Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited

Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited
by Statistical Regularities in Visual Search

Benchi Wang, Joram van Driel, Eduard Ort, and Jan Theeuwes

Abstracto

■ Salient yet irrelevant objects often capture our attention and in-
terfere with our daily tasks. Distraction by salient objects can be re-
duced by suppressing the location where they are likely to appear.
The question we addressed here was whether suppression of fre-
quent distractor locations is already implemented beforehand, in an-
ticipation of the stimulus. Using EEG, we recorded cortical activity of
human participants searching for a target while ignoring a salient dis-
tractor. The distractor was presented more often at one location
than at any other location. We found reduced capture for distractors
at frequent locations, indicating that participants learned to avoid
distraction. críticamente, we found evidence for “proactive suppression”

as already “prior to display onset,” there was enhanced power in
parieto-occipital alpha oscillations contralateral to the frequent dis-
tractor location—a signal known to occur in anticipation of irrelevant
información. Locked to display onset, ERP analysis showed a distrac-
tor suppression-related distractor positivity (PD) component for this
ubicación. En tono rimbombante, this PD was found regardless of whether dis-
tracting information was presented at the frequent location. In addi-
ción, there was an early PD component representing an early
attentional index of the frequent distractor location. Nuestros resultados
show anticipatory (proactive) suppression of frequent distractor
locations in visual search already starting prior to display onset. ■

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

3
1
1
0
1
5
3
5
1
8
6
0
8
1
1

/

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
4
3
3
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

INTRODUCCIÓN

In everyday life, we try to attend to events that are rele-
vant to us and ignore information that could distract us.
Por ejemplo, when driving along a busy road, we devote
attention to relevant objects such as road signs and other
cars while ignoring salient irrelevant objects such as bill-
boards. The extent to which we are able to avoid distrac-
tion from salient events has been a central question for
many years.

Traditionally, attentional selection is assumed to be the
result of the interaction between the goals of the observer
(current selection goals) and the physical properties of the
visual environment (salience of the objects; Theeuwes,
2010; Itti & Koch, 2001; Egeth & Yantis, 1997). Recientemente, él
was pointed out that, in many cases, selection is neither the
result of goals of the observer nor the result of stimulus-
driven (es decir., bottom–up saliency) factores (Failing &
Theeuwes, 2018; Theeuwes, 2018; Awh, Belopolsky, &
Theeuwes, 2012). A third category labeled as “selection his-
tory” referred to those instances in which selection is the
result of previous attentional deployments (Theeuwes,
2019). These previous deployments can result in lingering
and enduring selection biases that are neither related to
the current goals nor to the saliency of objects.

A well-known example of how selection history affects
attentional selection is research known as “contextual
cueing,” demonstrating that search for a target is

Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam

© 2019 Instituto de Tecnología de Massachusetts

facilitated when it appears in a visual layout that was pre-
viously searched relative to visual layouts that were never
seen before ( Jiang & Chun, 2001; Chun & Jiang, 1998,
1999). These studies have highlighted that people pick
up on the statistical regularities present in the display,
such that they selectively attend those locations that
are relevant for the task. More recently, it was also dem-
onstrated that people did not only activate locations that
are relevant but also selectively inhibit locations of
salient distractors that distract them (Failing, Wang, &
Theeuwes, 2019; Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a, 2018b,
2018C). In these studies, Wang and Theeuwes used the
well-established additional singleton task (Theeuwes,
1992) in which observers search for a unique shape sin-
gleton while a unique and salient color singleton is simul-
taneously present. Because observers cannot search for
one specific target feature, large capture effects are typi-
cally observed in this paradigm (see Wang & Theeuwes,
2018C). Wang and Theeuwes also manipulated the distri-
butional properties of the location of salient distractor. Él
was shown that when a salient distractor appeared more
often in one location than in all other locations, its dis-
tracting effect (the extent to which it captured attention)
was attenuated, suggesting that this frequent distractor
location competed less for attention than all other loca-
ciones. They also showed that when the target happened
to be presented at that location, its selection was ham-
pered (see Ferrante et al., 2018, for similar results).
Además, there was a spatial gradient of suppression
around this location.

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva 31:10, páginas. 1535–1548
doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01433

The finding that we are able to inhibit frequent dis-
tractor locations that are likely to contain salient dis-
tractor has important implications for theories of visual
selection and other related fields. ERP studies have
shown that ignoring of a salient distractor is typically accom-
panied by a distractor positivity (PD) component in the ERP
waveform (Feldmann-Wüstefeld, Uengoer, & Schubö, 2015;
Burra & Kerzel, 2014; Gaspar & McDonald, 2014; Sawaki &
Luck, 2010), indicating spatial suppression of its location
during search (Hickey, Di Lollo, & McDonald, 2008).
Sin embargo, it remains unclear whether such suppression
of frequent distractor locations is already implemented
beforehand, in anticipation of the stimulus.

En el estudio actual, using EEG, we recorded cortical
activity of human participants who were searching for a
objetivo, while ignoring a salient distractor. The salient
distractor was presented more often in one location
(high-probability location) than in all other locations
(low-probability location). Consistent with a suppression
account and our previous behavioral studies ( Wang &
Theeuwes, 2018a, 2018b, 2018C), we expected to observe
the classic distractor-suppression-related PD component
in the ERP signal when a distractor was presented at
the high-probability location. Sin embargo, if spatial suppres-
sion to this location is applied well before display onset,
we would also expect to observe increased power in
parieto-occipital alpha oscillations contralateral to the lo-
cation that was more likely to contain the distractor rela-
tive to the ipsilateral location ( Worden, Foxe, Wang, &
Simpson, 2000; see Klimesch, 1999, para una revisión). Nosotros también
conducted a baseline experiment in which all locations
were equally likely to contain a distractor (es decir., sin
any statistical regularities), and here, we expected to see
no EEG components related to spatial suppression.

MÉTODOS

The study was approved by the ethical review committee
of the Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences of the
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.

Participantes

Separate groups with 18 participants each (13 women
y 5 men in Experiment 1 with a mean age of 20.9
años; 17 women and 1 man in Experiment 2 con un
mean age of 19.7 años) took part in the study. All par-
ticipants provided written informed consent prior to the
experiment and were reimbursed for their participation
either with money or course credit. They reported
normal color vision and normal or corrected-to-normal
visual acuity.

Apparatus and Stimuli

Stimulus presentation and response registration were
controlled by custom scripts written in Python run on

an HP Compaq 8000 Elite computer with a 22-in. liquid
crystal display color monitor (Samsung Syncmaster 2233,
1680 × 1050 pixels at 120 Hz). Participants were seated
in a sound-attenuated and dimly lit laboratory, at a viewing
distance of 75 cm. They were instructed to sit as relaxed as
possible to minimize muscle activity and other electro-
physiological noise to appear in the EEG signal.

The primary experimental display contained a visual
search array consisting of eight discrete stimuli with dif-
ferent shapes (one circle vs. seven unfilled diamonds, o
viceversa), with each shape containing a vertical or hor-
izontal gray line (0.3° × 1.5°) inside. These stimuli were
presented on an imaginary circle with a radius of 4°, cen-
tered at the fixation (a white cross measuring 1° × 1°),
against a black background (6 cd/m2). The circle’s radius
was 1°, the unfilled diamond was subtended 2° × 2°, y
each shape had a red or green outline (the color was cho-
sen from CIE Lab color space, centered at L = 70, a = 5,
b = 0, with a radius of 60, with an almost equal lumi-
nance range: 36–44 cd/m2).

Procedure and Design

A fixation cross appeared and remained visible through-
out the trial. Después 1350 mseg, the search array was pre-
sented for 2450 msec or until the participant responded.
Participants had to search for a circle (objetivo) entre
seven diamonds (distractors) or vice versa and indicate
whether the line segment inside the target was vertical
or horizontal by pressing the up or left key on the key-
board as fast as possible. Because the target singleton
could either be a diamond between circles or a circle
between diamonds, observers most likely adopted a
“singleton detection mode,” which makes it impossible
to selectively attend one specific feature (see Bacon &
Egeth, 1994). The intertrial interval was between 0 y
350 msec at random.

Experimento 1: With High-Probability Location

The target was presented in each trial, and it was equally
likely to be a circle or a diamond. A uniquely colored dis-
tractor singleton appeared in 66% of the trials, con el
same shape as other distractors but a different color (rojo
or green with an equal probability). All conditions were
randomized within blocks. Those display elements were
presented at eight locations from the imaginary ring with
a 4° radius. The target and distractor singleton were only
presented along the vertical or horizontal axis. El
remaining four locations served as neutral display ele-
mentos. Para cada participante, one of the horizontal loca-
tions contained a distractor singleton in 55% of trials
(high-probability location); for three other locations (en
the cardinal axis), the probability of containing a distractor
singleton was 15% cada (low-probability location). El
high-probability location was counterbalanced across
Participantes. In the distractor singleton present condition,

1536

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva

Volumen 31, Número 10

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

3
1
1
0
1
5
3
5
1
8
6
0
8
1
1

/

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
4
3
3
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

the target appeared equally often at each possible loca-
ción, except for the high-probability location.1 In the no-
distractor condition, the target appeared equally often at
each possible location.

Participants completed 40 practice trials and 13 bloques,
each containing 120 ensayos. Warning messages were pre-
sented if they did not respond or pressed the wrong
key. Depending on the singleton distractors and target
ubicación, a total of five conditions were analyzed:
(1) the distractor singleton was presented at the high-
probability location while the target was presented verti-
cally; (2) the target was presented at high-probability
location while the distractor singleton was either absent
or presented vertically; (3) the distractor singleton was
either absent or presented vertically, while the target
was presented vertically; (4) the distractor singleton
was presented along the horizontal axis at the lateral
low-probability location, while the target was presented
vertically; y (5) the distractor singleton was either
absent or presented vertically, while the target was
presented along the horizontal axis at the lateral low-
probability location.

Experimento 2: Without High-Probability Location

Experimento 2 served as a baseline experiment to compare it
with the results acquired in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2,
we used the same design as in Experiment 1 except that all
four potential target and singleton distractor locations were
equiprobable. Participants completed 40 practice trials and
nine blocks, each containing 120 ensayos. As there was no
high-probability location in Experiment 2, only three condi-
tions were analyzed: (1) the distractor singleton was pre-
sented laterally, while the target was presented vertically;
(2) the target was presented laterally when the distractor
singleton was absent or presented vertically; y (3)
the distractor singleton was either absent or presented
vertically, while the target was also presented vertically.
Tenga en cuenta que, in both experiments, both target and distractor
singleton were salient objects.

Data Recording and Preprocessing

EEG data were collected using a BioSemi ActiveTwo sys-
tema (www.biosemi.com) de 64 electrodes, placed ac-
cording to the extended 10–20 system and digitized at
512 Hz. Además, to record eye movements, the ver-
tical EOG was recorded from electrodes located 2 cm
above and below the right eye, and the horizontal EOG
was recorded from electrodes 1 cm lateral to the external
canthi.

EEG data were re-referenced to the mean of the left
and right earlobes and high-pass filtered using a cutoff
de 1.5 Hz (for independent component analysis [ICA]
solo) y 0.1 Hz (for final analyses). High-pass filtering
was done with default settings of the pop_basicfilter
function of ERPLAB (Lopez-Calderon & Luck, 2014),

using a Butterworth IIR filter. Continuous EEG was first
epoched from –3000 to 3500 msec relative to search dis-
play onset (to avoid edge artifacts resulting from wavelet
convolution; see below). Trial rejection and ICA were
done on smaller epochs (–1500 to 1500 mseg) to avoid
overlapping time points across epochs. para mejorar
ICA, epochs were baseline-normalized using the whole
epoch as baseline (Groppe, Makeig, & Kutas, 2009).

Malfunctioning electrodes were visually detected and
temporarily removed from the data. An adapted version
of an automatic trial rejection procedure, as imple-
mented in the Fieldtrip toolbox (Oostenveld, Fries,
Maris, & Schoffelen, 2011), was used to detect EMG arti-
hechos. Específicamente, we used a 110–140 Hz bandpass filter
to capture muscle activity and allowed for variable z-score
cutoffs per participant based on the within-subject vari-
ance of z scores. After trial rejection, ICA, as implemented
in the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme & Makeig, 2004), era
performed on the clean electrodes only. Together with
the vertical EOG and horizontal EOG signals, we visually
inspected and removed ICA components that captured
eye blinks, eye movement, or other artifacts that were
clearly not brain-driven activity. Afterwards, we interpo-
lated the malfunctioning electrodes identified earlier
using spherical spline interpolation as implemented in
EEGLAB’s eeg_interp.m function. Trials with an incorrect
(or no) response were also excluded from further analy-
ses. All steps together left 87.4% (±6.0% across partici-
pants) trials in Experiment 1 y 73.5% (±3.4% across
Participantes) trials in Experiment 2.

Time–Frequency Analysis
Time–Frequency Decomposition: Laplacian
Before time–frequency decomposition, we estimated the
surface Laplacian of the EEG data (Perrin, Pernier,
Bertrand, & Echallier, 1989), which is equivalent to the
current source density approach. This method acts as a
spatial high-pass filter by accentuating local effects while
filtering out distant effects due to volume conduction;
de este modo, it sharpens the EEG topography (Kayser & Tenke,
2015; cohen, 2014). For estimating the surface Laplacian,
we used a 10th-order Legendre polynomial, and λ was
set at 10−5.

Morlet Wavelet Convolution

The epoched EEG time series were decomposed into
their time–frequency representations using custom-
written MATLAB scripts (github.com/joramvd/tfdecomp).
The time series were convolved with a set of Morlet wave-
lets with frequencies ranging from 1 a 40 Hz in 25 loga-
rithmically spaced steps. The complex wavelets were
created by multiplying sine waves (ei2πft, where i is the
complex operator, f is frequency, and t is time) con un
−t^2/2s^2, where s is the width of the Gaussian).
Gaussian (mi

Wang et al.

1537

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

3
1
1
0
1
5
3
5
1
8
6
0
8
1
1

/

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
4
3
3
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

The width of the Gaussian was set as s = δ/2πf, where δ
represents the number of cycles of each wavelet, logarith-
mically spaced between 3 y 12 to have a good trade-off
between temporal and frequency precision. Convolution
was applied in the frequency domain: The fast Fourier
transform was applied to both the EEG data and the
Morlet wavelets, and these were multiplied in the fre-
quency domain, after which the inverse fast Fourier trans-
form was applied. The frequency-specific power at each
time point was defined as the squared magnitude of the
complex signal resulting from the convolution (real(Zt)2 +
imagen(Zt)2). Raw power was transformed into one com-
mon scale across frequencies using a within-subject per-
mutation testing procedure (see below).

Frequency Band and Time Window of Interest

Based on previous studies, we selected PO3/4, PO7/8, y
O1/2 for the analysis (de Vries, van Driel, & Olivers, 2017;
van Driel, Gunseli, Meeter, & Olivers, 2017). We were spe-
cifically interested in the lower (7.5–10 Hz) and upper
(10–12.5 Hz) alpha frequency ranges (see Klimesch,
1996, 1999, 2012, for reviews) to track how attention is
guided by implicit regularities before the search display
onset, in a time window comprising the intertrial interval
(–1500 to 0 mseg). Además, because participants could
only anticipate the high-probability distractor location and
not the target location nor the actual distractor location,
we combined all five conditions together in both experi-
ments for the prestimulus alpha-band analysis.

Z Transformation of Lateralized Power

Raw power can only take positive values and has a 1/f
power scaling, which requires a transformation such as
dB conversion or percent signal change (cohen, 2014).
Such relative metrics, Sucesivamente, require a baseline time
range, for which typically a prestimulus interval is chosen.
Sin embargo, for this analysis, we were specifically interested
in a between-group comparison of condition average
lateralized (contra- vs. ipsilateral to anticipated distractor
ubicación) alpha power during this prestimulus interval.
We thus Z-transformed time–frequency power through
a within-subject statistical procedure. For each contra-
and ipsilateral channel pair (thus excluding midline elec-
trodes) and each time–frequency point, we first computed
the trial average difference scores of contra- minus ipsi-
lateral power. Próximo, at each of 1000 permutations, nosotros
swapped for a random subset of trials the order of this
comparación (es decir., ipsilateral minus contralateral) and com-
puted the trial average across all trials. This gave a distri-
bution of “lateralized” power values under the null
hypothesis of no difference between contra- and ipsilateral
canales. The observed lateralized power values were
then statistically transformed with respect to the mean
and standard deviation of this distribution, by computing
their Z values. This procedure yields a metric that shows

interpretable dynamics over time as well as frequency.
Because there was no anticipated distractor location in
the baseline experiment (Experimento 2), we labeled one
lateral side as “contralateral” and the other side as “ipsilat-
eral” consistently for all participants and accordingly com-
puted the same Z transformation for time–frequency
power as was done for the real testing (Experimento 1) con
predictable lateralized distractors.

Estadísticas
A nivel de grupo, time–frequency maps of power of the
selected electrode groups were first statistically tested
against baseline within each experimental group, usando
a permutation-based approach with cluster size correc-
ción. Sin embargo, with permutation testing, the more points
are added to the analysis, the less sensitive it becomes. Si
it is known in advance that a specific frequency band is
likely to exhibit the effect of interest, then one should in-
corporate this prior knowledge and limit the statistical
test to this certain band (Oostenveld et al., 2011; Maris
& Oostenveld, 2007). Our a priori hypothesis pertained
specifically to preonset alpha band enhancement. De este modo,
in addition to a group-specific descriptive test over all
time–frequency points, we further narrowed our hypoth-
esis test of a group difference to the alpha-band only.

We ran two types of cluster-corrected permutation
pruebas: (1) Within each group, we used one-sample t tests
across time and frequency; at each test, the sign of the
power values was shuffled across participants in 2000
permutations to yield a null distribution against which
observed clusters of summed t values were thresholded
using p < .05; (2) Between groups, we used indepen- dent-samples t tests, comparing the two groups across time and frequency, and on alpha-band average power over time. Here, we shuffled participants across experi- ment in 2000 permutations for the null distribution. ERP Analysis The ERP analysis was done on a prespecified time window from −100 to 900 msec relative to display onset. To obtain ERPs, single-trial EEG data were first baseline- corrected using a prestimulus baseline period from −100 to 0 msec, after which data were trial-averaged. Experiment 1: With High-Probability Location For each participant, mean contralateral and ipsilateral activ- ity over conditions relative to the high-probability location was calculated for electrodes PO7/8. To determine the time window used for further statistical analyses of different ERP components, the lateralized ERP (contralateral minus ipsi- lateral to the high-probability location) was calculated, showing lateralized activity due to the high-probability distractor location. The time window was also selected re- ferred to previous studies (Feldmann-Wüstefeld & Vogel, 1538 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 31, Number 10 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 2019; Hickey et al., 2008; Hickey, McDonald, & Theeuwes, 2006; Eimer, 1996). Thus, the early PD epoch was deter- mined as ±20 msec from the most positive peak be- tween 50 and 150 msec. The PD epoch was determined as ±20 msec from the most positive peak between 150 and 250 msec. When the target was presented at the lateral low-probability location, the target N2pc epoch was deter- mined as ±20 msec from the most positive peak be- tween 150 and 250 msec (note that the positive peak was used, because the lateralized ERP was defined with respect to the high-probability distractor location). When the target was presented at the high-probability location, the target–N2pc epoch was determined as the most negative peak between 250 and 350 msec, because we assumed a delayed target selection due to the sup- pression on the high-probability location. The mean am- plitude for these time windows was calculated separately for laterality (contra vs. ipsi), each condition, and each participant. Experiment 2: Without High-Probability Location For each participant, mean contralateral and ipsilateral activ- ity over conditions relative to the target or distractor single- ton was calculated for electrodes PO7/8. To determine the time window used for further statistical analyses of different ERP components, both the lateralized ERP (contralateral minus ipsilateral) for trials with lateral target and for trials with lateral distractor singleton were collapsed, showing lateralized activity due to targets or distractor singletons. The target–N2pc epoch was determined as ±15 msec from the most negative peak between 200 and 300 msec in the lateral targets waveform. The mean amplitude for these time windows was calculated separately for laterality (contra vs. ipsi), each condition, and each participant. RESULTS Behavior Consistent with Wang and Theeuwes (2018a, 2018b), distractors at the high-probability location interfered less (828 msec) than distractors at the low-probability loca- tion (865 msec), t(17) = 3.42, p = .003. Compared with the no-distractor condition (777 msec), there were signif- icant attentional capture effects for a distractor present at the high-probability location, t(17) = 4.26, p = .001, and the low-probability location, t(17) = 8.61, p < .001. Also, consistent with earlier studies ( Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a, 2018b, 2018c), target selection was less efficient when the target appeared at the high-probability distrac- tor location (808 msec) relative to the low-probability distractor location (767 msec), t(17) = 3.73, p = .002. To show that these effects were due to the predictability of the high-probability location, we also ran a baseline exper- iment (Experiment 2) in which all locations were equally likely to contain a salient distractor singleton. Consistent with earlier studies (Theeuwes, 1991, 1992), this showed a classic attentional capture effect, with slower RTs when the distractor singleton was present (850 msec) compared with when it was absent (773 msec), t(17) = 10.59, p < .001. See Appendix for more details. Prestimulus: Local Alpha Power As the measure of anticipation, we chose lateralized alpha power (following Klimesch, 1996). We analyzed the oscillation in the alpha band (7.5–12.5 Hz) for elec- trodes PO3/4, PO7/8, and O1/2 (see Methods section for oscillation band and electrode selection) and found a lateralized effect consisting of a stronger enhancement in power for contra- versus ipsilateral relative to the high- probability location in Experiment 1 (see Figure 1A). Importantly, there was no lateralized enhancement in Experiment 2 (see Figure 1B). A permutation test across time and frequency comparing Experiments 1 and 2 confirmed this finding, showing that the significant differ- ence ( p < .05, uncorrected) between groups was mainly seen in the alpha band (see Figure 1C, upper panels for an illustration). Importantly, a cluster-based permutation test on the alpha-band comparing Experiments 1 and 2 revealed a reliable difference between the two groups in the −1220 and −660 msec interval prior to display onset ( p < .05; see Figure 1C, lower left panel for an illustration). Also, even without using the cluster-based permutation test, when comparing the average increased alpha power between the two groups, there was a reli- able difference before display onset, t = 2.37, p = .03. We interpret these results as evidence for anticipatory suppression of the high-probability location. Previous research has shown that lower alpha band (7.5–10 Hz) is more related to short-lasting expectation and attentional processing, whereas the upper alpha band (10–12.5 Hz) is more related to semantic memory processing (see Klimesch, 1996, 1999, 2012, for reviews). Given these findings, we divided the alpha band in two, in order to explore whether the anticipatory suppression effect related to the high-probability location would in- deed only occur in the lower alpha band. For the lower alpha band, a cluster-based permutation test between ex- periments showed that the power enhancement was sig- nificant from −1260 to 60 msec relative to display onset ( p < .05; see Figure 1C, middle panel). No effects were observed for the upper alpha band (see Figure 1C, right panel). In short, current results suggest that observers start sup- pressing the high-probability location about ∼1200 msec before display onset. Moreover, this prestimulus sup- pression (i.e., implicit expectation that a distractor would appear on the high-probability location) is mainly reflected in the lower alpha band, which is consistent with previous studies (see Klimesch, 2012, for a review). Wang et al. 1539 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 1. (A) Lateralized posterior alpha power in Experiment 1 with high-probability location, (B) lateralized posterior alpha power in Experiment 2 as a baseline, and (C) their comparison results. A and B show time–frequency plots of contralateral minus ipsilateral power at the average of O1/2, PO3/4, and PO7/8 in different experiments (red dashed outline indicates the alpha band oscillation between stable fixation and search display onset, and black solid outline indicates [uncorrected] significant power difference between contralateral and ipsilateral in grand average at p < .05). In C, upper left panel shows time–frequency plots of Experiment 1 minus Experiment 2 (baseline) power at the average of O1/2, PO3/4, and PO7/8; upper right panel shows half topographies, grand-averaged scalp distributions of alpha power at ipsilateral electrodes subtracted from those at contralateral electrodes, collapsed across hemispheres, and power is averaged over the alpha band oscillation between stable fixation and search display onset; lower panels present time series of contralateral minus ipsilateral alpha power difference between Experiments 1 and 2 for overall alpha band, lower alpha band, and upper alpha band (black horizontal line represents time points with a significant difference between experiments after cluster-size thresholding at p < .05). The red shadow area indicates means ± SEM of difference values. Poststimulus: PD To provide converging evidence that the high-probability location is suppressed, we first examined whether a PD was elicited when the distractor singleton was presented at the high- versus the low-probability location (see Hickey et al., 2008). Consistent with the behavioral data showing reduced capture, we observed a PD when the distractor was presented in the high-probability location and not when it was presented in the low-probability location. A two-way ANOVA with within-subject factors Laterality (contra- vs. ipsilateral to the distractor single- ton) and Distractor location (high vs. low probability) was conducted on the average ERP amplitude within the PD time window (186–226 msec; see Methods section for choice of time window). The results showed a main effect for Laterality, F(1, 17) = 9.51, p = .007, ηp 2 = .36, but not for Distractor location, F(1, 17) = 2.92, p = .105, ηp 2 = .15. However, importantly, the interaction was reliable, F(1, 17) = 4.98, p = .039, ηp 2 = .23. Planned comparisons trac- ing the source of the interaction indeed showed a reliable PD only for high-probability locations, t(17) = 3.15, p = .006 (see Figure 2, upper middle and lower left panels).2 One possible reason for not observing a PD for distrac- tors presented at the low-probability location might be that there are opposing lateralized suppression effects working at the same time (i.e., the spatial suppression of the high-probability location and the feature suppression of the distractor singleton at the opposing low-probability location). If the PD reflecting the suppression on high- probability location is larger than the PD reflecting the sup- pression on the distractor at the low-probability location, then it is possible that this larger PD overshadows the smaller distractor-generated PD. Note, however, it is also possible that the distractor at the low-probability location 1540 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 31, Number 10 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 2. Examples of the search displays in various conditions of Experiment 1. The dashed circles indicate the high-probability location. The display elements with solid outline indicate possible target and distractor singleton locations; the display elements with dashed outline indicate locations that never contained a target or distractor singleton. Each subpanel shows, per condition, a grand-averaged ERP recorded at PO7/PO8 and the lateralized difference waves (contra- minus ipsilateral). Green, red, and blue dashed squares indicate the time window that was used to calculate mean amplitude of early PD, PD, and N2pc, respectively. Note that, in the lower right panel, the ERP waves were plotted contralateral to the high-probability location, but the target was presented at the low-probability location. Thus, the N2pc was reversed here, suggesting a selection of the target. All waveforms were low-pass filtered at 30 Hz (half power cutoff, 24 dB) for visualization. does not generate a PD at all, as the distractor at this loca- tion generates a strong capture effect. Moreover, if the suppression is both anticipatory- and location-based and not elicited by the presence of spe- cific distractor information, we would expect a PD for the high-probability location even if no distractor single- ton is presented there. We analyzed this component separately for when the target appeared at this location (upper right panel of Figure 2), for when the distractor singleton appeared at this location (upper middle panel of Figure 2), and for when a neutral display element was presented at this location (no target nor distractor single- ton, upper left panel of Figure 2). A two-way ANOVA with within-subject factors Laterality (contra- vs. ipsilateral to the high-probability location) and Type of display ele- ment (distractor, target, or neutral display element) was conducted on average ERP amplitude within PD time win- dow. The results showed a main effect for Laterality, F(1, 17) = 7.96, p = .012, ηp 2 = .32, but not for Type of display element, F(2, 34) = 2.15, p = .13, ηp 2 = .11, and no inter- action was observed, F(2, 34) = 1.93, p = .161, ηp 2 = .1. The activity was more positive in the contralateral side for each condition, all ts ≥ 2.46, all ps ≤ .025, indicating that a PD was found regardless of which display element was pre- sented there. This suggests that the suppression we ob- served is completely spatial in origin. Note, however, as can be seen in Figure 3, the mean PD amplitude generated when a distractor was present at the high-probability loca- tion (1.58 μV) was slightly larger than the amplitude when a neutral display element presented at high-probability lo- cation (1.09 μV), t = 2.14, p = .047. This suggests that the presence of distracting information at this high-probability location generates additional suppression above and be- yond suppression that is already present when no dis- tracting information is present. There was no reliable difference when the target (1.43 μV) and the neutral dis- play element were presented at high-probability location, t = 1.2, p = .248. Wang et al. 1541 presented at the low-probability location (with the average peak latency of 205 msec) were significantly different, t(17) = 7.89, p < .001. It is clear that the N2pc for targets presented at the high-probability location is weaker than in the low-proba- bility location. If we use the same time period (±20 msec) for calculating the N2pc, as was used for targets presented at the low-probability location, then there N2pc is no longer reliable. This is probably due to the suppression (indexed by the PD) exerted at the high-probability loca- tion, which makes the N2pc less robust. Therefore, we used the peak point to calculate the mean N2pc ampli- tude for targets presented at the high-probability location. When we use this method, a reliable yet less robust N2pc was observed, as we have outlined above. Poststimulus: Early PD Consistent with the notion of anticipatory suppression of the high-probability location, we found evidence for an early PD representing an early modulation of the high- probability location in Experiment 1. A two-way ANOVA with within-subject factors Laterality (contra- vs. ipsilateral to the high-probability location) and Type of display ele- ment (distractor, target, or neutral display element) was conducted on average ERP amplitude within the early PD time window (74–114 msec; see Methods section for choice of time window). The results indicated a main significant effect for Laterality only, F(1, 17) = 4.94, p = .04, ηp 2 = .23, and no interaction across conditions (see Figure 2), suggesting that the early PD was present for the high-probability location regardless of whether a target, a distractor, or a neutral display element was presented. ERP Results in Experiment 2 (Baseline) In the baseline experiment, the target and distractor single- ton were equally likely presented at each location. Based on previous studies (Hickey et al., 2006), we expected an N2pc component for both the target and distractor single- ton. A two-way ANOVA with within-subject factors Laterality (contra- vs. ipsilateral to the singleton) and Lateralized item (target vs. distractor) was conducted. The results showed a significant main effect for Laterality, F(1, 17) = 5.01, p = .039, ηp 2 = .23 (see Figure 4), but no interaction suggesting that both target and distractor singletons generated an N2pc (Hickey et al., 2006). There was indeed a significant N2pc for targets, t(17) = 2.17, p = .045, and a trend in the same direction for distractors, t(17) = 1.75, p = .099. The same time window was chosen to analyze when a neutral display element was presented laterally (i.e., like what we did in Experiment 1). The results indicate that there was no difference between contralateral and ipsilateral sides, t < 1. It should be noted that, unlike in Experiment 1 (which had the high-probability location), in the current baseline experiment, no PD was observed in any condition. Figure 3. Upper panel shows mean PD amplitude for different items presented at high-probability location. Lower panel shows mean N2pc amplitude for target presented at high- and low-probability locations. Error bars denote within-subject 95% confidence intervals. Poststimulus: Target-elicited N2pc To determine whether there was attentional selection of the target and/or the distractor when presented at the low-probability location, we analyzed the N2pc toward the target or distractor singleton ( lower panels of Figure 2). A two-way ANOVA with within-subject factors Laterality (contra- vs. ipsilateral to the low-probability lo- cation) and Type of display element (distractor vs. target) was conducted on mean ERP amplitude within the N2pc time window (186–226 msec; see Methods section for choice of time window). Only the interaction was reli- able, F(1, 17) = 7.5, p = .014, ηp 2 = .31. Subsequent planned comparisons tracing the source of the inter- action showed a reliable N2pc for targets only, t(17) = 2.35, p = .031 (see Figure 2, lower panels). Furthermore, we also observed what seems to be a delayed N2pc (peaked at 294 msec) when the target was presented at high-probability location, t(17) = 1.88, p = .039 (one-tailed). This amplitude of the delayed N2pc for the target at the high-probability location did not differ from the amplitude of the N2pc for the target at the low-probability location, t < 1 (see Figure 3). We used a jackknife procedure (Kiesel, Miller, Jolicœur, & Brisson, 2008) to statistically test the difference between the N2pc latencies for targets presented at the high- versus low-probability location and obtained a reliable estimate of the numerical difference. The peak latencies between the delayed N2pc (with the average peak latency of 294 msec) for target presented at the high-probability location and the N2pc observed when the target was 1542 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 31, Number 10 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 4. Example of the search displays in various conditions of Experiment 2. The dashed circles indicate the high-probability location. The display elements with solid outline indicate possible target and distractor singleton locations; the display elements with dashed outline indicate locations that never contained a target or distractor singleton. Grand-averaged ERPs recorded at PO7/PO8 and their difference waves (contra- minus ipsilateral) for different conditions were presented in different subpanels. Blue dashed squares indicate the time window that was used to calculate mean amplitude of N2pc. All waveforms were low-pass filtered at 30 Hz (half power cutoff, 24 dB) using digital filtering. DISCUSSION Our data show that the reduction of attentional capture by a salient singleton is accompanied by prestimulus an- ticipatory suppression. Because participants have learned that the salient distractor is more likely to appear at one specific location, this location is suppressed as there is increased alpha power contralateral to this location relative to the ipsilateral location. Notably, alpha-band oscillations have been associated with neural inhibition ( Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010), serving as a general gating mechanism. Previous studies have reported increased alpha power at electrodes over ipsilateral visual cortex, representing the location where participants expected distractors to appear (Kelly, Lalor, Reilly, & Foxe, 2006). This was interpreted as evidence of active atten- tional suppression. It has been claimed that prestimulus alpha operates as a gating mechanism, allowing the sup- pression of distracting information (Moore & Armstrong, 2003). When the salient distractor was presented at the high- probability location, we found a PD, suggesting spatial suppression. Critically, following this PD, the attentional capture by salient distractor was attenuated, as no N2pc was observed toward that location. In the baseline exper- iment, when all locations were presented equally likely, there was no PD, and consequently, attention was cap- tured by the salient distractor as evidenced by an N2pc, consistent with Hickey et al. (2006). Notably a PD was also found when the target was presented at the high- probability location in Experiment 1, resulting in a delayed selection (i.e., delayed N2pc) of the target consistent with the behavioral data showing delayed RT when the target is presented at that location. Previous studies have shown that attentional suppres- sion of distractors is typically accompanied by a PD wave- form (Feldmann-Wüstefeld & Vogel, 2019; Cosman, Lowe, Zinke, Woodman, & Schall, 2018; Gaspelin & Luck, 2018a; Gaspar, Christie, Prime, Jolicœur, & McDonald, 2016; Gaspar & McDonald, 2014; Feldmann- Wüstefeld, Wykowska, & Schubö, 2013; Sawaki & Luck, 2013; Hickey et al., 2008). Observing a PD is attributed to the processing of distracting information and is as- sumed to reflect active suppression of this distractor infor- mation (van Moorselaar & Slagter, 2019; Gaspelin & Luck, 2018b; Burra & Kerzel, 2014; Gaspar & McDonald, 2014; Sawaki & Luck, 2013). For example, in a visual search study, Gaspelin and Luck (2018a) showed that, when the salient distractor was presented, it generated a PD, which was correlated with behavior that showed suppression. Gaspar et al. (2016) showed that a salient distractor (e.g., a red item between green background items) gener- ated a PD whereas a relatively low salient distractor (e.g., an orange item between red background items) did not result in a PD. Overall these findings indicate that a PD is only observed if a salient distractor is present and if its salience signal needs to be actively suppressed. The find- ings of the current study are dramatically different in this respect. Consistent with previous finding, we do find a PD when a salient distractor was present at the high- probability location (Figure 2, upper middle panel); yet, unlike previous studies, we also find a PD when neutral (background) elements were present (Figure 2, upper left panel) and even when the target was present at the high- probability location (Figure 2, upper right panel). In the latter case, the suppression signified by the PD resulted in a delayed N2pc and a delayed behavioral response to the target. It is possible that the suppression that we Wang et al. 1543 report here is different from the suppression that is ob- served when a salient distractor is present. The type of suppression reported here can be labeled as “proactive suppression,” in the sense that specific lo- cations within the spatial priority map are suppressed (Ferrante et al., 2018; Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a, 2018b, 2018c). This suppression is spatial and feature-blind, and happens regardless of what information is present at that location (whether it is a target, a salient distractor, or a neutral element). Consequently, also the neural correlates of such suppression (contralateral alpha enhancement, early PD, and PD) are observed in all these conditions. Critically, and consistent with the notion of proactive sup- pression, the enhanced alpha power contralateral to the high-probability location is observed prior to display onset. This indicates that the suppression is indeed proactive in the sense that the high-probability location is already suppressed before display onset. As argued, increased alpha-band oscillations are assumed to represent neural inhibition ( Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010), serving as a general attentional gating mechanism. Also, there is another suppression that is considered to be reactive in the sense that attention is first captured by the sa- lient distractor (even for the briefest moment) and then is immediately suppressed (Won, Kosoyan, & Geng, 2019). Depending on the task design, it is possible that the disengagement of attention can be so fast that there are virtually very little (or no) effects of the presence of the salient distractor (i.e., basically no attentional capture). It is similar to Moher and Egeth’s (2012) conception of what has been labeled as the “search and destroy” hypothesis. Moher and Egeth showed that participants that were instructed to inhibit a particular feature could only do so after attending to the location of the to-be-ignored feature. It should be noted that the PD that we observed when a distractor singleton was present at the high-probability location was slightly larger than the PD that we found for neutral elements presented at the high-probability loca- tion. This may suggest that the presence of distracting in- formation generates additional (retroactive) suppression above and beyond the proactive suppression that was induced through statistical learning. We also observed an early PD component (80–120 msec poststimulus) toward the high-probability location in all conditions regardless of whether a target, a salient dis- tractor, or a neutral element was presented at the high- probability location. An early PD (also termed “positivity posterior contralateral” or “lateralized P1”) often co-occurs with the later PD in displays with lateralized stimuli. Initially, Luck and colleagues (Luck & Kappenman, 2012; Luck, Heinze, Mangun, & Hillyard, 1990) found that the early PD (named “lateralized P1” in their study) was en- hanced when the target appeared at the attended hemi- field, suggesting that it may represent facilitation of early sensory processing. This is consistent with some studies suggesting that the early PD is related to initial processing of stimuli and/or is assumed to represent some low-level sensory imbalance between the two hemispheres. (Jannati, Gaspar, & McDonald, 2013; Fortier-Gauthier, Moffat, Dell’Acqua, McDonald, & Jolicœur, 2012). However, in the current study, the early PD was also found when nonsalient neutral display elements were presented at the high- probability location, indicating that the early PD does not represent early sensory processing of salience nor hemispheric imbalance but instead represents a signature of early suppression ( Weaver, van Zoest, & Hickey, 2017). The current study shows that participants learn to sup- press the location where the salient distractor is likely to appear. The pattern of results shows that relative to the high-probability location, there is prestimulus, contralat- eral alpha enhancement, an early PD, and the PD. The neural correlates were found regardless of whether a sa- lient distractor, a salient target, or a neutral element were presented at this location. These findings suggest that the location that is likely to contain a salient distractor is proactively suppressed through statistical learning. APPENDIX Behavior Results in Experiment 1 Trials on which the RTs were faster than 200 msec and slower than 2000 msec (2.17%) were excluded from analyses. Attentional Capture Effect Mean RTs and mean error rates are presented in Figure A1A. With Distractor condition (high-probability location, low- probability location, and no distractor) as a factor, a repeated-measures ANOVA on mean RTs showed a main effect, F(2, 34) = 32.09, p < .001, partial η2 = .65. Subsequent planned comparisons showed that there were significant attentional capture effects for a distractor presented at the high-probability location, t(17) = 4.26, p = .001, and the low-probability location, t(17) = 8.61, p < .001. Crucially, however, there was also a reliable dif- ference between the high- and low-probability locations, t(17) = 3.42, p = .003, suggesting that attentional cap- ture effect was reduced for trials in which the distractor appeared at a high-probability location. The results on error rates mimicked those for RTs. With Distractor condition (high-probability location, low- probability location, and no-distractor) as a factor, a repeated- measures ANOVA showed a main effect, F(2, 34) = 21.41, p < .001, partial η2 = .56. Subsequent planned comparisons showed that significant attentional capture effects were observed when a distractor singleton was pre- sented at the high-probability location, t(17) = 3.82, p = .001, and the low-probability location, t(17) = 5.74, p < .001. Importantly, however, there was also a reliable dif- ference between the high- and low-probability locations, t(17) = 3.35, p = .004. Taken together, these results suggest that attentional capture effect was reduced for trials in which the distractor singleton appeared in a 1544 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 31, Number 10 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure A1. The mean RTs (left) and the mean error rates (right) in (A) singleton distractor present condition and (B) singleton distractor absent (no-distractor) condition. Error bars denote within-subject 95% confidence intervals. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 high-probability relative to the low-probability location, a straightforward replication of Wang and Theeuwes (2018a, 2018b). Target at the High-probability Distractor Location To determine whether not only attentional capture but also the efficiency of selecting the target was affected, we examined RTs in the no-distractor condition. We ex- pected that, even when the distractor was absent, observers would respond more slowly toward a target when it was presented at the high-probability distractor location relative to the low-probability location. This is exactly what we found (see the left panel of Figure A1B): Participants were slower to respond when the target was presented at the high-probability distractor location rather than at the Figure A2. The spatial distribution of attentional capture effect by the means of RTs (left) and error rates (right). Here, Dist-0 refers to the high-probability distractor location, Dist-1 refers to the low-probability distractor location with one unit (90° polar angle) away from the high- probability distractor location, and so on. Error bars denote within-subject 95% confidence intervals. Wang et al. 1545 low-probability distractor location, t(17) = 3.73, p = .002, and the response was less accurate when the target was presented at the high-probability distractor location, t(17) = 2.15, p = .046. The Spatial Distribution of the Suppression Effect We examined RTs and error rates of the target when the distractor singleton appeared at different locations in re- lation to the distance from the high-probability location. If the effect has a spatial extent, the RT and the error rate are expected to scale with distance from this area. Thus, the distractor location was divided into three distances (dist-0, dist-1, dist-2)3 from the high-probability location. The mean RTs and mean error rates for these conditions are presented in Figure A2A. Repeated-measures ANOVAs on mean RTs and mean error rates both showed significant main effects for Distance, F(2, 34) = 11.57, p < .001, partial η2 = .41 and F(2, 34) = 7.15, p = .003, partial η2 = .3, respec- tively. Moreover, we fitted the data with a linear function and used their slopes to determine whether mean RTs and mean error rates changed with distance. The slopes were significantly larger than zero for mean RTs (15.4 msec per display element), t(17) = 4.43, p < .001, and mean error rates (0.8% per display element), t(17) = 3.73, p = .002, suggesting a spatial gradient (the distractor effect grew when the distractor was presented farther away from the high-probability distractor location). In the no-distractor condition, there was also a signifi- cant main effect for Distance, F(2, 34) = 7.64, p = .002, partial η2 = .31. The gradient effect was now reversed: With increased distance, the selection of the target be- came progressively more efficient, signified by a slope of −11.15 msec per display element that differed from zero, t(17) = 3.31, p = .004. This latter effect suggests spatial suppression that is not dependent on whether or not a distractor is present. Behavior Results in Experiment 2 Trials on which the RTs were faster than 200 msec and slower than 2000 msec (2.24%) were excluded from anal- yses. Mean RTs and mean error rates are presented in Figure A3. Paired-samples t test between different distrac- tor conditions (with vs. without distractor singleton) showed a significant capture effect, reflected by mean RTs, t(17) = 10.59, p < .001, and mean error rates, t(17) = 4.16, p = .001. Figure A3. The mean RTs (left) and the mean error rates (right) in different distractor condition. Error bars denote within- subjects 95% confidence intervals. 1546 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 31, Number 10 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Acknowledgments This research was supported by a China Scholarship Council (CSC) scholarship (201508330313) to Benchi Wang. Reprint requests should be sent to Benchi Wang, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands, or via e-mail: wangbenchi.swift@gmail.com. Notes 1. Because the target never appears at the high-probability lo- cation in the distractor present condition, it is possible that the effects reported are due to the target probability and not to the distractor probability. However, in a recent study, we showed that the probability of the target does not matter: the suppres- sion effect observed here and in previous studies (Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a, 2018b, 2018c) was solely due to the distrac- tor being presented more often in this one location (Failing et al., 2019). 2. When manually removing all the eye movement trials in Experiment 1, the results stay the same. 3. The dist-0 location refers to the high-probability distractor location; the dist-1 location means that the distractor was pre- sented next to the high-probability distractor location (90° polar angle); the dist-2 location refers to 180° from the high-probability distractor location. REFERENCES Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top–down versus bottom–up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 437–443. Failing, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2018). Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 514–538. Failing, M., Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2019). Spatial suppression due to statistical regularities is driven by distractor suppression not by target activation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81, 1405–1414. Feldmann-Wüstefeld, T., Uengoer, M., & Schubö, A. (2015). You see what you have learned. Evidence for an interrelation of associative learning and visual selective attention. Psychophysiology, 52, 1483–1497. Feldmann-Wüstefeld, T., & Vogel, E. K. (2019). Neural evidence for the contribution of active suppression during working memory filtering. Cerebral Cortex, 29, 529–543. Feldmann-Wüstefeld, T., Wykowska, A., & Schubö, A. (2013). Context heterogeneity has a sustained impact on attention deployment: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Psychophysiology, 50(8), 722–733. Ferrante, O., Patacca, A., Di Caro, V., Della Libera, C., Santandrea, E., & Chelazzi, L. (2018). Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering. Cortex, 102, 67–95. Fortier-Gauthier, U., Moffat, N., Dell’Acqua, R., McDonald, J. J., & Jolicœur, P. (2012). Contralateral cortical organisation of information in visual short-term memory: Evidence from lateralized brain activity during retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1748–1758. Gaspar, J. M., Christie, G. J., Prime, D. J., Jolicœur, P., & McDonald, J. J. (2016). Inability to suppress salient distractors predicts low visual working memory capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 113, 3693–3698. Gaspar, J. M., & McDonald, J. J. (2014). Suppression of salient objects prevents distraction in visual search. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 5658–5666. Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018a). Combined attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496. Burra, N., & Kerzel, D. (2014). The distractor positivity (Pd) signals lowering of attentional priority: Evidence from event-related potentials and individual differences. Psychophysiology, 51, 685–696. electrophysiological and behavioral evidence for the suppression of salient distractors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 1265–1280. Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018b). The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 79–92. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit Groppe, D. M., Makeig, S., & Kutas, M. (2009). Identifying learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 28–71. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top–down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10, 360–365. Cohen, M. X. (2014). Analyzing neural time series data: Theory and practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cosman, J. D., Lowe, K. A., Zinke, W., Woodman, G. F., & Schall, J. D. (2018). Prefrontal control of visual distraction. Current Biology, 28, 414.e3–420.e3. Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 9–21. de Vries, I. E., van Driel, J., & Olivers, C. N. (2017). Posterior α EEG dynamics dissociate current from future goals in working memory-guided visual search. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 1591–1603. Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 269–297. Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225–234. reliable independent components via split-half comparisons. Neuroimage, 45, 1199–1211. Hickey, C., Di Lollo, V., & McDonald, J. J. (2008). Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 760–775. Hickey, C., McDonald, J. J., & Theeuwes, J. (2006). Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 604–613. Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 194–203. Jannati, A., Gaspar, J. M., & McDonald, J. J. (2013). Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1713–1730. Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: Gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 186. Jiang, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2001). Selective attention modulates implicit learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 54, 1105–1124. Wang et al. 1547 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Kayser, J., & Tenke, C. E. (2015). Issues and considerations for using the scalp surface Laplacian in EEG/ERP research: A tutorial review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97, 189–209. Kelly, S. P., Lalor, E. C., Reilly, R. B., & Foxe, J. J. (2006). Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3844–3851. Kiesel, A., Miller, J., Jolicœur, P., & Brisson, B. (2008). Measurement of ERP latency differences: A comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. Psychophysiology, 45, 250–274. Klimesch, W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, 61–100. Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 169–195. Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 606–617. Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: An open- source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 213. Luck, S. J., Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 528–542. Luck, S. J., & Kappenman, E. S. (2012). ERP components and selective attention. In E. S. Kappenman & S. J. Luck (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (pp. 295–327). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164, 177–190. Moher, J., & Egeth, H. E. (2012). The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1590–1605. Moore, T., & Armstrong, K. M. (2003). Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature, 421, 370–373. Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 9. Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., & Echallier, J. F. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 184–187. Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. J. (2010). Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1455–1470. Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. J. (2013). Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 296–301. Theeuwes, J. (1991). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 184–193. Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606. Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135, 77–99. Theeuwes, J. (2018). Visual selection: Usually fast and automatic; seldom slow and volitional. Journal of Cognition, 1, 21. Theeuwes, J. (2019). Goal-driven, stimulus-driven, and history- driven selection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 97–101. van Driel, J., Gunseli, E., Meeter, M., & Olivers, C. N. L. (2017). Local and interregional alpha EEG dynamics dissociate between memory for search and memory for recognition. Neuroimage, 149, 114–128. van Moorselaar, D., & Slagter, H. A. (2019). Learning what is irrelevant or relevant: Expectations facilitate distractor inhibition and target facilitation through distinct neural mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience. https://doi.org/ 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0593-19.2019 Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018a). Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44, 13–17. Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018b). How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top–down suppression. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 860–870. Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018c). Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture independent of search strategy. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 1763–1774. Weaver, M. D., van Zoest, W., & Hickey, C. (2017). A temporal dependency account of attentional inhibition in oculomotor control. Neuroimage, 147, 880–894. Won, B.-Y., Kosoyan, M., & Geng, J. J. (2019). Evidence for second-order singleton suppression based on probabilistic expectations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 125–138. Worden, M. S., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., & Simpson, G. V. (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, RC63. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / 3 1 1 0 1 5 3 5 1 8 6 0 8 1 1 / / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 4 3 3 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 1548 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 31, Number 10Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image
Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited image

Descargar PDF