It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of

It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of
Sensory Distractor Processing Induced by
Late Response Conflict

Bernhard Pastötter and Christian Frings

Abstrakt

■ Understanding the neural processes that maintain goal-
directed behavior is a major challenge for the study of attentional
Kontrolle. Although much of the previous work on the issue has
focused on prefrontal brain areas, little is known about the
contribution of sensory brain processes to the regulation of atten-
tional control. The present EEG study examined brain oscillatory
activities invoked in the processing of response conflict in a later-
alized Eriksen single-flanker task, in which target letters were
presented at fixation and single distractor letters were presented
either left or right to the targets. Distractors were response
compatible, response incompatible, or neutral in relation to the
responses associated with the targets. The behavioral results
showed that responses to targets in incompatible trials were
slower and more error prone than responses in compatible trials.
The electrophysiological results revealed an early sensory lateral-

ization effect in (both evoked and induced) theta power (3–6 Hz)
that was more pronounced in incompatible than compatible
Versuche. The sensory lateralization effect preceded in time a mid-
frontal conflict effect that was indexed by an increase of (induced)
theta power (6–9 Hz) in incompatible compared with compatible
Versuche. The findings indicate an early modulation of sensory dis-
tractor processing induced by response conflict. Theoretical im-
plications of the findings, in particular with respect to the theory
of event coding and theories relating to stimulus–response bind-
ing [Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner,
A. Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends im kognitiven Bereich
Wissenschaften, 18, 376–384, 2014; Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben,
G., & Prinz, W. The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework
for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain
Wissenschaften, 24, 849–878, 2001], are discussed. ■

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

EINFÜHRUNG

Our brain is constantly bombarded with information from
the different senses while at the same time only a small
subset of the current information is actually relevant for
our goals. To make this situation even worse, humans
typically have several goals activated at a time (z.B., über-
arching goals like “avoiding danger” that should be
always active and very specific goals like “searching for
your keys”). The different stimulus information must be
matched to our current goals to promote goal-directed
behavior. Obviously, there might be conflict between
the processing of currently relevant and, for the task at
Hand, irrelevant information, which can arise either at
the perceptual level or the level of response selection.
Stimulus conflict arises at the perceptual level when the
relevant information is perceptually different from the
irrelevant information, whereas response conflict arises
at the level of response selection when distracting stimuli
suggest interfering actions. Although it is clear that human
behavior outside the laboratory is quite complex—as we
can juggle several goals with different priorities at a time
with ease and evaluate and process stimuli accordingly—it

University of Trier

© 2018 Massachusetts Institute of Technology

is nevertheless important to pinpoint aspects of this
complex behavior and investigate the tools with which
humans achieve efficient behavior. Controlling stimulus
and response conflict is one process with which humans
Akt, although it is clear that there is much more to action
regulation than just controlling stimulus information (z.B.,
behavioral routines due to binding, self-monitoring, tasks
sets, attentional and intentional weighting, prioritization
due to self-relevance, und so weiter).

Entsprechend, research in the cognitive sciences has
developed several experimental paradigms to pinpoint
the control processes with which humans can handle
these conflicts and successfully achieve goal-directed
behavior. One task in particular, the Eriksen flanker task,
is well suited to independently measure conflicts at the
level of perception and the level of response selection
(Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; see Eriksen, 1995, für eine Rezension).
In the visual variant of this task, flanker or distractor stim-
uli are presented adjacent to a target stimulus. Stimulus
conflict is measured by comparing trials in which the
flankers are identical to the target (d.h., they are stimulus
congruent and response compatible) with trials in which
the flankers are perceptually different from the target
but still suggest the same response (d.h., they are stimulus
incongruent but response compatible). Im Gegensatz,

Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften 30:7, S. 985–998
doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01267

response conflict is measured by comparing trials in
which the flankers are perceptually different from the
targets but suggest the same response (d.h., they are
stimulus incongruent and response compatible) Und
trials in which the flankers are also perceptually different
from the target but, in addition, suggest a different re-
sponse (d.h., they are stimulus incongruent and response
incompatible). Typically, both stimulus and response
conflict lead to cost effects in the Eriksen flanker, indexed
by slower RTs and more errors in conflict than non-
conflict trials. The costs are typically explained by atten-
tional control processes. One idea is that the internal
representation of the flanker stimuli may be inhibited
or actively suppressed at the perceptual level and also
at the level of response selection, yet inhibition of inter-
fering information needs attentional resources, welche
leads to the observed cost effects (z.B., Frings, Wentura, &
Wühr, 2012; Lamers & Roelofs, 2011; Wühr & Frings, 2008;
Heil, Osman, Wiegelmann, Rolke, & Hennighausen, 2000;
Natürlich, there are other interpretations of the flanker
effect in terms of response biases or response competition
or in terms of an attentional gradient, z.B., White, Ratcliff,
& Starns, 2011). The effects are not restricted to visual
Reize. Both stimulus and response conflicts in the flanker
Aufgabe (or variants thereof ) have been well documented in
the literature to arise across the senses, in vision (Eriksen
& Eriksen, 1974), in audition (Chan, Merrifield, & Spence,
2005), in touch (Evans & Craig, 1992), and even cross-
modally (Frings & Spence, 2010).

Understanding the neural processes that maintain
goal-directed behavior is a major challenge for the study
of attentional control. Much of the previous work on the
issue has focused on prefrontal brain areas, insbesondere
the ACC and associated regions of the medial frontal wall,
which are believed to play a vital role in the detection of
conflict and the regulation of attentional control. It is a
prominent idea that ACC functions as a conflict monitor
that detects conflicts between concurrently activated rel-
evant and irrelevant information (Botvinick, Cohen, &
Fuhrmann, 2004; Van Veen & Fuhrmann, 2002A). Corresponding
evidence arose from fMRI studies showing that ACC is
more activated on conflict than nonconflict trials in
conflict tasks, including the Stroop task (MacDonald,
Cohen, Stenger, & Fuhrmann, 2000), the Simon task (Peterson
et al., 2002), and the Eriksen flanker task (Botvinick,
Nystrom, Fissell, Fuhrmann, & Cohen, 1999). Zusätzlich,
EEG studies have demonstrated that conflicting stimuli or
responses can elicit larger negative deflections in early ERP
components (z.B., N200 and N400) over midfrontal elec-
trodes, likely originating from ACC (z.B., Appelbaum,
Schmied, Boehler, Chen, & Woldorff, 2011; Hanslmayr et al.,
2008; Van Veen & Fuhrmann, 2002B; Liotti, Woldorff, Perez, &
Mayberg, 2000). Consistently, both EEG and MEG studies
on brain oscillatory activity have shown that midfrontal
theta oscillations (4–8 Hz) typically show a power increase
in conflict compared with nonconflict trials, an effect that
has been source-localized to ACC (z.B., Pastötter, Berchtold,

& Bäuml, 2012; Nigbur, Ivanova, & Stürmer, 2011; Pastötter,
Hanslmayr, & Bäuml, 2010; Hanslmayr et al., 2008; sehen
Cavanagh & Frank, 2014, für eine Rezension). Thereby, es hat
been shown that mainly induced (d.h., not phase-locked)
but less evoked (d.h., phase-locked) theta oscillations are
related to the processing of (response) conflict (Cohen &
Donner, 2013; Cavanagh, Zambrano-Vazquez, & Allen,
2012). Although there is evidence that ACC responds to
both stimulus and response conflict, ACC conflict effects
seem to be strongest for conflict arising at the level of
response selection (Nigbur, Cohen, Ridderinkhof, &
Stürmer, 2012; Van Veen, Cohen, Botvinick, Stenger, &
Fuhrmann, 2001).

In addition to ACC, other brain regions are involved in
conflict processing and attentional control, einschließlich der
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal
gyrus, posterior parietal cortex, anterior insula, and visual
Kortex, with the different structures being differentially
related to stimulus and response conflict (see Nee, Wager,
& Jonides, 2007, für eine Rezension). Wichtig, both fMRI and
EEG studies employing the flanker task (or variants thereof )
have shown that early sensory distractor processing in visual
brain areas can be modulated by conflicting stimuli (Marini,
Demeter, Roberts, Chelazzi, & Woldorff, 2016; Donohue,
Todisco, & Woldorff, 2013; Appelbaum et al., 2011; Kelley
& Lavie, 2011). In the study by Appelbaum et al. (2011),
stimulus and response conflict were varied nonorthog-
onally. Occipital brain activity was contrasted between
conditions with conflicting and (partially) nonconflicting
flankers in a lateralized version of the visual Eriksen
flanker task, in which the distractors were presented left
and right to the targets. In no-conflict trials, all distractors
were nonconflicting (stimulus congruent and response
compatible); in conflict trials, all distractors were conflict-
ing (stimulus incongruent and response incompatible). In
a third condition, the distractors were conflicting in one
hemifield but nonconflicting in the other hemifield. In
these partial-conflict trials, the results showed a sensory lat-
eralization effect over occipital electrode sites, with more
positive ERPs contralateral than ipsilateral to the conflicting
distractor side, that emerged simultaneously in time with a
midfrontal N200 conflict effect. The sensory laterali-
zation effect was suggested to indicate attentional dis-
traction caused by the conflicting flankers. The findings
by Appelbaum et al. (2011), together with the findings
from other fMRI and EEG work (z.B., Marini et al., 2016;
Donohue et al., 2013; Kelley & Lavie, 2011), support the
view that occipital brain activity in general and sensory
lateralization in particular are modulated by stimulus
conflict arising at the perceptual level. Jedoch, Weil
of the nonorthogonal manipulation of stimulus and re-
sponse conflict in the earlier work, grundsätzlich, both stim-
ulus and response conflict could have contributed to the
modulation of the sensory lateralization effect.

This study examined whether (lateralized) Hinterhaupt
brain activity is modulated by response conflict arising at
the level of response selection; das ist, we propose the

986

Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften

Volumen 30, Nummer 7

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

counterintuitive idea that response conflict arising at later
processing stages can influence early sensory processing.
Noch, with respect to the literature on action control in the
last decade, one can make a clear argument for this idea. In
besondere, it has been established that the links between
perception and action are much closer and more inter-
twined than previously assumed. A theory that has been
widely used to investigate action control is the theory of
event coding (TEC; Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, &
Prinz, 2001), which provides a framework for understand-
ing the linkages between perception and action. Based on
the ideomotor principle (see Shin, Proctor, & Capaldi,
2010; Stock & Stock, 2004, for reviews), TEC assumes
that actions are represented by codes of their perceptual
consequences and further that perceived events and
produced actions may share the same representations
(Hommel, 2004, 2009). TEC has been used to explain a
range of phenomena, including stimulus–response (S-R)
effects in general and priming effects in particular (for re-
Ansichten, see Henson, Eckstein, Waszak, Frings, & Horner,
2014; Henson, 2009). For our purpose here, it is relevant
that TEC assumes that, even after a single pairing of a
stimulus and a response, the S-R episode is bound into a
Erinnerung (or event file) and upon repetition of any part
of this episode is again reinstated. In der Tat, dazugehörigen
evidence arose from fMRI and EEG studies, showing that
the repetition of a stimulus directly retrieves the response
that was previously associated to it, thereby suggesting
that—after an initial pairing—reencountering a stimulus
makes the previous response instantly accessible (Horner
& Henson, 2012; Dobbins, Schnyer, Verfaellie, & Schacter,
2004).

Concerning our study, we therefore assume that, in einem
typical response conflict task with fixed S-R mappings,
reencountering a stimulus after a few trials directly re-
trieves the particular response. Given that research on
action control yields ample evidence that also flanker
stimuli can directly activate responses due to S-R bindings
(z.B., Frings & Rothermund, 2011; Frings, Rothermund, &
Wentura, 2007; Mayr & Buchner, 2006), we hypothesized
that reencountering a flanker might instantly activate the
associated response, and because of S-R bindings, inter-
ference due to response conflict might influence the
processing of flankers at very early levels of sensory infor-
mation processing. Natürlich, such view is in contrast to
established sequential information processing models
from research on the psychological refractory period (sehen
Pashler, 1994).

This study examined the behavioral and electrophysio-
logical effects of response conflict in a lateralized single-
flanker version of the Eriksen task. This version was
chosen because previous imaging work has shown that
it can produce reliable distractor competition effects in
early visual cortex (Kelley & Lavie, 2011). Targets were
presented at central fixation, and single flanking dis-
tractors were presented either left or right to the targets.
Distractors were always stimulus incongruent, das ist,

perceptually different from the targets. Response conflict
effects were examined by comparing behavioral and EEG
data between response-incompatible (INC), response-
compatible (COM), and neutral (NEU) Bedingungen. Regard-
ing the electrophysiological effects of response conflict,
oscillatory brain activities and lateralized readiness poten-
tials (LRPs) were examined. With regard to LRPs, onset
times of stimulus-locked and response-locked potentials
(S-LRP and LRP-R, jeweils) were analyzed as chrono-
metric markers for the duration of premotor (S-LRP) Und
motor processing stages (LRP-R), jeweils (see Eder,
Leuthold, Rothermund, & Schweinberger, 2012). Mit
regard to oscillatory activities, both stimulus-related and
response-related power changes across EEG frequencies
aus 1 Zu 30 Hz were computed and compared between
experimental conditions. Both evoked and induced parts
of the oscillatory signal were calculated. Evoked oscilla-
tions are phase-locked to stimulus or response onset (als
is the ERP), whereas induced oscillations are not (sehen
Tallon-Baudry & Bertrand, 1999). Both nonlateralized
and lateralized (to distractor presentation side) effects of
response conflict were examined.

Several expectations arose. Behaviorally, response con-
flict effects were expected for both RT and response accu-
racy, reflected in slower and more error-prone responses
on incompatible than compatible (and neutral) Versuche. Bei
the electrophysiological level, with respect to LRP onsets,
we expected a response conflict effect on S-LRP onset,
due to later S-LRP onset on incompatible than compatible
and neutral trials, but no conflict effect on LRP-R onset
(z.B., Kopp, Rist, & Mattler, 1996; Gratton, Coles, Sirevaag,
Eriksen, & Donchin, 1988). With regard to nonlateralized
EEG activity, we expected an increase of induced (Aber
not evoked) theta power over midfrontal electrodes on
incompatible compared with compatible (and neutral)
Versuche (z.B., Cohen & Donner, 2013; Cavanagh et al.,
2012). Most important, with regard to lateralized EEG activ-
ität, we expected a modulation of sensory distractor pro-
cessing induced by response conflict. The hypothesis was
based on the finding that sensory distractor processing can
be modulated by the processing of conflicting information
(z.B., Appelbaum et al., 2011) and current theories on ac-
tion control and S-R binding, which suggest a close link
between perception and action, and the processing of
stimulus and response conflict, jeweils (Henson et al.,
2014; Hommel, 2004, 2009; Hommel et al., 2001). Weil
mainly low-frequency oscillations in the theta (around 5 Hz)
and alpha band (around 10 Hz) have been linked to latera-
lized control of visuospatial attention (theta: z.B., Van der
Lubbe, Bundt, & Abrahamse, 2014; Grün & McDonald,
2008; alpha: z.B., Thut, Nietzel, Brandt, & Pascual-Leone,
2006; Worden, Foxe, Wang, & Simpson, 2000), wir erwartet
to find a sensory lateralization effect in theta or alpha
power over occipital electrode sites. As argued above,
theories from action control research would predict that
the flanker stimuli directly activate the associated responses
in parallel with the sensory processing. Daher, interference

Pastötter and Frings

987

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

due to response conflict might already impact visual
processing of the irrelevant distractors, leading to enhanced
sensory lateralization during the processing of response
incompatible distractors.

METHODEN

Teilnehmer

Thirty-four students from the University of Trier, Deutschland,
were included in the study (29 Frauen, 6 left-handed,
Durchschnittsalter = 20.7 Jahre, SD = 2.2 Jahre). Six additional
participants were tested but eliminated from analysis
because of excess physiological noise in the EEG data (vier
Teilnehmer) or not following task instructions (two par-
ticipants). All participants reported normal or corrected-
to-normal vision; no participant reported any history of
neurological disease. All participants gave written informed
consent before examination and received course credit for
participation. The study was conducted in accordance
with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local
ethics review committee at the University of Trier.

Stimuli and Task

Participants performed a lateralized Eriksen single-flanker
Aufgabe, in which target letters were presented at central fix-
ation and flanking distractor letters were presented either
left or right to the targets. In jedem Versuch, only a single dis-
tractor letter was shown. Three experimental conditions
were included in this study (Figur 1). In all conditions,
target stimuli were the letters D, F, J, and K. In the COM

Figur 1. Experimental task. Participants performed a lateralized
Eriksen single-flanker task, in which target letters were presented at
central fixation and flanking distractor letters were presented at either
left or right to the targets. Participants responded to target letters
with their left and right index fingers. In the COM condition, targets and
distractors were mapped to the same response, whereas in the INC
condition, targets and distractors were mapped to different responses.
Zusätzlich, an NEU condition with two neutral-trial types was included,
in which the distractors were not mapped to any responses. Letters
were used as distractors in neutral-letter trials; a box was presented
as distractor in neutral-box trials.

and INC conditions, distractor stimuli were also the letters
D, F, J, and K. The letters “D” and “F” were mapped to a
left-hand response, the letters “J” and “K” were mapped
to a right-hand response. In the COM condition, the dis-
tractors were response compatible to the targets, das ist,
targets and distractors were mapped to the same response
(z.B., target “D” and distractor “F”). Wichtig, distractors
never matched the targets, das ist, distractors were always
stimulus incongruent to the targets at the perceptual level.
In the INC condition, distractors were response incompat-
ible to the targets, das ist, targets and distractors were
mapped to different responses (z.B., target “D” and distrac-
tor “K”). The assignment of the single distractor letters to
the target letters was chosen to be one-to-one and kept
constant for all participants (“D”–“K” and “J”–“F”), als es
naturally was in the COM condition (“D”–“F” and
“J”–“K”). Response (In)compatibility effects were exam-
ined by comparing data between compatible and incom-
patible trials in the COM and INC conditions, jeweils.
Zusätzlich, a NEU condition with two neutral trial types
was included in the study. In neutral-letter trials, the dis-
tractors were the letters “S,” “G,” “L,” and “H,” which were
not mapped to any response; the assignment of the single
distractor letters to the target letters was chosen to be
one-to-one and kept constant for all participants
(“D”–“‘S,” “F”–“G,” “J”–“L,” and “K”–“H”). In neutral-box
Versuche, a box was presented as distractor (); the box
was not mapped to any response.

All stimuli were shown in white on black background,
at viewing distance of 65 cm. Stimuli were approximately
1.2° in size. Letters were written in Arial font. Targets
were presented in the center of the screen. Distractors
were centered 3.5° to the left or right of the targets. Sei-
tween displays, a fixation cross was shown in the center
of the screen, which was 0.5° in size. Two response but-
tons were marked on a Chronos response box (Psychol-
ogy Software Tools). Participants were instructed to place
their left index finger on the left button and their right
index finger on the right button of the Chronos response
box (siehe Abbildung 1). They were asked to press the left but-
ton in response to the target letters “D” and “F,” and the
right button in response to the target letters “J” and “K.”
Participants were asked to respond as quickly and accu-
rately as possible to the targets and to ignore the flankers.
They were asked to maintain fixation on the center of the
screen at all times and to not move the eyes.

An experimental session consisted of 320 single trials,
presented in 10 blocks of 32 trials each. Within each
block, all predefined combinations of target and distractor
Reize, with the same number of left- and right-hemifield
distractor presentations, were realized in a counter-
balanced manner. Order of compatible, incompatible,
neutral-letter, and neutral-box trials was randomized with
the constraints that no trial type, target letter, or distractor
hemifield was repeated more than three trials in a row.
Each single trial began with a fixation cross, which was
shown in the center of the screen for an interval of variable

988

Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften

Volumen 30, Nummer 7

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

duration of 2–2.5 sec. If a participant did not fixate the
cross at the end of this interval, the duration of the inter-
val was prolonged until fixation. Eye gaze was controlled
by simultaneous eye tracking (Eyegaze System, LC Tech-
nologies; calibration was done before the experiment).
Maximum permissible gaze deviation was set to 1.75°
radius from the fixation cross. After presentation of the
fixation cross, the target letter was shown together with
one single distractor letter. The target was shown in the
middle of the screen, and the distractor was shown either
left or right to the target. Target and distractor stimuli re-
mained on the screen until a response was made by the
participant. No feedback was provided. After the re-
sponse, the next single trial started with presentation of
the fixation cross. To familiarize participants with the
procedure, they were given one block of 32 practice trials
before the experiment. Presentation and recording of
behavioral responses was done with E-Prime software
(v2.0, Psychology Software Tools).

Analysis of Behavioral Data

Both mean RT and error rate were analyzed. For each
participant, trials with RT greater or less than 2.5 standard
deviations from the individual mean RT were excluded
from further analysis. For RT analysis, only trials were in-
cluded for which responses on both the current trial and
the previous trial were correct. Mean numbers of trials
included in the final analysis were 74.9 compatible trials
(SD = 4.1, Min = 65, Max = 80), 74.3 incompatible trials (SD =
4.4, Min = 61, Max = 80), 75.0 neutral-letter trials (SD = 3.5,
Min = 63, Max = 80), Und 74.7 neutral-box trials (SD = 4.0,
Min = 66, Max = 80).

Behavioral data were analyzed with repeated-measures
ANOVAs with the factor of Experimental condition (COM,
INC, NEU). Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied
where appropriate. Post hoc comparisons were conducted
using Tukey honestly significant difference (HSD) testing,
which accounts for the problem of multiple testing and
maintains the significance threshold at 5% (HSD.05). Notiz
Das, a priori, the behavioral (and EEG) data in the NEU
condition were contrasted between neutral-letter and
neutral-box trials. As it turned out, there were no significant
differences between the two neutral trial types, neither in
the behavioral data nor in the EEG data. daher, Die
present results were based on a single NEU condition,
including both neutral-letter and neutral-box trials.

Recording of EEG Data

Electrophysiological data were recorded from 65 Ag/AgCl
Elektroden, which were positioned according to the 10–
10 electrode system with reference to FCz (EC80, Montage
NEIN. 1, Easycap). The ground was placed at location AFz.
The EOG was recorded from four bipolar channels, posi-
tioned on the inferior and superior regions of the left eye
and the outer canthi of both eyes, to monitor the vertical

and horizontal EOG. Electrode-skin impedance was kept
below 5 kΩ for all electrodes. Signals were digitalized with
a sampling rate of 500 Hz and amplified between 0.016
Und 250 Hz (BrainAmp, BrainVision Recorder, v1.20, Gehirn
Products).

Preprocessing of EEG Data

EEG recordings were re-referenced offline against aver-
age reference and EOG corrected by using calibration
data and generating individual EOG artifact coefficients,
as implemented in BESA Research (v6.1, BESA Software;
see Ille, Berg, & Scherg, 2002). Remaining artifacts were
marked by careful visual inspection. EEG data were seg-
mented into epochs ranging from −2.5 to 2.5 sec around
the onsets of stimuli and responses. To avoid filter arti-
facts at the edges of the segments, further analyses were
restricted to intervals ranging from −1.5 to 1.5 sec around
stimulus and response onsets. Segments containing arti-
facts and segments with response errors either on the cur-
rent or the previous trial were discarded from further
Analyse.

LRP Analysis

The EEG data were averaged time-locked to stimulus on-
set for S-LRP analysis and response onset for LRP-R anal-
ysis. For each participant and experimental condition, Die
ERP at the recording site over the motor cortex ipsilateral
to the response hand was subtracted from the ERP at the
homologous contralateral recording site (C4 minus C3
for left-hand responses, C3 minus C4 for right-hand re-
sponses). The LRP was calculated by averaging the result-
ing difference waveforms across hands, separately for
Bedingungen. Negative deviations of the resulting LRP indi-
cate stronger activation of the correct response hand
than of the incorrect response hand at the level of the
motor activation.

LRP onsets were measured after low-pass filtering (5 Hz,
12 dB/octave) and analyzed by applying the jackknife-
based procedure (Müller, Patterson, & Ulrich, 1998). Der
S-LRP was aligned to a 0.25-sec baseline before stimulus
onset; the LRP-R was aligned to a 0.25-sec baseline interval
starting at 1.25 sec before response onset (note that the
results and conclusions were the same with an LRP-R base-
line that started 0.55 sec before response onset; see Eder
et al., 2012). S-LRP onset was determined at the point in
time when the LRP amplitude reached 50% of the LRP peak
Amplitude, whereas LRP-R onset was calculated with a
fraction of 30% as suggested by Miller et al. (1998). LRP
onset latency measures were submitted to ANOVAs with
F values corrected to Fcorr = F/(n − 1)2, where Fcorr
denotes the corrected F value and n denotes the number
of participants (see Ulrich & Müller, 2001). Post hoc compar-
isons between conditions were calculated using jackknife-
based tcorr statistics (see Miller et al., 1998).

Pastötter and Frings

989

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Spectral EEG Analysis
The EEG data were transformed into the time–frequency
domain using a complex demodulation algorithm, welche
is implemented in BESA Research (v6.1; see Hoechstetter
et al., 2004). The algorithm consists of a multiplication of
the time domain signal with a complex periodic exponen-
tial function, having a frequency equal to the frequency
under analysis, and subsequent low-pass filtering. Der
low-pass filter is a finite impulse response filter of Gaussian
shape in the time domain, which is related to the envelope
of the moving window in wavelet analysis. The data were
filtered in a frequency range from 1 Zu 30 Hz. Time resolu-
tion was set to 78.8 ms (FWHM), and frequency resolu-
tion was set to 1.42 Hz (FWHM). Time–frequency data
were exported in bins of 50 msec and 1 Hz.

Both stimulus- and response-locked power changes
were calculated, time-locked to stimulus or response on-
set, jeweils. Stimulus- and response-locked changes
in power were determined by calculating the temporal-
spectral evolution, das ist, power changes for all time–
frequency points with power increases or decreases at
time point t and frequency f related to mean power at fre-
quency f over a preceding baseline interval (Pfurtscheller
& Aranibar, 1977). Stimulus-locked power changes were
determined in relation to a prestimulus baseline interval
that was set from −0.25 to 0 sec time-locked to stimulus
onset, whereas response-locked power changes were de-
termined in relation to a baseline interval that was set
from −1.25 to −1 sec time-locked to response onset. In
addition to total power, both induced and evoked power
changes were estimated. Evoked power changes were
estimated by calculating the temporal-spectral evolution
of the ERP, separately for each condition, electrode, Und
participant. Induced power changes were estimated by
calculating the temporal-spectral evolution of the EEG
signal after subtracting the ERP on each trial, separately
for each condition, electrode, and participant (Kalcher &
Pfurtscheller, 1995).

The following steps were taken to examine the effects
of response (In)compatibility between the COM and INC
Bedingungen. Erste, the topography of response conflict
effects in total power around response onset (−0.5 to
0.5 Sek, response-locked) between compatible and in-
compatible trials was examined with permutation-based
cluster analysis, which controls for the problem of multiple
testing (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007) using BESA Statistics
(v2.0, BESA Software). Speziell, paired t tests for all
data points (21 [time bins] × 30 [frequency bins] × 65 [elektr-
trodes]) were calculated and 3-D clusters of contiguous
data points that fell below a p value of .01 in the single
t tests were derived. For each empirical cluster, the sum
of t values of the single significant data points was kept
as a test statistic. Nächste, 5000 random permutations were
run in which the sum test statistic was repeatedly calcu-
lated for randomly shuffled data sets, with the data ran-
domly reordered across COM and INC conditions and

the permutation-based cluster with the highest sum of
t values was kept. By these means, a null distribution was
created from the random permutation runs, and the crit-
ical pcrit values for the empirically derived clusters were
berechnet. Empirical clusters with a critical pcrit value
below .05 went into further analysis. For those clusters,
mean power changes were calculated for total, evoked,
and induced power, averaged across data points of the
cluster’s maximum time range, maximum frequency
range, and corresponding electrodes, separately for each
experimental condition. Time courses of total power
changes in a cluster were compared between the three
experimental conditions (COM, INC, NEU); time courses
of evoked and induced power change in a cluster were
compared between the COM and INC conditions. Power
changes were time-locked to stimulus and response on-
sets in different analyses. Repeated-measures ANOVAs
that examined the interactions of Experimental condi-
tionen, Time, and Activity (evoked vs. induced) were cal-
culated. In all ANOVAs, Greenhouse–Geisser correction
was applied where appropriate, and Tukey HSD.05 War
used for post hoc testing.

Permutation-based cluster analysis was also used to ex-
amine the topography of (sensory) lateralization effects in
total power changes related to stimulus onset (0–0.5 sec)
between trials with left-hemifield distractor presentation
and right-hemifield distractor presentation, independent
of experimental condition (13 [time bins] × 30 [frequency
Mülleimer] × 65 [Elektroden]). In the permutation analysis, Die
data were randomly reordered across left-hemifield and
right-hemifield distractor presentations. Only clusters with
a critical pcrit value below .05 went into further analysis. Als
it turned out, two clusters over the left and right occipital
cortex did show very similar time–frequency characteris-
Tics, indicating a sensory lateralization effect over occipital
sites. daher, a sensory lateralization index (SLI) War
calculated by subtracting mean power changes of the
electrodes from the cluster ipsilateral to distractor presen-
tation from mean power changes of the electrodes from
the cluster contralateral to distractor presentation, aver-
aged across left- and right-hemifield distractor presenta-
tionen. SLIs were calculated for total, evoked, and induced
power changes, separately for conditions, and the time
courses of sensory lateralization effects were contrasted
time-locked to the onset of stimuli or responses. In all
ANOVAs, Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied
where appropriate. Tukey HSD.05 was used for post hoc
tests.

ERGEBNISSE

Verhaltensergebnisse

RTs

Mean RTs are depicted in Figure 2. A one-way repeated-
measures ANOVA with the factor of Experimental con-
dition (COM vs. INC vs. NEU) revealed a significant effect,

990

Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften

Volumen 30, Nummer 7

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

suggest a longer duration of premotor processes in
incompatible trials than in compatible and neutral trials.

LRP-R

Figur 3 also shows the LRP-R waveforms. A one-way
repeated-measures ANOVA on LRP-R onset with the factor
of Experimental condition (COM vs. INC vs. NEU) showed
no significant effect, Fcorr(2, 66) = 0.44, p = .645. LRP-R onset
was −192.8 msec in the COM condition, −186.8 msec in
the INC condition, and −197.8 msec in the NEU condition
(time-locked to response onset, 30% peak amplitude).
Daher, the LRP-R results suggest no difference in the dura-
tion of motor processes between experimental conditions.

Physiological Results: Power

Response Conflict
In the time–frequency analysis of response-locked con-
flict effects, the first step was to examine significant clus-
ters of differences in total power between the COM and
INC conditions. Permutation-based cluster analysis re-
vealed a single significant cluster over midfrontal elec-
trode sites (F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, and Cz),
which showed a larger response-locked theta power in-
crease (6–9 Hz) in the INC condition than in the COM
condition from −0.2 to 0.2 sec around response onset,
pcrit < .001 (Figure 4A). In the said time window, the cluster’s total theta power increase in the NEU condition (31.1%) was significantly smaller than in the INC condi- tion (45.1%), t(33) = 3.53, p = .001, but comparable with the COM condition (30.5%), t(33) < 1. Therefore, to in- crease statistical power in the analysis of conflict effects in the midfrontal cluster, subsequent analyses contrasted theta activities between the COM and INC conditions only. Time courses of total theta power changes in the three experimental conditions are depicted in Figure 4B. Figure 3. S-LRP and LRP-R for the three experimental conditions. S-LRP onset was generally later in the INC condition than in the other conditions; LRP-R onset did not differ between conditions (COM, INC, NEU). Pastötter and Frings 991 Figure 2. Behavioral results. Both mean RTs and error rates showed reliable response conflict effects, that is, slower and more error-prone responses on incompatible than compatible trials. In neutral trials, responding was reliably faster than in incompatible trials; other differences were not significant. Error bars represent within-subject standard errors (Cousineau, 2005). F(2, 66) = 9.38, p < .001. Post hoc testing indicated that the differences in RT between incompatible trials (600.1 msec) and compatible trials (586.3 msec) and be- tween incompatible and neutral trials (589.3 msec) were significant, whereas the difference between compatible and neutral trials was not significant (Tukey HSD.05 = 8.0 msec). Response Errors Error rates are also shown in Figure 2. A one-way repeated- measures ANOVA with the factor of Experimental condi- tion (COM vs. INC vs. NEU) revealed a significant effect, F(2, 66) = 3.44, p = .038. Post hoc testing showed that there was a significant difference in error rates between incompatible (3.32%) and compatible trials (2.29%); the other differences were not significant (neutral trials: 2.60%; Tukey HSD.05 = 0.97%). Physiological Results: LRPs S-LRP Figure 3 depicts the S-LRP waveforms for the three exper- imental conditions. A one-way repeated-measures ANOVA on S-LRP onset with the factor of Experimental condi- tion (COM vs. INC vs. NEU) revealed a significant effect, Fcorr(2, 66) = 3.68, p = .031. Indeed, S-LRP onset in the INC condition (364.5 msec; time-locked to stimulus onset, 50% peak amplitude) was significantly later than in the COM condition (330.1 msec), tcorr(33) = 2.05, p = .048, and the NEU condition (319.0 msec), tcorr(33) = 2.25, p = .031. The difference between the COM and NEU conditions was not significant, tcorr(33) < 1. Thus, the S-LRP results l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 0 7 9 8 5 1 7 8 7 5 8 3 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 2 6 7 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 4. Response conflict. (A) Topography of the response conflict effect in midfrontal theta power (difference between the INC and COM conditions). Electrodes in white depict the significant cluster in theta power (6–9 Hz; −0.2 to 0.2 sec around response onset). (B) Response-locked time course of midfrontal theta power changes in the significant cluster as a function of experimental condition (NEU). The red-shaded area indicates significant differences between incompatible and compatible trials, p < .05. (C, D) Response-locked and stimulus-locked time courses of midfrontal theta power changes in the significant cluster as a function of response conflict (COM vs. INC). Dotted lines refer to evoked theta power modulations; dashed lines refer to induced theta power modulations. The red-shaded areas indicate significant differences in induced theta power between compatible and incompatible trials, p < .05. Contrasting the time courses of theta activities be- tween the COM and INC conditions, a repeated-measures ANOVA with the factors of Condition (COM vs. INC) and Time (four 100-msec time intervals from −0.2 to 0.2 sec around response onset) revealed a significant main effect of Condition, F(1, 33) = 13.94, p < .001, and a significant main effect of Time, F(3, 99) = 12.67, p < .001, but no sig- nificant interaction between the two factors, F(3, 99) = 1.21, p = .309. These results suggest that the midfrontal conflict effect in total theta power was equally present before and after response execution. The contribution of evoked and induced theta activi- ties to the midfrontal conflict effect around response onset (−0.2 to 0.2 sec) was analyzed by calculating a repeated-measures ANOVA with the factors of Condition (COM vs. INC) and Activity (evoked vs. induced). The analysis showed a significant main effect of Condition, F(1, 33) = 13.76, p < .001, and a significant main effect of Activity, F(1, 33) = 11.70, p = .002, due to generally higher induced than evoked theta power. More im- portant, the analysis revealed a significant interaction between the two factors, F(1, 33) = 13.07, p < .001. Indeed, a significant conflict effect emerged for induced (COM: 23.0% vs. INC: 36.5%), but not for evoked theta power (7.5% vs. 8.6%; Tukey HSD.05 = 6.6%). Time courses of induced and evoked theta activities are depicted in Figure 4C. Figure 4D shows the results of the stimulus-locked analyses. Repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factors of Condition (COM vs. INC) and Time (seven 100-msec time intervals from 0 to 0.7 sec following stimulus onset) were calculated separately for induced and evoked theta power changes. Concerning induced theta power, the analysis showed a significant main effect of Condition, F(1, 33) = 4.60, p = .039, a significant main effect of Time, F(6, 198) = 10.64, p < .001, and also a significant interac- tion between the two factors, F(6, 198) = 6.71, p = .001. Indeed, reliable differences between the COM and INC conditions arose for the later time intervals from 500 to 700 msec only (means: 23.7% vs. 37.3%; Tukey HSD.05 = 9.55%). Regarding evoked theta power, there was a signif- icant main effect of time, F(6, 198) = 46.30, p < .001, but neither a significant main effect of Condition nor a sig- nificant interaction, both Fs < 1, suggesting that evoked theta power time-locked to stimulus onset was unaffected by response conflict.1 Sensory Lateralization Examination of lateralization effects in stimulus-locked power changes between trials with left- and right-hemifield distractor presentations revealed two significant clusters, one in the left hemisphere over occipital electrodes P5, P7, and PO7, pcrit = .015, and one in the right hemisphere 992 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 30, Number 7 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 0 7 9 8 5 1 7 8 7 5 8 3 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 2 6 7 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 over occipital electrodes P6, P8, and PO8, pcrit = .019 (Figure 5A). The two clusters showed concurrent theta power effects with similar time–frequency characteristics (3–6 Hz; minimum of 0.05 to maximum of 0.25 sec after stimulus onset), thus indicating a sensory lateralization effect. Therefore, for each time point, an SLI was calculated by subtracting mean power changes of the cluster ipsi- lateral to distractor presentation side from mean power changes of the cluster contralateral to distractor presen- tation side, averaged across left and right hemifield presen- tations. Time courses of the SLI for total power changes are depicted in Figure 5B, separately for the three experi- mental conditions. Following the same steps as in the analysis of the mid- frontal conflict effect reported above, next, we compared the time courses of the SLI between the COM and INC conditions. First, a repeated-measures ANOVA with the factors of Condition (COM vs. INC) and Time (six 100-msec time intervals from 0 to 0.6 sec following stimulus onset) was calculated. The analysis showed a significant main effect of Condition, F(1, 33) = 4.39, p = .044, a significant main effect of Time, F(5, 165) = 10.95, p < .001, and also a significant interaction between the two factors, F(5, 165) = 3.00, p = .013. Indeed, reliable differences be- tween conditions arose from 0.2 to 0.4 sec after stimulus onset with stronger sensory lateralization of total theta power in incompatible than compatible trials (means: 14.1% vs. −1.4%; Tukey HSD.05 = 14.2%). Additional anal- ysis showed that, in the said time interval, reliable differ- ences between conditions were similarly present in the first and second half of the experiment, F(1, 33) = 1.26, p = .270. In the analysis of the evoked and induced parts of the stimulus-locked sensory lateralization effect, theta activi- ties were averaged from 0.2 to 0.4 sec following stimulus onset and contrasted between the COM and INC condi- tions. A repeated-measures ANOVA with the factors of Con- dition (COM vs. INC) and Activity (evoked vs. induced) revealed significant main effects of Condition, F(1, 33) = 5.30, p = .028, and activity, F(1, 33) = 23.20, p < .001, but no significant interaction between the two factors, F(1, 33) < 1. The results indicate that evoked theta activity showed a stronger stimulus-locked sensory lateralization effect than induced theta activity. More important, compa- rable conflict effects arose for evoked (8.4% vs. 15.0%) and induced theta activities (−9.7% vs. −0.9%) contributing Figure 5. Sensory lateralization. (A) Topography of the sensory lateralization effect in occipital theta power (difference between left distractor presentation and right distractor presentation trials). Electrodes in white depict the two significant clusters in theta power (3–6 Hz; 0.05–0.25 sec after stimulus onset). (B) Stimulus-locked time course of the SLI (theta power changes in the cluster contralateral minus theta power changes in the cluster ipsilateral to distractor presentation side) as a function of experimental condition (COM, INC, NEU). The purple-shaded area indicates significant differences between incompatible and compatible trials, p < .05. (C, D) Response-locked and stimulus-locked time courses of the SLI as a function of response conflict (COM vs. INC). Dotted lines refer to the SLI of evoked theta power changes; dashed lines refer to the SLI of induced theta power changes. The purple-shaded areas indicate significant differences in the SLIs of both evoked and induced theta power changes between compatible and incompatible trials, p < .05. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 0 7 9 8 5 1 7 8 7 5 8 3 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 2 6 7 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Pastötter and Frings 993 Figure 6. Spearman correlations. Relationship of the sensory lateralization effect in evoked and induced theta power (INC minus COM; variable across time) with (A) the behavioral conflict effect in RT (INC minus COM; fixed) and (B) the midfrontal conflict effect in induced theta power (INC minus COM; fixed; based on the significant cluster in the response-locked analysis shown in Figure 4A). Asterisks mark significant correlations, p < .05, uncorrected. to the stimulus-locked sensory lateralization effect. The time courses are depicted in Figure 5C. In addition, the evoked and induced parts of the response-locked sensory lateralization effect were calcu- lated in the 500-msec interval before response onset (see Figure 5D). Repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factors of Condition (COM vs. INC) and Time (five 100-msec time intervals from −0.5 to 0 sec before response onset) were calculated separately for evoked and induced theta activities. The analyses showed a main effect of Time for induced theta, F(4, 132) = 5.93, p = .008, but not for evoked theta, F(4, 132) < 1. No significant main effect of Condition was found, neither for induced theta, F(1, 33) = 2.42, p = .130, nor for evoked theta, F(1, 33) < 1, and also no significant interaction arose, both Fs < 1. Together with the stimulus-locked analyses above, these results suggest that the modulation of sen- sory distractor processing induced by response conflict was primarily driven by stimulus-related theta activities and less, if at all, by response-related activities.2 Finally, correlational brain–behavior and brain–brain analyses were calculated. Figure 6 shows the results. Re- garding brain–behavior correlations, the results showed that the behavioral conflict effect in RT (individual differences between the INC and COM conditions) was positively related to the conflict effect in the SLI of evoked theta power change (individual differences between the INC and COM conditions) from 0.25 to 0.3 sec following stimulus onset (see Figure 6A). Regard- ing brain–brain correlations, the midfrontal conflict effect in induced theta power (response-locked; averaged from −0.2 to 0.2 sec around response onset) was negatively related to the conflict effect in the SLIs of both evoked and induced theta power change in different time inter- vals following the onset of stimuli (see Figure 6B). No significant correlation between the midfrontal conflict effect in induced theta power and the behavioral conflict effect in RT arose, r = .09, p = .599. DISCUSSION This study examined the behavioral and electrophysio- logical effects of response conflict in a lateralized version of the Eriksen flanker task. Behaviorally, the results showed effects of response (in)compatibility in both RT and accuracy, characterized by slower and more error- prone responses on incompatible than compatible trials, which is consistent with the findings from previous behavioral work (e.g., Frings & Spence, 2010; Evans & Craig, 1992; Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). At the electro- physiological level, the results showed a conflict effect on S-LRP onset, but no conflict effect on LRP-R onset, in- dicating that conflict affected the duration of premotor processes but did not influence the duration of motor processes (see Kopp et al., 1996; Gratton et al., 1988, for similar findings). With regard to EEG oscillatory activ- ities, we found a prominent effect of response conflict in (induced) midfrontal theta power (6–9 Hz), which is con- sistent with the findings from previous EEG work (e.g., Nigbur et al., 2011, 2012). Going beyond the previous work, the results showed that sensory lateralization of (both evoked and induced) theta activities (3–6 Hz) over occipital electrode sites was stronger on incompatible than compatible trials, thus indicating a modulation of early sensory distractor processing induced by late re- sponse conflict. Earlier research has shown that occipital brain activity in general and sensory lateralization in particular can be mod- ulated by the processing of conflicting information in the Eriksen flanker task (e.g., Marini et al., 2016; Appelbaum et al., 2011). In this earlier research, occipital brain activity was contrasted between trials with conflicting and (par- tially) nonconflicting distractor information. In partial- conflict trials, the distractors were conflicting (stimulus incongruent and response incompatible) in one hemifield but nonconflicting (stimulus congruent and response compatible) in the other hemifield. Examining ERPs, 994 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 30, Number 7 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 0 7 9 8 5 1 7 8 7 5 8 3 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 2 6 7 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Appelbaum et al. (2011) demonstrated a sensory lateraliza- tion effect in partial-conflict trials, with more positive ERPs over occipital sites contralateral than ipsilateral to the conflicting distractor presentation side. The effect arose from 0.2 to 0.55 sec after stimulus onset, simultaneously in time with the midfrontal N200 conflict effect. The researchers proposed that the sensory lateralization effect reflects attentional distraction toward the conflicting infor- mation. However, because of the nonorthogonal manipu- lation of stimulus and response conflict, it is not clear whether the distraction is caused by stimulus conflict, response conflict, or both. In this study, we examined whether occipital brain activity is modulated by response conflict arising at the response selection level. The results showed a clear modulation of early sensory distractor processing induced by response conflict. Indeed, both evoked and induced theta power over occipital electrode sites showed a stronger lateralization effect in incompati- ble than compatible trials from 0.2 to 0.4 sec following stimulus onset. On the individual subject level, the sensory lateralization effect in evoked theta power was correlated with the behavioral conflict effect in RT. Because theta oscillations in the visual system have been linked to later- alized control of visuospatial attention (e.g., Van der Lubbe et al., 2014; Green & McDonald, 2008), the sensory lateral- ization effect in theta power is suggested to reflect atten- tional distraction to conflicting distractor information. This view is consistent with the view by Appelbaum et al. (2011), with the additional proposal that it is response conflict at the response selection level that influences the processing of sensory distractor information. Future EEG work is needed that manipulates stimulus and response conflict orthogonally to evaluate the separate contribution of stimulus and response conflict to the sensory lateraliza- tion effect in a single experiment. TEC provides a theoretical framework for explaining the present results. TEC suggests a close link between the processes of stimulus perception and response activa- tion and the processing of stimulus and response conflict, respectively. TEC assumes that the perception of a stimulus and the action associated with this stimulus share a com- mon code and therefore are representationally and func- tionally related (Hommel, 2004, 2009). The idea is that, even after a single pairing of a stimulus and a response, an S-R episode is bound into a common memory, and upon repetition of any part of the episode, this memory is instantly retrieved. Imaging studies have suggested that posterior prefrontal, ventral temporal, and higher-order visual brain areas contribute to the processing of S-R bindings (Horner & Henson, 2008; Dobbins et al., 2004). These brain areas show an increase in activation when the response associated to a stimulus changes between stimulus presentations. With respect to the present flanker task, TEC predicts that—after an initial S-R pairing— reencountering a flanker stimulus should instantly activate the S-R episode and directly retrieve the associated re- sponse. This should lead to fast processing of response conflict in incompatible trials. In addition, TEC predicts that—due to S-R bindings—interference due to response conflict in incompatible trials could influence the process- ing at early levels of (sensory) information processing. This is exactly what the present results suggest, showing an early modulation of lateralized theta power over occipital electrodes induced by response conflict. Moreover, the modulation was found to be robust, that is, equally present in the first and second half of the experiment, which further corroborates the S-R binding view on the effect. Indeed, S-R bindings do not need to be gradually learned but are promptly constructed and stably retained (see Henson et al., 2014). In addition to the sensory lateralization effect, a promi- nent nonlateralized conflict effect in induced theta power (6–9 Hz) over midfrontal electrode sites was observed. This finding is in line with previous EEG studies that have demonstrated reliable conflict effects for induced theta power around response onset in various conflict tasks (e.g., Cohen & Donner, 2013; Cavanagh et al., 2012). The finding is also in line with previous studies that have re- ported conflict effects in induced theta power regardless of whether the evoked part of the signal was removed in the analysis or not (e.g., Cohen & Donner, 2013; Pastötter, Dreisbach, & Bäuml, 2013; Pastötter et al., 2012). The mid- frontal conflict effect in theta power has been source local- ized to ACC (e.g., Töllner et al., 2017; Nigbur et al., 2011; Pastötter et al., 2010; Hanslmayr et al., 2008), which is be- lieved to monitor and detect conflict between concurrent stimuli and responses (Botvinick et al., 2004; Van Veen & Carter, 2002a). According to the conflict-monitoring view, ACC is the key structure to initiate the temporal cascade of processes by which the brain responds to conflict and regulates attentional control. In this study, the midfrontal conflict effect in induced theta power began 0.5 sec after stimulus onset and thus followed in time the occipital con- flict effect in lateralized theta power. This finding suggests that the occipital cortex, but not ACC, was the first struc- ture to “detect” the conflict in the present task. Arguably, ACC thus may not have “detected” but may have “regis- tered” conflicting information and “communicated” the information to other brain areas in the present task. In fact, the present correlational analyses suggest that the conflict effect in lateralized theta power over occipital sites was negatively related to the midfrontal conflict effect in theta power, which indicates that conflict processing in the occipital cortex may have even reduced the processing of conflict in ACC. The finding of a negative correlation is consistent with recent work showing that early stimulus- related interregional phase coupling between parietal and frontal electrode sites in the theta-to-alpha frequency range is negatively related to midfrontal theta power increase in- duced by response conflict that was elicited by task-irrelevant motion direction of stimuli in a feature-based Simon task (Vissers, Ridderinkhof, Cohen, & Slagter, 2018). The proposal that ACC registers and communicates the conflict is in line with the cognitive control literature, Pastötter and Frings 995 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 0 7 9 8 5 1 7 8 7 5 8 3 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 2 6 7 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 which assumes that ACC detects (or registers) conflict on a current trial n and engages control functions in the DLPFC that bias future processing and reduce (potential) conflict on the following trial n + 1 (see Carter & van Veen, 2007, for a review). Consistent with this view, both behavioral and physiological conflict effects on trial n + 1 have been found to be modulated by conflict in trial n, indicating conflict adaptation effects (e.g., Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1992). Indeed, imaging studies have shown that ACC activ- ity is typically reduced on conflict trials following conflict trials as compared with conflict trials following nonconflict trials (e.g., Kerns, 2006; Kerns et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 1999). Corresponding evidence also arose from EEG stud- ies that examined the oscillatory signature of the conflict adaptation effect and showed a sequential modulation in midfrontal theta power (Töllner et al., 2017; van Driel, Swart, Egner, Ridderinkhof, & Cohen, 2015; Pastötter et al., 2013; Cohen & Cavanagh, 2011). Addressing the interplay between ACC and DLPFC, an intracranial EEG study by Oehrn et al. (2014) further showed that, in the Stroop task, the detection (or registration) of conflict in trial n is re- lated to theta power increase in ACC, whereas the com- munication of conflict to the DLPFC in trial n + 1 is mediated by theta phase synchronization between ACC and DLPFC and an increase in DLPFC gamma power (30–100 Hz). Addressing the contribution of visual brain regions to the sequential conflict adaptation effect in a Stroop task, an fMRI study by Egner and Hirsch (2005) showed that the processing of relevant targets (i.e., prom- inent faces) in high-level visual brain regions (i.e., the fusiform face area) can be enhanced in trials that followed the processing of conflicting distractors (i.e., incongruent names), whereas activity in the early visual cortex showed no conflict adaptation effect. In this study, we found a modulation of visual processes induced by conflict that was induced in the same trial n. Future EEG work is needed that examines possible sequential modulations of early visual processing in the present flanker and other conflict tasks (e.g., the accessory Simon task; see Footnote 2) with higher temporal resolution than fMRI. To conclude, previous fMRI and EEG work employing lateralized versions of the Eriksen flanker task have re- vealed modulations of early sensory distractor processing induced by conflict but did not further evaluate the sepa- rate contribution of stimulus and response conflict to sensory distraction. In the present EEG study, we took a first step in this direction and examined the contribu- tion of response conflict to the lateralized sensory dis- traction effect. The results showed a clear conflict modulation in lateralized theta power over occipital sites, indicating that early sensory distractor processing is modulated by late response conflict. The finding is con- sistent with theories of action control emphasizing S-R bindings (Henson et al., 2014; Hommel, 2004, 2009), which suggest a close link between stimulus and response repre- sentations, and the processing of stimulus and response conflict, respectively. Acknowledgments The authors thank B. Hommel and one anonymous reviewer for their comments on a previous version of the manuscript and T. Brinkmann, J. Paulus, J. Becker, and J. Lohner for their help with experimental setup and data collection. Reprint requests should be sent to Bernhard Pastötter, Depart- ment of Psychology, University of Trier, Universitätsring 15, 54286 Trier, Germany, or via e-mail: pastoetter@uni-trier.de. Notes 1. Regarding evoked theta power time-locked to stimulus onset, Figure 4D shows that there was a small but not significant difference between compatible and incompatible trials at peak power latency of 150 msec (28.4% vs. 30.6%), t(33) = 1.12, p = .273. Bayes hypothesis testing showed that the Bayes factor was 3.074, in favor of the null hypothesis over the two-sided alter- native hypothesis (calculated with default settings in the JASP soft- ware package; Wagenmakers et al., 2018). This indicates that the observed data were 3.074 times more likely under the null hypoth- esis than under the alternative hypothesis, thus providing moder- ate evidence for the null hypothesis (see Lee & Wagenmakers, 2013), which assumes that evoked theta power (150 msec after stimulus onset) was unaffected by response conflict. 2. The present flanker task additionally featured a Simon-like conflict effect, which has been called accessory Simon effect in the literature (e.g., Nishimura & Yokosawa, 2010). The effect refers to the finding that responses to centrally located targets are faster and less error-prone when the presentation side of the lateralized distractors is spatially congruent with the re- sponses to the targets. Therefore, we examined whether Simon congruency interacted with the major outcomes of the present task. ANOVAs on (median) RT, error rate, SLI of total theta power change, and induced midfrontal theta power change were calcu- lated with the additional factor of Simon congruency. All analyses showed a main effect of Simon congruency, due to slower RT, more response errors, stronger theta SLI, and higher midfrontal theta power in Simon-incongruent than Simon-congruent trials, all ps < .030. Importantly, none of the analyses showed a signif- icant interaction between Simon congruency and Experimental condition, all ps > .156, thus suggesting that the major outcomes
in the present task were unaffected by Simon congruency. Der
nonfinding of an interaction between Simon congruency and
experimental condition is consistent with the findings from an
earlier behavioral study by Diedrichsen, Ivry, Cohen, and Danziger
(2000), which also showed no modulation of response conflict
effects induced by Simon congruency (but did find such modula-
tion of stimulus-conflict effects). Vor allem, the present EEG finding
of a main effect of Simon congruency on theta SLI indicates that
the modulation of early sensory distractor processing induced by
(response) conflict may not be restricted to the present flanker
task but may generalize to other (response) conflict tasks.

VERWEISE

Appelbaum, L. G., Schmied, D. V., Boehler, C. N., Chen, W. D.,
& Woldorff, M. G. (2011). Rapid modulation of sensory
processing induced by stimulus conflict. Zeitschrift für Kognition
Neurowissenschaften, 23, 2620–2628.

Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Fuhrmann, C. S., & Cohen,
J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action
in anterior cingulate cortex. Natur, 402, 179–181.

Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Fuhrmann, C. S. (2004). Conflict
monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.

996

Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften

Volumen 30, Nummer 7

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Fuhrmann, C. S., & van Veen, V. (2007). Anterior cingulate cortex

and conflict detection: Ein Update von Theorie und Daten.
Kognitiv, Affektiv, & Verhaltensneurowissenschaften, 7, 367–379.

Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a
mechanism for cognitive control. Trends im kognitiven Bereich
Wissenschaften, 18, 414–421.

Cavanagh, J. F., Zambrano-Vazquez, L., & Allen, J. J. (2012).
Theta lingua franca: A common mid-frontal substrate for
action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology, 49, 220–238.
Chan, J. S., Merrifield, K., & Spence, C. (2005). Auditory spatial

attention assessed in a flanker interference task. Acta
Acustica United With Acustica, 91, 554–563.

Cohen, M. X., & Cavanagh, J. F. (2011). Single-trial regression
elucidates the role of prefrontal theta oscillations in response
conflict. Grenzen in der Psychologie, 2, 30.

Cohen, M. X., & Donner, T. H. (2013). Midfrontal conflict-related
theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict
behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 2752–2763.
Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject

designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method.
Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45.
Diedrichsen, J., Ivry, R. B., Cohen, A., & Danziger, S. (2000).
Asymmetries in a unilateral flanker task depend on the
direction of the response: The role of attentional shift and
perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 26, 113–126.

Dobbins, ICH. G., Schnyer, D. M., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L.
(2004). Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming
can result from rapid response learning. Natur, 428, 316–319.
Donohue, S. E., Todisco, A. E., & Woldorff, M. G. (2013). Der
rapid distraction of attentional resources toward the source
of incongruent stimulus input during multisensory conflict.
Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften, 25, 623–635.

Eder, A. B., Leuthold, H., Rothermund, K., & Schweinberger,
S. R. (2012). Automatic response activation in sequential
affective priming: An ERP study. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 7, 436–445.

Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms

resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant
Information. Naturneurowissenschaften, 8, 1784–1790.

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters
on the identification of target letters in a non-search task.
Wahrnehmung & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

Eriksen, C. W. (1995). The flankers task and response

competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of
cognitive problems. Visuelle Wahrnehmung, 2, 101–118.

Evans, P. M., & Craig, J. C. (1992). Response competition: A
major source of interference in a tactile identification task.
Attention, Wahrnehmung, & Psychophysics, 51, 199–206.
Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). To be or not to be…
included in an event file: Integration and retrieval of
distractors in stimulus-response episodes is influenced by
perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 37, 1209–1227.

Frings, C., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2007). Distractor

repetitions retrieve previous responses to targets. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1367–1377.

Frings, C., & Spence, C. (2010). Crossmodal congruency effects
based on stimulus identity. Gehirnforschung, 1354, 113–122.

Frings, C., Wentura, D., & Wühr, P. (2012). On the fate of
distractor representations. Journal of Experimental
Psychologie: Human Perception & Performance, 38, 570–575.

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing
the use of information: Strategic control of activation of
responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Allgemein,
121, 480–506.

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., &
Donchin, E. (1988). Pre- and poststimulus activation of

response channels: A psychophysiological analysis. Zeitschrift
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 14, 331–344.

Grün, J. J., & McDonald, J. J. (2008). Electrical neuroimaging
reveals timing of attentional control activity in human brain.
PLoS Biology, 6, e81.

Hanslmayr, S., Pastötter, B., Bäuml, K.-H., Gruber, S., Wimber,

M., & Klimesch, W. (2008). The electrophysiological
dynamics of interference during the Stroop task. Zeitschrift
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 215–225.

Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Hennighausen,
E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen task: Inhibitory executive
processes? Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 218–225.
Henson, R. N. (2009). Priming. In L. Squire, T. Albright, F.
Bloom, F. Gage, & N. Spitzer (Hrsg.), Encyclopedia of
neuroscience (S. 1055–1063). Amsterdam: Sonst.

Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A.
(2014). Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 18, 376–384.

Hoechstetter, K., Bornfleth, H., Weckesser, D., Ille, N., Berg, P.,
& Scherg, M. (2004). BESA source coherence: A new method
to study cortical oscillatory coupling. Brain Topography, 16,
233–238.

Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across
perception and action. Trends in den Kognitionswissenschaften, 8,
494–500.

Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory
of event coding). Psychological Research, 73, 512–526.
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001).

The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for
perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain
Wissenschaften, 24, 849–878.

Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2008). Priming, response

learning and repetition suppression. Neuropsychologie, 46,
1979–1991.

Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2012). Incongruent abstract
stimulus–response bindings result in response interference:
fMRI and EEG evidence from visual object classification
priming. Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften, 24,
760–773.

Ille, N., Berg, P., & Scherg, M. (2002). Artifact correction of the
ongoing EEG using spatial filters based on artifact and brain
signal topographies. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology,
19, 113–124.

Kalcher, J., & Pfurtscheller, G. (1995). Discrimination between
phase-locked and non-phase-locked event-related EEG activity.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 94,
381–384.

Kelley, T. A., & Lavie, N. (2011). Working memory load

modulates distractor competition in primary visual cortex.
Hirnrinde, 21, 659–665.

Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex

activity in an fMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the
Simon task. Neurobild, 33, 399–405.

Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., III, Cho, R. Y.,
Stenger, V. A., & Fuhrmann, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate
conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Wissenschaft, 303,
1023–1026.

Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task
as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control.
Psychophysiology, 33, 282–294.

Lamers, M. J. M., & Roelofs, A. (2011). Attentional control

adjustments in Eriksen and Stroop task performance can be
independent of response conflict. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 64, 1056–1081.

Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian cognitive
modeling: A practical course. Cambridge: Cambridge
Universitätsverlag.

Pastötter and Frings

997

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3

Liotti, M., Woldorff, M. G., Perez, R., & Mayberg, H. S. (2000).
An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word
interference effect. Neuropsychologie, 38, 701–711.

MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Fuhrmann, C. S.
(2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal
and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Wissenschaft,
288, 1835–1838.

Marini, F., Demeter, E., Roberts, K. C., Chelazzi, L., & Woldorff,

M. G. (2016). Orchestrating proactive and reactive
mechanisms for filtering distracting information:
Brain–behavior relationships revealed by a mixed-design
fMRT-Studie. Zeitschrift für Neurowissenschaften, 36, 988–1000.

Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical
testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Zeitschrift für Neurowissenschaften
Methoden, 164, 177–190.

Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for episodic retrieval
of inadequate prime responses in auditory negative priming.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
& Performance, 32, 932–943.

Müller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Jackknife-based
method for measuring LRP onset latency differences.
Psychophysiology, 35, 99–115.

Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D., & Jonides, J. (2007). Interference

Auflösung: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks.
Kognitiv, Affektiv, & Verhaltensneurowissenschaften, 7, 1–17.
Nigbur, R., Cohen, M. X., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Stürmer, B.

(2012). Theta dynamics reveal domain-specific control over
stimulus and response conflict. Zeitschrift für Kognition
Neurowissenschaften, 24, 1264–1274.

Nigbur, R., Ivanova, G., & Stürmer, B. (2011). Theta power as a
marker for cognitive interference. Clinical Neurophysiology,
122, 2185–2194.

Nishimura, A., & Yokosawa, K. (2010). Visual and auditory

accessory stimulus offset and the Simon effect. Attention,
Wahrnehmung, & Psychophysics, 72, 1965–1974.

Oehrn, C. R., Hanslmayr, S., Fell, J., Deuker, L., Kremers, N. A.,
Do Lam, A. T., et al. (2014). Neural communication patterns
underlying conflict detection, Auflösung, and adaptation.
Zeitschrift für Neurowissenschaften, 34, 10438–10452.

Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data

and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.

Pastötter, B., Berchtold, F., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2012). Oscillatory

correlates of controlled speed-accuracy tradeoff in a
response-conflict task. Kartierung des menschlichen Gehirns, 33,
1834–1849.

Pastötter, B., Dreisbach, G., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2013). Dynamic
adjustments of cognitive control: Oscillatory correlates of the
conflict adaptation effect. Zeitschrift für Kognition
Neurowissenschaften, 25, 2167–2178.

Pastötter, B., Hanslmayr, S., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2010). Conflict

processing in the anterior cingulate cortex constrains
response priming. Neurobild, 50, 1599–1605.

Peterson, B. S., Kane, M. J., Alexander, G. M., Lacadie, C.,

Skudlarski, P., Leung, H.-C., et al. (2002). An event-related
functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the
Simon and Stroop tasks. Kognitive Gehirnforschung, 13,
427–440.

Pfurtscheller, G., & Aranibar, A. (1977). Event-related cortical

desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp

EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology,
42, 817–826.

Schienbein, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). Eine Rezension von
contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin,
136, 943–974.

Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor

Aktion. Psychological Research, 68, 176–188.

Tallon-Baudry, C., & Bertrand, Ö. (1999). Oscillatory gamma
activity in humans and its role in object representation.
Trends in den Kognitionswissenschaften, 3, 151–162.

Thut, G., Nietzel, A., Brandt, S. A., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2006).
Alpha-band electroencephalographic activity over occipital
cortex indexes visuo-spatial attention bias and predicts visual
target detection. Zeitschrift für Neurowissenschaften, 26, 9494–9502.
Töllner, T., Wang, Y., Makeig, S., Müller, H. J., Jung, T. P., &

Gramann, K. (2017). Two independent frontal midline theta
oscillations during conflict detection and adaptation in a
Simon-type manual reaching task. Zeitschrift für Neurowissenschaften,
37, 2504–2515.

Ulrich, R., & Müller, J. (2001). Using the jackknife-based scoring
method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs.
Psychophysiology, 38, 816–827.

Van der Lubbe, R. H., Bundt, C., & Abrahamse, E. L. (2014).

Internal and external spatial attention examined with lateralized
EEG power spectra. Gehirnforschung, 1583, 179–192.
van Driel, J., Swart, J. C., Egner, T., Ridderinkhof, K. R., &

Cohen, M. X. (2015). (NEIN) time for control: Frontal theta
dynamics reveal the cost of temporally guided conflict
anticipation. Kognitiv, Affektiv, & Behavioral
Neurowissenschaften, 15, 787–807.

Van Veen, V., & Fuhrmann, C. S. (2002A). The anterior cingulate as a

conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology &
Behavior, 77, 477–482.

Van Veen, V., & Fuhrmann, C. S. (2002B). The timing of action-
monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex.
Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften, 14, 593–602.

Van Veen, V., Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., Stenger, V. A., &

Fuhrmann, C. S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict
Überwachung, and levels of processing. Neurobild, 14,
1302–1308.

Vissers, M. E., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Cohen, M. X., & Slagter, H. A.
(2018). Oscillatory mechanisms of response conflict elicited
by color and motion direction: An individual differences
Ansatz. Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften, 30, 468–481.

Wagenmakers, E.-J., Liebe, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A.,

Verhagen, A. J., et al. (2018). Bayesian inference for
Psychologie. Part II: Example applications with JASP.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Rezension, 25, 58–76.

White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., & Starns, J. J. (2011). Diffusion models

of the flanker task: Discrete versus gradual attentional
Auswahl. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 210–238.

Worden, M. S., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., & Simpson, G. V. (2000).
Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by
retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography
increases over occipital cortex. Zeitschrift für Neurowissenschaften, 20,
RC63.

Wühr, P., & Frings, C. (2008). A case for inhibition: Visual

attention suppresses the processing of irrelevant objects.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Allgemein, 137, 116–130.

998

Zeitschrift für kognitive Neurowissenschaften

Volumen 30, Nummer 7

l

D
Ö
w
N
Ö
A
D
e
D

F
R
Ö
M
H

T
T

P

:
/
/

D
ich
R
e
C
T
.

M

ich
T
.

e
D
u

/
J

/

Ö
C
N
A
R
T
ich
C
e

P
D

l

F
/

/

/

/

3
0
7
9
8
5
1
7
8
7
5
8
3

/
J

Ö
C
N
_
A
_
0
1
2
6
7
P
D

.

F

B
j
G
u
e
S
T

T

Ö
N
0
8
S
e
P
e
M
B
e
R
2
0
2
3It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of image
It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of image
It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of image
It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of image
It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of image
It’s the Other Way Around! Early Modulation of image

PDF Herunterladen