any
need for learning complex behavior will result in associated changes to the optimal periods of
protection and ages of first reproduction. The physical and biological practicalities associated with the
coevolution of the various factors will certainly bias what emerges in biological populations, depending
upon the characteristics of the particular species involved. Folglich, mapping the results of the
simplified simulations onto particular species is fraught with difficulties. Trotzdem, this article has
explored the general principles of lifetime learning effects on life histories, which can be used in the
future as the basis for more detailed studies aligned to particular species.

A major problem with most artificial

life simulations is the extreme simplifications that are
required to render them computationally feasible. If the models are oversimplified, there is a risk that
they will become unreliable and misleading. Zum Beispiel, it was seen in this study how important the
inclusion of certain details was, such as the differences and improvements that emerge on allowing
the evolved neural learning rates to depend on age. To proceed in the face of computational intrac-
tability, one can run quite detailed simulations to establish the behavior of particular aspects and how
they interact with other aspects, and then attempt to formulate computationally inexpensive approx-
imations of them that may be included in future models that represent the other factors in more
detail. Determining how to do that reliably is a major challenge for the field of artificial life in general.
If different features of the system under investigation do not interact, then progress in this direction
should be straightforward, but usually the assumption of no interaction will be yet another approx-
imation that has to be accommodated.

It was in this spirit that the previous section of this article considered how the neural learning
Verfahren, which is at the heart of this study, might be approximated to give massive savings in
computation time that could then be used to allow other aspects to be modeled in more detail. It was
shown how this could be done in such a way that the key results that emerged were qualitatively
equivalent to those of the full evolving neural network study. Darüber hinaus, there was a simulation time
speedup factor on the order of 10,000. It was also seen how the abstract approximated learning
process could be parameterized in such a way as to represent learning tasks and processes of dif-
fering difficulty, and in that way the simulations could be regarded as extensions of the neural sim-
ulations that were tied to a particular simplified task and neural network. Further work would be able
to benefit from improvements to the parameterization of the abstract learning processes that might
represent known aspects of human/animal learning even better than the neural network models used
in this study. Full evolving neural network simulations are still likely to be required to ground the
whole process (to relate the abstract performance improvements to the age dependences of real
neural learning abilities), so they cannot be avoided completely. Zum Beispiel, it would be virtually
impossible to predict the effect of extended protection periods on all the evolved neural network

406

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

parameters and the resultant learning performance, without running at least one set of full simu-
Beziehungen. But once the key patterns of learning performance have been established and abstracted, Die
hope is that they can be avoided in future simulations. Checks are required, Natürlich. Zum Beispiel,
the correspondence of the abstracted learning results seen in Figure 17 with those in Figures 10 Und
11 is evidence that the chosen approximation is at least good enough to explore the effect of varying
the life span and allowing procreation while protected.

It seems likely then that future work in this area will, for reasons of computational feasibility, best
proceed by pursuing this abstracted learning approach further, with better matching of the learning
patterns to real tasks and species. The computational speedups over full neural network simulations
will then facilitate explorations over whole ranges of abstract learning parameter values, analogous to
the plots against y and U in Figures 14 Und 15. As noted at various stages in the above, there certainly
remain many related life history issues and refinements that could usefully be incorporated into
extensions of the study presented in this article.

The most obvious set of extensions are those that relate directly to the learning process, wie zum Beispiel
the changes to the learning experience that can result from parental protection — for example, fällig
to guided exploration, exploration without risk, mimicking of parental behavior, parental instruc-
tion, und so weiter. It will probably actually be easier to incorporate these factors into an abstracted
Lernprozess, so this should be a
learning process than it would be to adjust a full neural
particularly promising avenue to explore further. Then there are more realistic accounts of the costs
of reproduction and protection, how the protection abilities depend on the parental fitness and
number of children, and how these affect the patterns of competition and deaths. Ähnlich, Die
effect of numerous other biological and environmental details that influence the life span, age of
maturity, fecundity, and length of the reproductive and growing periods could be incorporated
into the models. Individual age-related factors, such as biological deterioration affecting the ability to
compete or reproduce at older ages, may also have important consequences under certain circum-
stances. There is also the need to pay more attention to the evolutionary pressures and consequences
that arise from the introduction of competition with, and coevolution with, other species. Zum Beispiel-
reichlich, the performance differences seen in Figure 12 are likely to have different consequences if the
competition is between species, rather than purely within one species. Parameterized representa-
tions of such additional factors will allow further explorations of the associated tradeoffs, better
understanding of how these factors interact with learning, and how different patterns arise for
different species. Various aspects of these extensions will hopefully be presented elsewhere in the
near future.

Danksagungen
Preliminary versions of many of the key ideas in this article were first presented at the 2007 Neuronal
Computation and Psychology Workshop (NCPW10) [12] und das 2007 Genetic and Evolutionary
Computation Conference (GECCO) [11].

Verweise
1. Allman, J., McLaughlin, T., & Hakeem, A. (1993). Brain weight and life-span in primate species.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 90, 118 – 122.

2. Bailey, J. D., Bruer, B. T., Symons, F., & Lichtman, J. W. (Hrsg.) (2000). Critical thinking about critical

periods. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

3. Balduin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. The American Naturalist, 30, 441 – 451.

4. Bishop, C. M. (1995). Neural networks for pattern recognition. Oxford, Vereinigtes Königreich: Oxford University Press.

5. Bogin, B. (1997). Evolutionary hypotheses for human childhood. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40,

63 – 89.

6. Bullinaria, J. A. (2001). Simulating the evolution of modular neural systems. In Proceedings of the

Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (S. 146 – 151). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

407

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

7. Bullinaria, J. A. (2003). From biological models to the evolution of robot control systems. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society A, 361, 2145 – 2164.

8. Bullinaria, J. A. (2003). Evolving efficient learning algorithms for binary mappings. Neural Networks,

16, 793 – 800.

9. Bullinaria, J. A. (2005). Evolved age dependent plasticity improves neural network performance. In
Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Hybrid Intelligent Systems (HIS 2005) (S. 79 – 84).
Piscataway, NJ: IEEE.

10. Bullinaria, J. A. (2007). Using evolution to improve neural network learning: Pitfalls and solutions.

Neural Computing & Applications, 16, 209 – 226.

11. Bullinaria, J. A. (2007). The effect of learning on life history evolution. In Proceedings of the Genetic

and Evolutionary Computation Conference (GECCO 2007) (S. 222 – 229). New York: ACM.

12. Bullinaria, J. A. (2008). Another reason why we should look after our children. In R. M. French &
E. Thomas (Hrsg.), From associations to rules: Connectionist models of behavior and cognition (S. 41 – 52).
Singapur: World Scientific.

13. Charnov, E. L., & Berrigan, D. (1990). Dimensionless numbers and life history evolution: Age at

maturity versus the adult lifespan. Evolutionary Ecology, 4, 273 – 275.

14. Clutton-Brock, T. H., Albon, S. D., & Guinness F. E. (1989). Fitness costs of gestation and lactation in

wild mammals. Natur, 337, 260 – 262.

15. Clutton-Brock, T. H., Guinness, F. E., & Albon, S. D. (1983). The cost of reproduction to red deer

hinds. Journal of Animal Ecology, 52, 367 – 383.

16. Cole, L. C. (1954). The population consequences of life history phenomena. Quarterly Review of Biology,

29, 103 – 137.

17. Gadgil, M., & Bossert, W. (1970). Life history consequences of natural selection. American Naturalist,

104, 1 – 24.

18. Harvey, P. H., & Zammuto, R. M. (1985). Patterns of mortality and age at first reproduction in natural

populations of mammals. Natur, 315, 319 – 320.

19. Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H., & Charnov, E. L. (1998). Grandmothering,
menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
USA, 95, 1336 – 1339.

20. Julesz, B., & Kovacs, ICH. (Hrsg.) (1995). Maturational windows and adult cortical plasticity. Reading, MA:

Addison-Wesley.

21. Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution:

Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156 – 185.

22. Kennedy, G. E. (2005). From the ape’s dilemma to the weanling’s dilemma: Early weaning in its

evolutionary context. Journal of Human Evolution, 48, 123 – 145.

23. Lamb, M. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Teti, D. M. (2002). Development in infancy: An introduction. Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

24. Leigh, S. R. (2004). Brain growth, life history and cognition in primate and human evolution. amerikanisch

Journal of Primatology, 62, 139 – 164.

25. Leutenegger, W. (1979). Evolution of litter size in primates. American Naturalist, 114, 525 – 531.

26. Marrow, P., Stevenson, ICH. R., McNamara, J. M., Houston, A. ICH., & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1996).

State-dependent life history evolution in Soay sheep: Dynamic modelling of reproductive scheduling.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 351, 17 – 32.

27. Roff, D. A. (2002). Life history evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

28. Sloman, A., & Chappell, J. (2005). The altricial-precocial spectrum for robots. In Proceedings of the International

Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 1187 – 1193.

29. Sober, E., & Wison, D. S. (1999). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Boston, MA:

Harvard University Press.

30. Stearns, S. C. (1989). Trade-offs in life history evolution. Functional Ecology, 3, 259 – 268.

31. Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford, Vereinigtes Königreich: Oxford University Press.

408

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

J. A. Bullinaria

Lifetime Learning as a Factor in Life History Evolution

32. Stearns, S. C., & Crandall, R. E. (1981). Quantitative predictions of delayed maturity. Evolution,

35, 455 – 463.

33. Stearns, S. C., & Koella, J. (1986). The evolution of phenotypic plasticity in life-history traits:
Predictions for norms of reaction for age- and size-at-maturity. Evolution, 40, 893 – 913.

34. Striedter, G. F. (2005). Principles of brain evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

35. Warner, R. R. (1984). Deferred reproduction as a response to sexual selection in coral reef fish: A test

of the life historical consequences. Evolution, 38, 148 – 162.

36. Weber, B. H., & Depew, D. J. (2003). Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered. Cambridge, MA:

MIT Press.

37. Wolf, J. B., Brodie, E. D., Cheverud, J. M., Moore, A. J., & Wade, M. J. (1998). Evolutionary

consequences of indirect genetic effects. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 13, 64 – 69.

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Artificial Life Volume 15, Nummer 4

409

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u
A
R
T
l
/

/

l

A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

F
/

/

/

/

1
5
4
3
8
9
1
6
6
2
6
1
1
A
R
T
l
.

/

.

2
0
0
9
1
5
3
B
u

.

.

l
l
ich

.

N
A
R
ich
A
0
1
0
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3Lifetime Learning as a Factor in image
Lifetime Learning as a Factor in image

PDF Herunterladen