INFORME
The Pandemic in Words: Tracking Fast Semantic
Changes via a Large-Scale Word Association Task
Julieta Laurino1,2, Simon De Deyne3, Álvaro Cabana4,5*, and Laura Kaczer1,2*
1Instituto de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Neurociencias (IFIBYNE)-CONICET, Buenos Aires, Argentina
2Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
3Computational Cognitive Science Lab, Complex Human Data Hub, Universidad de Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
4Instituto de Fundamentos y Métodos y Centro de Investigación Básica en Psicología (CIBPsi), Facultad de Psicología,
Universidad de la República, Montevideo, Uruguay
5Centro Interdisciplinario en Ciencia de Datos y Aprendizaje Automático (CICADA), Universidad de la República,
Montevideo, Uruguay
* Equal contribution.
Palabras clave: diachronic change, word associations, semantic change, COVID-19, semantic
semejanza, redes
ABSTRACTO
Most words have a variety of senses that can be added, removed, or altered over time.
Understanding how they change across different contexts and time periods is crucial for
revealing the role of language in social and cultural evolution. In this study we aimed to explore
the collective changes in the mental lexicon as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Nosotros
performed a large-scale word association experiment in Rioplatense Spanish. The data were
obtained in December 2020, and compared with responses previously obtained from the Small
World of Words database (SWOW-RP, Cabana et al., 2023). Three different word-association
measures detected changes in a word’s mental representation from Precovid to Covid. Primero,
significantly more new associations appeared for a set of pandemic-related words. These new
associations can be interpreted as incorporating new senses. Por ejemplo, the word ‘isolated’
incorporated direct associations with ‘coronavirus’ and ‘quarantine’. Segundo, when analyzing
the distribution of responses, we observed a greater Kullback-Leibler divergence (es decir., relative
entropy) between the Precovid and Covid periods for pandemic words. De este modo, some words
(p.ej., ‘protocol’, or ‘virtual’) changed their overall association patterns due to the COVID-19
pandemic. Finalmente, using semantic similarity analysis, we evaluated the changes between the
Precovid and Covid periods for each cue word’s nearest neighbors and the changes in their
similarity to certain word senses. We found a larger diachronic difference for pandemic cues
where polysemic words like ‘immunity’ or ‘trial’ increased their similarity to sanitary/health
words during the Covid period. We propose that this novel methodology can be expanded to
other scenarios of fast diachronic semantic changes.
INTRODUCCIÓN
Language is not set in stone; it is constantly evolving to accommodate new ideas, technolo-
gies, and social change. Throughout our lives we are exposed to situations that require a
change in the meaning of the words we already know. Por ejemplo, we have recently learned
un acceso abierto
diario
Citación: Laurino, J., De Deyne, S.,
Cabana, Á., & Kaczer, l. (2023). El
Pandemic in Words: Tracking Fast
Semantic Changes via a Large-Scale
Word Association Task. Mente abierta:
Discoveries in Cognitive, 7, 221–239.
https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00081
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00081
Recibió: 13 Enero 2023
Aceptado: 29 Abril 2023
Conflicto de intereses: Los autores
declare no conflict of interest.
Autor correspondiente:
Laura Kaczer
laurakaczer@gmail.com
Derechos de autor: © 2023
Instituto de Tecnología de Massachusetts
Publicado bajo Creative Commons
Atribución 4.0 Internacional
(CC POR 4.0) licencia
La prensa del MIT
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
/
mi
d
tu
oh
pag
metro
i
/
yo
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
F
/
d
oh
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
oh
pag
metro
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
oh
pag
metro
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
pag
d
/
.
i
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
that ‘cloud’ also makes reference to data storage over the internet, and a ‘bug’ could refer to an
error in a computer script. These new meanings did not replace earlier ones but extended the
range of applications for these words ( Wilkins, 1996). This process is evident in the case of
polysemic words: Por ejemplo, the word ‘run’ has up to 35 different related senses [p.ej., "el
athlete runs the race,” “the politician runs for election,” or “the car runs on petrol”; Parks et al.
(1998); Rodd (2020)]. It is possible to incorporate new meanings and modify the emotional
value of certain words according to an individual’s life experiences (Gentsch et al., 2018).
Por lo tanto, memory for words is malleable and different aspects of a word’s meaning are sub-
ject to being updated or modified (Laurino et al., 2022). En el presente estudio, we are interested
in addressing the plasticity of lexico-semantic representations in a large-scale and ecologically
valid experiment in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic.
Regarding collective language change, in diachronic (or historical) linguistics, semantic
change refers to any change in the meaning(s) of a word over time (Hamilton et al., 2016).
Most words have a variety of senses, which can be added, removed, or altered over time while
the form remains constant. Por ejemplo, the word ‘gay’ shifted from meaning “cheerful” or
“frolicsome” to referring to homosexuality, and the word ‘awful’ underwent a process of
pejoration, as it shifted from meaning “full of awe” to meaning “terrible or appalling” (Simpson
et al., 1997). Like any linguistic change, a semantic change is usually slow, not acquired simul-
taneously by all members of a speech community, and is difficult to predict ( Wilkins, 1996).
Sin embargo, desde 2020 we have been experiencing the COVID-19 pandemic, one of the most
influential global events in modern human history, which also implies a great deal of linguistic
change in a short period of time. In just a few months, some already existing low frequency
words became commonplace (p.ej., ‘quarantine’, ‘immunity’) and others such as ‘bubble’ have
acquired new senses (“a social bubble”). De este modo, the pandemic offers a unique opportunity to
quantify the drift of word concepts (see Carrillo et al., 2015) that could eventually lead to a
semantic change. De este modo, in this study, we aimed to explore the collective changes in the mental
lexicon as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Sin embargo, how do we best capture
these changes in word meanings in a large group of people? Several studies have derived word
embeddings (such as LSA or word2vec) through known historical changes (p.ej., Garg et al.,
2018; Hamilton et al., 2016; Stella et al., 2020). Sin embargo, one of the main limitations of nat-
ural language-based approaches to meaning is that they only capture a subset of semantic
relations that humans know (Thompson et al., 2020). Several studies have shown that they
capture the experiential structure of the word only partially. Psychological measurements of
meaning can complement purely text-based approaches as they tend to capture experiential,
multimodal aspects of semantic representations. Por ejemplo, a comparison of text-based
models with human word associations showed that the latter capture both perceptual (visual)
and affective properties that are only partly encoded in language (De Deyne et al., 2020). Este
paper will investigate whether word associations from the multilingual Small World of Words
can provide new insight into diachronic semantic changes.
To investigate semantic change, we will consider the meaning of words embedded in seman-
tic graphs. This follows the seminal work of Collins and Quillian (1969), que representa
semantic memory as a network, or graph, in which nodes represent words, concepts or specific
characteristics connected to each other. Desde esta perspectiva, the meaning of a word can be
understood as long as its relationship with the other words in the semantic network is studied. En
semantic networks, two concepts are semantically related if they are together or close in the
network. en este estudio, semantic networks are obtained from the data derived through word asso-
ciation tasks. These are apparently simple experiments: a word (es decir., the cue) is presented and the
subject has to respond with the first word that comes to mind [es decir., the response; see Nelson et al.
MENTE ABIERTA: Descubrimientos en ciencia cognitiva
222
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
/
mi
d
tu
oh
pag
metro
i
/
yo
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
F
/
d
oh
i
/
i
.
/
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
oh
pag
metro
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
oh
pag
metro
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
pag
d
/
.
i
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
(2000)]. Despite their simplicity, they constitute a powerful methodological tool to study the
internal representations and the processes involved in the meaning of words (De Deyne &
Storms, 2008a) since that associations are free from the basic demands of natural language com-
munication (Steyvers et al., 2006). Relative to other tasks, the word association technique pro-
vides us with a more general and unbiased approach to measure meaning (Deese, 1965; Mollin,
2009) and provides access to common sense and experiential meaning (Borge-Holthoefer &
Arenas, 2009; De Deyne et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2022). This means that a variety of stimuli
can be used as cues, regardless of their part-of-speech or how abstract or concrete they are.
Tomados juntos, these properties make word associations a useful tool to study words’ internal
representations and to determine the plasticity of the lexical-semantic system. En este sentido, el
“Small World of Words” project (SWOW, De Deyne et al., 2019) is a large-scale collaborative
study that has produced sizable norms of free association in languages such as English, Dutch,
Mandarin and Iberian Spanish. This database was also recently generated for the Rioplatense
Spanish variant, containing more than 13,000 palabras, collected from more than 60,000 partícipe-
ipants in Argentina and Uruguay (Cabana et al., 2023), where we focused our study.
Until now, semantic change has mostly been addressed using word embeddings in large
diachronic text corpora against long-term historical changes (p.ej., Hamilton et al., 2016).
Although powerful, we suggest that these methods lack the psychological foundation available
for word associations which take into account individual and demographic information
directly (p.ej., De Deyne et al., 2017). In this study our aim is to characterize the use of word
associations as a valuable tool for understanding the fast evolution of a word’s meaning. Nosotros
focused on the changes caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, using a large-scale word asso-
ciation task that already had data prior to the pandemic, Small World of Words, SWOW-RP
(Cabana et al., 2023). Primero, we analyzed if certain pandemic-related words (p.ej., ‘virus’, ‘vac-
cine’, ‘mask’, ‘corona’, ‘contact’) drift from their relatively static associations to a new set of
linked words. Segundo, we asked whether there was an increase in response variability, as cer-
tain words might have shifted their prevalent associations, as well as incorporated new senses.
De este modo, we compared the distribution of responses for each cue between the two periods (Pre-
covid and Covid), using a KL divergence measure. Finalmente, we captured and analyzed semantic
shifts by obtaining network-based similarity estimates using the word association data. Para esto
purpose, we evaluated changes in each cue word’s nearest neighbors and changes in their
similarity to certain word senses. The possibility of analyzing natural linguistic changes caused
by the environment is useful for determining the plasticity of the lexical-semantic system. Nosotros
aim to demonstrate this in the context of COVID-19, but propose that this methodology could
be extended to address linguistic changes in general in a short time.
MÉTODOS
Participantes
All participants in this study are part of and included in the Rioplatense Small World of Words
datos (SWOW-RP, Cabana et al., 2023). The following section summarizes the demographic
properties of SWOW-RP participants in general and participants who were presented with
the COVID-19 cue set in particular.
Participants were recruited online, using a crowd-sourced approach that relied on social
media, e-mail, and university websites. Recruitment was oriented towards Rioplatense Native
Speakers. The participants consisted of 61,106 volunteers, of whom 51,043 (84%) identified as
femenino, 9,464 (15%) identified as male, y 599 (1%) responded using the unspecified gender
categoría. The average age was 38 años (DE = 15). Besides gender and age, we also collected
MENTE ABIERTA: Descubrimientos en ciencia cognitiva
223
yo
D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d
F
r
oh
metro
h
t
t
pag
:
/
/
d
i
r
mi
C
t
.
metro
i
t
.
/
mi
d
tu
oh
pag
metro
i
/
yo
a
r
t
i
C
mi
–
pag
d
F
/
d
oh
i
/
i
.
/
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
oh
pag
metro
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
oh
pag
metro
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
pag
d
.
/
i
F
b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t
t
oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
Mesa 1.
Participant’s demography after preprocessing steps.
Precovid, norte = 17,263
Covid, norte = 4,181
Gender
Female
Male
X
Age
Native language
Uruguay Rioplatense
Argentina Rioplatense
Argentina Cordobés
Argentina Nor-oriental-guaraní
Educación
None
Primary
Secondary
Universidad
Posgraduate
13,834 (80%)
3,322 (19%)
107 (0.6%)
37 (14)
10,894 (63%)
6,231 (36%)
40 (0.2%)
17 (<0.1%)
9 (<0.1%)
232 (1.3%)
6,285 (36%)
9,063 (52%)
1,674 (9.7%)
3,287 (79%)
846 (20%)
48 (1.1%)
40 (13)
1,003 (24%)
3,069 (74%)
48 (1.2%)
19 (0.5%)
5 (0.1%)
54 (1.3%)
1,318 (32%)
2,283 (55%)
521 (12%)
information about the native language of the participants. Most people were native Argentinian
(51%) or Uruguayan Rioplatense speakers (42%), with Argentinean Cordobés (1%) and Argen-
tinean Nor-oriental-guaraní (1%) speakers as the following largest groups represented in the
data. Regarding education, most participants indicated that their highest level was secondary
school (41%) or university (48%).
For the first two analyses, we filtered the database to include only the target and control
cues and their corresponding responses from each period, resulting in a smaller number of
participants. The final participant demographics in these two analyses (after preprocessing,
see Cabana et al., 2023 for full details) by time period (Precovid and Covid) is shown in
Table 1. As seen from Table 1, the Precovid and Covid cohorts were comparable, except
for the regional language variant: There were more speakers from Argentina in the second
cohort. We will return to this point in the discussion.
Materials
Precovid group. For SWOW-RP, stimulus materials (cue words) for the Precovid group were
constructed using a snowball sampling method as in De Deyne et al. (2019). The final set
of cue words consisted of 13,546 Spanish cues.
Covid group. A subset of cues presented during Precovid were selected as stimuli for a follow-
up during the Covid pandemic. Cue words for the Covid time-period consisted of 664 Spanish
cues that were either target, control or filler Spanish words. A description of each cue type is
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
224
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
Table 2.
Cue words presented during the Covid time period.
Cue type
control
N
150 Do not present any evident relationship with the pandemic.
Description
Examples
rain, gasoline, dragon, wheel
pandemic
107
Incorporated a new sense or modified its more frequent sense since pandemic.
bubble, alcohol, contact, virus
emotion
119
Related to emotions and that may have been affected during the pandemic.
anxiety, pain, funeral, postpone
routine
108
Related to aspects of the routine that were affected by the confinement.
bar, job, tourism, screen
filler
180 Do not present any evident relationship with the pandemic.
decade, town, start, basketball
given in Table 2. The target cues consisted of pandemic, emotion and routine words and were
selected with the criteria of implying an association with the COVID-19 pandemic and being
present in the SWOW database. Candidate words were screened by the authors taking into
account their frequency in newspapers and conversations in social media since the start of the
pandemic. Control and filler cues were selected using the snowball procedure described by
Cabana et al. (2023) and by checking that they did not present any evident relationship with
the pandemics. Importantly, control words were included for comparison with target cues,
constituting a baseline of random semantic drift whereas fillers were included to reduce the
priming effects among target cues.
As we aimed to ensure that the participants knew all cues well, we selected words relatively
common in Rioplatense Spanish (with a word frequency greater than 0.03 per million words,
determined with the EsPal Latin American Spanish subtitle tokens (Duchon et al., 2013). The
imageability, familiarity and concreteness was balanced across the different groups of cue
types (see Appendix, Table 3).
Procedure
General procedure. The SWOW-RP data collection is identical to Cabana et al. (2023), which
follows procedures for similar data in Dutch (SWOW-NL, De Deyne & Storms, 2008b; De
Deyne et al., 2013) and English (SWOW-EN, De Deyne et al., 2019). A word association task
was collected in a cross-sectional sample at two different time points before the COVID-19 pan-
demic (Precovid) and during the pandemic (Covid). The Precovid period spanned from Decem-
ber 2013 to March 2020, while the Covid period spanned from December 2020 to April 2022.
Participants were instructed that a word would appear on the screen and they were asked to
respond with the first three words that came to mind. If they could not think of any further
responses, they could indicate this by pressing the “No More Responses” button. If a word
was unknown, they were asked to select the “Unknown Word” button. They were also
instructed to respond only to the word displayed on top of the screen (not to their previous
responses) and to avoid typing full sentences as responses. Each stimulus appearing on the
screen was followed by a form consisting of three text fields, one for the first (R1), second
(R2), and third (R3) response. Once a field was completed by pressing ‘enter’ or clicking a
button, that entry could no longer be changed.
Precovid group. During the Precovid period, each participant was presented with a list of 14 to
18 stimuli. The stimuli presented were selected randomly from those cues with the fewest
responses in the current iteration.
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
225
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
Covid group. During the Covid period, each participant was presented with a list of 20 stimuli
based on random selection of 4 target cues (pandemic, emotion and routine cues), 4 control
and 12 filler cues for each participant. The small proportion of target cues as opposed to con-
trol and filler cues was used to prevent participants from perceiving the aim of the experiment
and biasing the data.
Data Preprocessing
Exclusions. We excluded participants from the dataset if they did not meet the a priori criteria
used in SWOW-RP. The exclusion criteria are based on Cabana et al. (2023) and are summa-
rized below. First, we excluded participants where more than 30% of the responses consisted
of short sentences (494 or 1% of participants). We excluded participants for whom fewer than
80% of the responses were unique (47 or 0.08% of participants). We also removed participants
with fewer than 60% of their responses appearing on a Spanish word list (944 or 2% of the
participants). Finally, participants who indicated that they did not know more than 60% of
their words were excluded (917 or 2% of participants).
In addition to the participant’s exclusion criteria used in SWOW-RP, we filtered the data-
base to include only the target and control cues from each time period. As our study aims to
compare the responses of pandemic-related words between the Precovid and the Covid time
periods, we included 60 participants per time period (120 participants per cue) for target and
control cues. In the cases where more than 60 responses were available for a cue, we prefer-
entially included data from native speakers, first Argentinean and Uruguayan Rioplatense
speakers and then Argentinean Cordobés and Nor-oriental-guaraní speakers. Some target cues
were not included in the final dataset because they were not presented during the Precovid
time period due to a mistake. These were three words from the emotion set, eight pandemic
and four routine target cues. The final dataset consisted of 21,444 participants (17,263 Preco-
vid and 4,181 Covid), 469 cues and 168,840 responses.
Normalization. Consistent with the SWOW-RP data in Cabana et al. (2023), the data were
normalized in several steps. We first removed tags, quotes, final punctuation, double spaces
and other unintended symbols. We manually checked cues and responses that were mis-
spelled multiple times and corrected them. Next, taking into account the difference in partic-
ipants’ demography in terms of Native Language between the two time periods (Precovid and
Covid), we included a step for consistency purposes during the analysis. As some words
appeared as responses in multiple forms corresponding to regional variations (e.g., pileta
and piscina, meaning swimming pool), in such cases, we replaced the Rioplatense Argentin-
ean lexical variants with their Rioplatense Uruguayan counterparts (10 responses).
Data Analysis
All data was analyzed with R (R Core Team, 2021). We performed three exploratory analyses
to detect and characterize diachronic changes in a predefined set of words (pandemic, control,
emotion, routine).
We first analyzed the proportion of new responses within the Covid group, in order to deter-
mine the appearance of new associations for target cues, so that we can effectively confirm
that a diachronic change occurred.
Second, we compared the distribution of responses for each cue between the two time
periods to characterize the semantic change. To do that, we calculated the Kullback–Leibler
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
226
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
.
/
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
(KL) divergence, a non-symmetric measure of the directed divergence between two probability
distributions.
Third, we obtained similarity estimates using the word association data and subsequently
determined the cue words’ semantic shifts by using the similarity ratio of each cue and its
nearest neighbors in both periods as proposed in Rodriguez et al. (2023). Further details on
this analysis are presented in the Results section.
Lastly, and as a first approach to a new measurement, we calculated for both periods the
similarity between each cue and paradigm words that represented the core of certain word’s
senses (e.g., ‘immunity’ could be related to the resistance to a pathogen or to diplomatic
immunity). Thus, we were able to determine the shift in similarity from one sense to another
across time.
Regarding statistical analysis, the proportion of new responses for each cue was analyzed
fitting a generalized linear model using a binomial family and the logit link. KL divergence
data was analyzed fitting a linear model using a normal distribution. The similarity ratio
was analyzed fitting a generalized linear model using a gamma family and the inverse link,
as the data were continuous, non-negative and positive-skewed. The significance of a factor
was determined with an F-Test or a Chi-square Test, as appropriate. Pairwise post-hoc analysis
was performed using the emmeans package (Lenth, 2022) and applied the Tukey correction to
the p-values for comparisons. The significance of the shift in similarities between periods was
determined with a permutation test by determining the shift of each cue to 1000 random pairs
of paradigm words. These were selected from the similarity matrix and each one had a cosine
similarity comparable (i.e., ±0.05) to the similarity between the pairs originally used for each
sense.
RESULTS
Proportion of New Responses
As a first step to detect whether the proposed methodology could be used to detect language
changes, we determined whether we could reliably find new senses associated with known
target words. If that was the case, new senses would be represented in our data as new
responses to a cue. Thus, we determined the proportion of new responses for each cue. A
response was considered as new if it presented zero frequency at the Precovid time period
and a frequency bigger than zero at the Covid time period.
The proportion of new responses by cue was analyzed using a generalized linear
model that included Cue type (levels: control, pandemic, emotion and routine) as fixed
factor. Statistical analysis revealed a significant effect of Cue type [χ2(3) = 112.21, p <
0.001]. Pairwise simple contrasts revealed that control cues differ significantly from pan-
demic cues (p = < 0.001, Δ = −0.128) and from routine cues (p = < 0.001, Δ = 0.130),
indicating that pandemic cues presented a greater proportion of new responses than con-
trol cues, while routine cues presented a smaller proportion of new responses than control
cues (Figure 1).
In order to visualize some of these new associations, we selected five pandemic cues with
the highest proportion of new responses (see Appendix, Table 4 for a list of the top 10 pan-
demic cues) and created a semantic network. For each cue, their adjacent vertices (or nodes)
were included in the network. To determine adjacent vertices, only outgoing pandemic cue’s
edges (or links) were considered, that is, words that were given as a response both during the
Precovid and the Covid periods when the pandemic cue was presented. The edges between
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
227
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
Figure 1. Proportion of new responses for each cue by cue type. Horizontal lines indicate the
mean of each cue type and error bars the standard error of the mean. Stars indicate statistical differ-
ences with the control cue type.
the adjacent vertices were also included. Words with similar meaning are presented in close
proximity by calculating the pairwise cosine similarity and using UMAP (McInnes et al., 2018)
to extract a two-dimensional solution that respects both the local and global structure of the
similarity space. To reduce visual clutter, an adjacent vertex was included if it occurred at least
two times in the entire dataset. Finally, old responses (only given during the Precovid period),
new responses (only given during the Covid period) and shared responses (given both in Pre-
covid and Covid periods) were determined and are shown in different colors. As Figure 2D
shows, the word ‘aislado’ (isolated) presented new associations during the Covid time period
that were tightly related to the pandemic (such as ‘quarantine’, ‘coronavirus’, shown in red).
We can observe a similar behavior for the words ‘virtual’, ‘protocol’, ‘confinement’ and ‘virus’
(Figure 2).
Changes in Response Distribution
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
In the next section we compare how the meaning of cues changes over time by comparing
their response distributions. This would not only consider the new associations but also the
changes in response dominance (e.g., it might be possible that during Covid times the cue
‘alcohol’ led mostly to sanitary associations, reducing the connecting strength to drinking asso-
ciations). To do so, we calculated the Kullback–Leibler (KL) divergence between the Precovid
and the Covid probability distributions of responses for each cue.
We used the Philentropy package (Drost, 2018) to compute the KL divergence for each
cue in nats (natural units). KL divergence values were analyzed using a generalized linear
model that included the fixed factor Cue type (levels: Control, Pandemic, Emotion and
Routine). Statistical analysis revealed a significant effect of Cue type [F(3, 465) = 9.59,
p < 0.001]. Pairwise simple contrasts revealed that control cues differ significantly from
pandemic cues (p = 0.02, Δ = −0.209) and routine cues (p = 0.02, Δ = 0.206), indicating
that pandemic cues presented a greater KL divergence between Precovid and Covid
periods than control cues, while routine cues presented a smaller KL divergence between
Precovid and Covid time periods than control cues (Figure 3), suggesting stable meaning
compared to controls. See Appendix, Table 5 for a list of the top 10 pandemic cues with
highest KL divergence.
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
228
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Figure 2. Network visualization of five pandemic cues with the highest proportion of new responses: ‘protocolo’ (protocol) (A), ‘virus’
(virus) (B), ‘virtual’ (virtual) (C), ‘aislado’ (isolated) (D) and ‘encierro’ (confinement) (E) with their corresponding adjacent vertices. The
node size corresponds to the percentage the response was given for the pandemic cue. The color is based on the difference of the percentage
the response was given during the Precovid and during the Covid periods (responses specific to the Precovid period are shown in blue and
responses specific to the Covid period are shown in red).
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
229
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
Figure 3. Kullback–Leibler divergence measure between Precovid and Covid response distribu-
tions for each cue, by cue type. Horizontal lines indicate the mean of each cue type and error bars
the standard error of the mean. ** p < .01.
Analyzing Semantic Shifts through Similarity
So far, we determined that pandemic cue words present a greater proportion of new responses
and a greater divergence in the distribution of responses between Precovid and Covid time
periods. The third measure we present makes use of the fact that word associations can be used
to produce good predictions of similarity judgements (De Deyne et al., 2019). Here, we propose
that changes in a word’s meaning can be captured by changes in its similarity with other words.
First, we asked whether cue words would present more changes in their most similar words
(i.e., nearest neighbors) compared to control words. Second, we explored whether the changes
detected reflect a drift to a pandemic/sanitary/health dimension.
As we wanted to measure the similarity between the words during Precovid and during
Covid we constructed two different datasets: the Precovid set that only included the responses
given in the Precovid period (for the target and control cues); and the Covid set that only
included the responses given in the Covid period (for the target and control cues). In contrast
to previous analyses, we used the entire SWOW-RP data comprising 56,379 participants,
13,468 cues and 2,508,660 responses. In other words, both datasets shared the responses
given for all the cues, except the ones that were target or control. Then, we use these two word
association datasets to construct networks that connect associated words and assume that sim-
ilarity between two words reflects not only the immediate neighbors of a word but also indirect
paths, consistent with a spreading activation mechanism (Collins & Loftus, 1975). More pre-
cisely, we implemented a spreading activation mechanism as a decaying random walk process
(see De Deyne et al., 2019). The procedure is summarized here. We first constructed a cue-by-
cue count matrix, where elements reflected how often each cue was given as an association in
response to each other cue. Rows of this matrix were normalized to sum to 1 and the positive
pointwise mutual information (PPMI) transformation was applied, obtaining a transformed
matrix P. Then, the distributional representation of each cue was calculated as a matrix G
by simulating an attenuated random walk over P using the equation: G = (I – αP)−1, where
I is the identity matrix, and α a decay parameter set to 0.75, the default value used in previous
work (e.g., Cabana et al., 2023; De Deyne et al., 2019). Finally, we calculate the similarity
between two words as the cosine between rows of G. In this context, the similarity between
pairs of words is captured by the distributional overlap of the direct and indirect paths they
share. This procedure was performed for the two databases separately, obtaining two similarity
matrices: one for the Precovid and one for the Covid time periods.
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
230
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Figure 4. Ratio of Covid to Precovid similarities between the words ‘strain’ (A), ‘immunity’ (B) and ‘trial’ (C) and their nearest neighbors in
the Precovid period (blue) in the Covid period (pink) and shared between the two time periods (green). When the ratio is large, it means that
word is more related to the Covid sense of the cue than to the Precovid one. The y-axis indicates the word’s rank in terms of distance to a ratio
of 1 (dashed vertical line) from the most distinct at the top to the less distinct at the bottom.
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
231
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
In order to determine whether cue words present more changes in their most similar words
(i.e., nearest neighbors) compared to control words, we first determined the top 20 nearest
neighbors (i.e., the 20 most similar words) of each cue for each time period. This resulted
in each cue having nearest neighbors in the Precovid period, in the Covid period and shared
between the two time periods. Then we calculated the ratio of Covid to Precovid similarities
between each word and all of its nearest neighbors (cosine similarity ratio). Hence, a ratio of 1
would indicate that the words have the same cosine similarity in both datasets (Precovid and
Covid), while departures from 1 would indicate they are more similar at one time period. The
cosine similarity ratio was analyzed using a generalized linear mixed model that included Cue
type (levels: control, pandemic, emotion and routine) and Neighbor type (levels: precovid,
covid, and shared) as fixed factors, and random intercepts for cues. Statistical analysis revealed
a significant interaction between Cue type and Neighbor type [χ2(3) = 90.58, p < 0.001]. Pair-
wise simple contrasts revealed that control cues differ significantly only from pandemic cues
and only for the covid Neighbor type (p = 0.015, Δ = 0.041), indicating that pandemic cues
presented higher similarity ratios for covid neighbors than control cues.
Consistent with the rationale of this methodology, we show here the results corresponding
to three of the cue words with the highest mean ratio deviation from 1 (calculated as the abso-
lute difference between the similarity ratio and 1): ‘strain’ and ‘immunity’ and ‘trial’ (Figure 4).
See Appendix, Table 6 for a list of the top 10 pandemic cues with the highest ratio deviation.
In Figure 4A, we observe that the word ‘strain’ presents mostly Covid nearest neighbors
concerning infectious diseases (‘genetics’, ‘mutation’, ‘diseases’, ‘microorganisms’, ‘antibiotics’).
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
.
/
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
Figure 5. Semantic similarity ratings to each sense for the pandemic cues ‘strain’ (A), ‘immunity’
(B) and ‘trial’ (C), for the Precovid and Covid time period.
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
232
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
In contrast, the dominant terms in the Precovid period were related to wine (‘cultivation’, ‘seed’,
‘harvest’, ‘Mendoza’). In Figure 4B for the case of ‘immunity’, we can distinguish a set of
specific terms in the Covid period that are health-related (‘prevention’, ‘AIDS’, ‘sick’, ‘nurse’),
opposite to Precovid where most terms relate to politics (‘legislator’, ‘senators’, ‘senate’,
‘representative’). Finally, in Figure 4C, the word ‘trial’ presents mostly Covid nearest neighbors
concerning experimental and scientific trials (‘science’, ‘investigation’, ‘tubes’, ‘laboratory’). In
contrast, the dominant terms of ‘trial’ (also meaning ‘essay’ in Spanish) during Precovid were
related to literature and theater (‘stage’, ‘narration’, ‘writing’, ‘script’).
Lastly, as a first step in the development of a new measurement of semantic change, we
explored whether the changes observed in the nearest neighbors for the three examples reflect
a shift to a pandemic/sanitary/health dimension. To do this, we used the information revealed
by Figure 4 and calculated the similarity of each word (‘strain’, ‘immunity’ and ‘trial’) to specific
word senses (cf. Grand et al., 2022, for a similar procedure), shown in Figure 5. For each cue, we
selected two paradigm words that would represent the anchor of two different senses: the domi-
nant sense in the Precovid period and the sense that we predicted increased its dominance during
the Covid period. The two pairs of paradigm words were taken from the definitions found in the
dictionary of the Real Academia Española (RAE) (2001) and in the WordReference Spanish
Dictionary (Landesman, 2002). A cue’s final rating for each of the senses was the sum of the
word’s similarity towards each sense’s paradigm words. The two ratings were determined for
the Precovid and the Covid period. We conducted a permutation test to establish the signifi-
cance of the shifts by calculating the shift of each cue to randomly chosen pairs of paradigm
words. We observe that, for the three words explored, the shift in the predicted direction is sta-
tistically significantly larger than its permuted value, p < 0.001. This result indicates that, over
many possible directions, semantic drifts move the cues closer to pandemic-related senses.
DISCUSSION
Understanding how words change their meanings over time is relevant to studies of language
and cultural evolution. Semantic change is usually tracked through the collection and careful
inspection of data that span decades or centuries (e.g., Boleda, 2020; Garg et al., 2018; Hamilton
et al., 2016). However, the COVID-19 crisis has influenced our daily discourse drastically in a
short period of time. The present study aims at developing a novel method to address this rapid
semantic change, specifically the shifts in lexical meaning in the recent history of Rioplatense
Spanish. By using a free word association task we were able to capture subtle and informative
changes in lexical mental representations, comparing it with Precovid data. We found that sev-
eral words drifted from their previous semantic neighborhoods, now revealing associations to
the COVID-19 pandemic. In addition, we developed several measurements such as the propor-
tion of new responses, the divergence of response distributions, and measurements of semantic
shifts that could be used in other scenarios of diachronic semantic change. This method might
therefore be useful to understand fast changes in meaning in a collective scenario.
Three different word association measures were found to detect changes in a word’s mental
representation from Precovid to Covid times. First, we found that significantly more new asso-
ciations appeared for a set of pandemic-related words. These new associations can be inter-
preted as the incorporation of new connotations and senses. In addition, the introduction of
new associations reduces the probability of generating word associations related to Precovid
times. To visualize and interpret these changes in individual words we performed a network
analysis for the words with the highest number of new associations (e.g., ‘isolated’, ‘protocol’,
‘virtual’, ‘confinement’ and ‘virus’, see Figure 2 and Table 4). Visual inspection of the graphs
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
233
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
supports changes consistent with our lived experience of the pandemic. For example, the word
‘isolated’ incorporated direct associations with coronavirus and the pandemic. It is important
to consider that many words have several alternative meanings (i.e., homonyms and polyse-
mous words) that might have changed their frequency of usage during the Covid times. In this
sense, our methodology could be useful to measure these dominance changes through time.
For example, for the cue word ‘virtual’ several associations such as ‘imaginary’, ‘unreal’ and
‘intangible’ are lost, while associations related to technological virtuality appear (e.g., ‘school’,
‘work’ and ‘Zoom’), thus reinforcing the idea of a rearrangement in the semantic network dur-
ing Covid period. This is remarkable, considering that participants are asked to give three
responses, so they do get an opportunity to give responses related to the old senses, but our
results show that either old or new senses would prevail.
Second, when analyzing the distribution of responses, we observe a greater KL divergence
for pandemic words. This may be because pandemic-related words changed their meanings
(increasing their ambiguity or number of senses) due to the COVID-19 pandemic, and thus
presented a higher relative entropy of the distribution of responses in Covid time with respect
to Precovid time. We have also found that the ‘routine’ words show a reduction in their pro-
portion of new responses and KL divergence with respect to control words. We speculate that
this effect might be related to a reduced use of routine words (e.g., related to transportation,
leisure, clothing) during the Covid period. In this sense, it has been proposed that less fre-
quently used words would be less prone to semantic drift (Bybee, 2006, but see Hamilton
et al., 2016). That is, these words would be less used and therefore their plasticity would be
downgraded, leading to a reduced change in response patterns.
Finally, using a semantic similarity analysis we found a greater difference between neigh-
bors in the Precovid and Covid periods for pandemic cues, compared with control cues. In this
sense, polysemic words like ‘immunity’ or ‘trial’ were found to increase their sanitary/health
association during the Covid period. In this case, word’s senses were determined by manual
inspection of the nearest neighbors in each time period and by using this information to select
paradigm words from dictionary entries. However, further work to automatically determine
each word’s sense might be useful in order to scale this analysis to all words and, therefore,
be able to develop a new measure of semantic drift.
We consider that this might be a first stage in a semantic broadening process, by which the
meaning of a word evolves over time to represent a more general concept or thing than it did
originally (Hamilton et al., 2016). The described methodology could be used in a variety of
different scenarios to analyze semantic changes in language usage over time, like tracking
changes in politically-related words (Rodriguez et al., 2023), in scientific terminology (e.g.,
related to climate change or emerging technologies), or cultural shifts. By analyzing the near-
est neighbors and tracking changes in semantic similarity over time, we could gain insights
into the underlying factors driving changes in language usage.
Limitations
One limitation of the present study is that it only involves two time periods, not allowing us to
measure the persistence of the observed changes. We aim to perform a follow-up study to deter-
mine whether the shift in some associations still persists after the COVID pandemic impact has
passed. We predict that for some words (such as ‘virus’ or ‘virtual’) the changes were so dramatic
that surely we will still find these new associations through several years to come.
An additional caveat of the present study is that the comparison between Precovid and
Covid times was performed in a different set of participants. Although it would be interesting
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
234
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
.
/
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
to have a within-participant comparison to address individual changes, we consider that given
the sample size our study deals with collective language properties that are representative of
the population under study ( Wertsch & Roediger, 2008). In addition, there was a difference
in the regional variant of native Rioplatense Spanish between the Precovid and Covid datasets,
where the former included more participants of the Uruguayan variant while the latter had
more participants that selected the Argentinean variant. Although this might have led to dif-
ferences unrelated to COVID, we consider it is unlikely that regional differences account for
the results observed for several reasons. First, Uruguayan Spanish and Argentinian Spanish are
two linguistic variants of Rioplatense Spanish that share many distinctive phonological, gram-
matical and lexical features (Di Tullio & Kailuweit, 2011). These substantial lexical character-
istics justify the construction of unified lexical norms specific to the Rioplatense dialect, as was
done in the SWOW-RP norms (Cabana et al., 2023). Second, none of the most common new
responses given during the Covid period (Figure 2) were lexical variants or homophones of
Uruguayan and Argentinian Spanish. This was verified by authors native to each of the two
countries. Third, we controlled for the regional variations that we detected as responses (even
though they were not the most common new responses for target words) by replacing the
Argentinean Rioplatense lexical variants with their Uruguayan Rioplatense counterparts (10
responses). Finally, it is important to consider that country-specific patterns during the pan-
demic could have differentially affected the observed lexical-semantic changes. While both
Uruguay and Argentina experienced large degrees of mobility restrictions in 2020 (Cabana
et al., 2021; Fiori et al., 2022) that negatively impacted several aspects of everyday life, the
governmental response was different in both countries. Argentina underwent a severe lock-
down while in Uruguay schools were closed and social gatherings were restricted primarily
during the first half of 2020 and 2021, and the resulting mobility reduction had an important
voluntary component (Cabana et al., 2021). In any case, the potential interaction effects
between nationality and the pandemic would not invalidate our claims that we are indeed
measuring semantic shifts that can be attributed to the COVID-19 pandemic and its major
social and behavioral changes. Furthermore, considering that the pandemic had a global
impact, it would be interesting to expand the measures to other languages as well. In this
sense, the Small World of Words project currently contains large datasets in English, Dutch,
Iberic Spanish, Mandarin, Cantonese, German, Italian and Japanese, making it likely to
address the dynamics of various languages.
Relevance
We consider that the present study is relevant as a methodological approach to semantic change
in an ecologically valid setting. In this sense, one advantage of the current word association task
is that it was not framed within a pandemic-related context, but instead it was the context that
imposed itself.In other words, we did not lead participants to find any connection with the
pandemic, because the target words were presented in isolation and ‘diluted’ within fillers.
Word associations can usefully complement text-based analysis by providing insight into
conceptual changes through a controlled procedure in which words are presented relatively
context-free, which, consistent with previous work, taps into multimodal and common-sense
representations (De Deyne et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2022; Simmons et al., 2008). In addition, while
distributional semantics based on text corpus has tremendous potential to accelerate research in
semantic change (Boleda, 2020), a major challenge is that these methods need very large datasets,
which are usually available in English. Moreover, the vast majority of studies focus on the Google
Books Ngrams corpus, which covers only the period between 1850 and 2009 (Boleda, 2020).
Word associations thus offer a viable alternative to identify meaning changes in a
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
235
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
demographic-aware fashion, for underrepresented languages for which large diachronic corpora
are not available.
Conclusion
This study introduces a new research paradigm for investigating semantic change in the context of
COVID-19, which utilizes a large-scale word-association task in two different times. This approach
combines methods from computational linguistics (such as semantic networks, and natural lan-
guage processing techniques) and psycholinguistics (demographic-aware experiment design with
selected stimuli). Importantly, the data was obtained without any contextual reference, just using
isolated words. Thus, the pandemic context imposed itself, making it one of the rare cases of natural
experiments being employed in linguistics. Since research questions of this nature are not usually
not tested on an individual’s scale but instead rely on large linguistic corpora, we suggest that the
development and adaptation of this research methodology is an important contribution of this study.
Furthermore, we demonstrated that the semantic associations collected for several selected
words changed after the COVID-19 pandemic, compared with associations collected before
the pandemic. Although this is an intuitive result, it nevertheless shows that semantic associ-
ations in aggregated groups of real people are measurably influenced and undergo change in a
short period of time, a noteworthy result.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Nicole Coaker for help with the initial data acquisition and Dr. Luz Bavassi for help-
ful suggestions during data analysis.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The dataset presented here is available for research purposes only under the Creative
Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-NoDerivs 3.0 Unported License. No distribution or
repackaging is allowed without explicit consent from the last author of this paper. Please refer
to this dataset as SWOW-RP-COVID word association data.
Raw and processed data are available at https://smallworldofwords.org/uy/project/research.
Scripts to process are available through a GitHub repository linked on the same page.
REFERENCES
Boleda, G. (2020). Distributional semantics and linguistic theory.
Annual Review of Linguistics, 6, 213–234. https://doi.org/10
.1146/annurev-linguistics-011619-030303
Bybee, J. (2006). Frequency of use and the organization of lan-
guage. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:
oso/9780195301571.001.0001
Borge-Holthoefer, J., & Arenas, A. (2009). Navigating word associ-
ation norms to extract semantic information. In Proceedings of
the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society
( Vol. 621, pp. 2777–2782).
Cabana, Á., Etcheverry, L., Fariello, M. I., Bermolen, P., & Fiori, M.
(2021). Assessing the impact of mobility reduction in the second
wave of COVID-19. In 2021 XLVII Latin American Computing
Conference (CLEI) (pp. 1–10). IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109
/CLEI53233.2021.9639974
Cabana, Á., Zugarramurdi, C., Valle-Lisboa, J., & De Deyne, S.
(2023). The “small world of words” free association norms for
Rioplatense Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 1–18. https://
doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02070-z, PubMed: 36922451
Carrillo, F., Cecchi, G. A., Sigman, M., & Fernandez Slezak, D.
(2015). Fast distributed dynamics of semantic networks via
social media. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience,
2015, Article 712835. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/712835,
PubMed: 26074953
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory
of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407–428.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407
Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from
semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 8(2), 240–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022
-5371(69)80069-1
De Deyne, S., Cabana, Á., Li, B., Cai, Q., & McKague, M.
(2020). A cross-linguistic study into the contribution of affective
connotation in the lexico-semantic representation of concrete
and abstract concepts. In Proceedings for the 42nd Annual
Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society: Developing a Mind:
Learning in Humans, Animals, and Machines, CogSci 2020
(pp. 2776–2782).
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
236
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
De Deyne, S., Kenett, Y. N., Anaki, D., Faust, M., & Navarro, D.
(2017). Large-scale network representations of semantics in the
mental lexicon. In M. N. Jones (Ed.), Big data in cognitive science
(pp. 174–202). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., Collell, G., & Perfors, A. (2021).
Visual and affective multimodal models of word meaning in lan-
guage and mind. Cognitive Science, 45(1), Article e12922.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12922, PubMed: 33432630
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., & Storms, G. (2013). Better explana-
tions of lexical and semantic cognition using networks derived
from continued rather than single-word associations. Behavior
Research Methods, 45(2), 480–498. https://doi.org/10.3758
/s13428-012-0260-7, PubMed: 23055165
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., Perfors, A., Brysbaert, M., & Storms,
G. (2019). The “small world of words” English word association
norms for over 12,000 cue words. Behavior Research Methods,
51(3), 987–1006. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1115-7,
PubMed: 30298265
De Deyne, S., & Storms, G. (2008a). Word associations: Network and
semantic properties. Behavior Research Methods, 40(1), 213–231.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.213, PubMed: 18411545
De Deyne, S., & Storms, G. (2008b). Word associations: Norms for
1,424 Dutch words in a continuous task. Behavior Research
Methods, 40(1), 198–205. https://doi.org/10.3758/ BRM.40.1
.198, PubMed: 18411543
Deese, J. (1965). The structure of associations in language and
thought. Johns Hopkins Press.
Di Tullio, A., & Kailuweit, R. (2011). El español rioplatense:
Lengua, literatura, expresiones culturales. Iberoamericana/ Vervuert.
https://doi.org/10.31819/9783865278821
Drost, H. G. (2018). Philentropy: Information theory and distance
quantification with R. Journal of Open Source Software, 3(26),
765. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.00765
Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, A., & Carreiras,
M. (2013). EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word proper-
ties. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1246–1258. https://doi
.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1, PubMed: 23468181
Fiori, M., Bello, G., Wschebor, N., Lecumberry, F., Ferragut, A., &
Mordecki, E. (2022). Decoupling between SARS-CoV-2 transmis-
sibility and population mobility associated with increasing
immunity from vaccination and infection in South America. Sci-
entific Reports, 12(1), Article 6874. https://doi.org/10.1038
/s41598-022-10896-4, PubMed: 35478213
Garg, N., Schiebinger, L., Jurafsky, D., & Zou, J. (2018). Word embed-
dings quantify 100 years of gender and ethnic stereotypes. Proceed-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(16), E3635–E3644.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1720347115, PubMed: 29615513
Gentsch, K., Loderer, K., Soriano, C., Fontaine, J. R., Eid, M.,
Pekrun, R., & Scherer, K. R. (2018). Effects of achievement
contexts on the meaning structure of emotion words. Cognition
and Emotion, 32(2), 379–388. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931
.2017.1287668, PubMed: 28278740
Grand, G., Blank, I. A., Pereira, F., & Fedorenko, E. (2022). Seman-
tic projection recovers rich human knowledge of multiple object
features from word embeddings. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(7),
975–987. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01316-8,
PubMed: 35422527
Hamilton, W. L., Leskovec, J., & Jurafsky, D. (2016). Diachronic word
embeddings reveal statistical laws of semantic change.
arXiv:1605.09096. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1605.09096
Landesman, B. (2002). WordReference.com. Reference Reviews.
Laurino, J., Forcato, C., Coaker, N., Pedreira, M. E., & Kaczer, L.
(2022). Learning new words: Memory reactivation as a
mechanism for strengthening and updating a novel word’s mean-
ing. Memory & Cognition, 50(4), 655–671. https://doi.org/10
.3758/s13421-021-01247-1, PubMed: 34751905
Lenth, R. V. (2022). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-
squares means. R package version 1.7.3.
Liu, C., Cohn T., De Deyne, S. & Frermann L. (2022). WAX: A new
dataset for word association explanations. In The 2nd Conference
of the Asia-Pacific Chapter of the Association for Computational
Linguistics and the 12th International Joint Conference on Natural
Language Processing (AACL-IJCNLP) (pp. 106–120). Association
for Computational Linguistics.
McInnes, L., Healy, J., & Melville, J. (2018). UMAP: Uniform
manifold approximation and projection for dimension reduc-
tion. arXiv: 1802.03426. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1802
.03426
Mollin, S. (2009). Combining corpus linguistics and psychological
data on word co-occurrence: Corpus collocates versus word
associations. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 5, 175–200.
https://doi.org/10.1515/CLLT.2009.008
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free associa-
tion and what does it measure?. Memory & Cognition, 28(6),
887–899. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03209337, PubMed: 11105515
Parks, R., Ray, J., & Bland, S. (1998). Wordsmyth English dictionary-
thesaurus [Electronic version]. University of Chicago.
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical
computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Real Academia Española. (2001). Diccionario de la lengua Española
(22nd ed.). Espasa Calpe.
Rodd, J. M. (2020). Settling into semantic space: An ambiguity-
focused account of word-meaning access. Perspectives on Psy-
chological Science, 15(2), 411–427. https://doi.org/10.1177
/1745691619885860, PubMed: 31961780
Rodriguez, P. L., Spirling, A., & Stewart, B. M. (2023). Embedding
regression: Models for context-specific description and infer-
ence. American Political Science Review, 1–20. https://doi.org
/10.1017/S0003055422001228
Simmons, W. K., Hamann, S. B., Harenski, C. L., Hu, X. P., &
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). fMRI evidence for word association
and situated simulation in conceptual processing. Journal of
Physiology–Paris, 102(1–3), 106–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.jphysparis.2008.03.014, PubMed: 18468869
Simpson, J. A., Proffitt, M., & Weiner, E. S. (Eds.). (1997). Oxford
English dictionary additions series ( Vol. 3). Oxford University Press.
Stella, M., Restocchi, V., & De Deyne, S. (2020). #lockdown:
Network-enhanced emotional profiling in the time of covid-19.
Big Data and Cognitive Computing, 4(2), Article 14. https://doi
.org/10.3390/bdcc4020014
Steyvers, M., Griffiths, T. L., & Dennis, S. (2006). Probabilistic infer-
ence in human semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
10(7), 327–334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.005,
PubMed: 16793324
Thompson, B., Roberts, S. G., & Lupyan, G. (2020). Cultural influences
on word meanings revealed through large-scale semantic alignment.
Nature Human Behaviour, 4(10), 1029–1038. https://doi.org/10.1038
/s41562-020-0924-8, PubMed: 32778801
Wertsch, J. V., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Collective memory:
Conceptual foundations and theoretical approaches. Memory,
16(3), 318–326. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701801434,
PubMed: 18324555
Wilkins, D. P. (1996). Natural tendencies of semantic change and
the search for cognates. In M. Durie & M. Ross (Eds.), The com-
parative method reviewed: Regularity and irregularity in language
change (pp. 264–304). Oxford University Press.
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
237
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
/
.
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
/
.
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
APPENDIX
Table 3. Mean frequency, imageability, familiarity, and concreteness for each cue type.
Cue type
control
pandemic
emotion
routine
Frequency
11.7
Imageability
5.1
Familiarity
5.6
Concreteness
4.9
9.8
13.6
16.4
4.6
4.8
5.7
5.6
5.7
6.2
4.5
4.5
5.3
Note. Frequency, imageability, familiarity, and concreteness for each word was determined with the
EsPal Latin American Spanish subtitle tokens.
Table 4.
Top 10 pandemic cues with highest proportion of new responses.
Cue
protocolo [protocol]
responsabilidad [responsibility]
medidas [measures]
virus [virus]
restricción [restriction]
virtual [virtual]
normalidad [normal]
aglomeración [agglomeration]
aislado [isolated]
encierro [confinement]
Prop. of new responses
0.62
0.57
0.57
0.57
0.56
0.56
0.55
0.54
0.5
0.5
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
238
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3
The Pandemic in Words
Laurino et al.
Table 5.
Top 10 pandemic cues with highest KL divergence.
Cue
protocolo [protocol]
virus [virus]
responsabilidad [responsibility]
virtual [virtual]
medidas [measures]
restricción [restriction]
normalidad [normal]
aglomeración [agglomeration]
aislado [isolated]
social [social]
KL
4.43
4.03
3.92
3.9
3.9
3.89
3.89
3.62
3.57
3.55
Table 6.
Top 10 pandemic cues with highest mean similarity ratio deviation from one.
Cue
cepa [strain]
inmunidad [immunity]
mutación [mutation]
metros [meters]
garganta [throat]
fase [phase]
ensayo [trial]
grupo [group]
esperanza [hope]
adultos [adults]
Ratio deviation
0.6
0.44
0.43
0.38
0.34
0.33
0.33
0.32
0.32
0.31
OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science
239
l
D
o
w
n
o
a
d
e
d
f
r
o
m
h
t
t
p
:
/
/
d
i
r
e
c
t
.
m
i
t
.
/
e
d
u
o
p
m
i
/
l
a
r
t
i
c
e
-
p
d
f
/
d
o
i
/
i
/
.
/
1
0
1
1
6
2
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
2
1
3
3
8
4
8
o
p
m
_
a
_
0
0
0
8
1
p
d
.
/
i
f
b
y
g
u
e
s
t
t
o
n
0
7
S
e
p
e
m
b
e
r
2
0
2
3