INVESTIGACIÓN

INVESTIGACIÓN

Fluid and flexible minds: Intelligence reflects
synchrony in the brain’s intrinsic
network architecture

Michael A. Ferguson1,2∗

, Jeffrey S. Anderson2, y r. Nathan Spreng1∗

1Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, Human Neuroscience Institute, Department of Human Development, Universidad de Cornell,
Ítaca, Nueva York, 14853
2Departments of Bioengineering and Neuroradiology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, 84132

Palabras clave: intelligence, resonancia magnética funcional, resting state functional connectivity, aprendizaje automático, cognition

un acceso abierto

diario

ABSTRACTO

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

t

/

/

mi
d
tu
norte
mi
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

/

1
2
1
9
2
1
0
9
1
9
2
5
norte
mi
norte
_
a
_
0
0
0
1
0
pag
d

t

.

Human intelligence has been conceptualized as a complex system of dissociable cognitive
procesos, yet studies investigating the neural basis of intelligence have typically emphasized
the contributions of discrete brain regions or, more recently, of specific networks of
functionally connected regions. Here we take a broader, systems perspective in order to
investigate whether intelligence is an emergent property of synchrony within the brain’s
intrinsic network architecture. Using a large sample of resting-state fMRI and cognitive data
(norte = 830), we report that the synchrony of functional interactions within and across
distributed brain networks reliably predicts fluid and flexible intellectual functioning. Por
adopting a whole-brain, systems-level approach, we were able to reliably predict individual
differences in human intelligence by characterizing features of the brain’s intrinsic network
architecture. These findings hold promise for the eventual development of neural markers to
predict changes in intellectual function that are associated with neurodevelopment, normal
aging, and brain disease.

RESUMEN DEL AUTOR

In our study, we aimed to understand how individual differences in intellectual functioning
are reflected in the intrinsic network architecture of the human brain. We applied statistical
methods, known as spectral decompositions, in order to identify individual differences in the
synchronous patterns of spontaneous brain activity that reliably predict core aspects of
human intelligence. The synchrony of brain activity at rest across multiple discrete neural
networks demonstrated positive relationships with fluid intelligence. A diferencia de, global
synchrony within the brain’s network architecture reliably, and inversely, predicted mental
flexibility, a core facet of intellectual functioning. The multinetwork systems approach
described here represents a methodological and conceptual extension of earlier efforts that
related differences in intellectual ability to variations in specific brain regions, redes, o
their interactions. Our findings suggest that the neural basis of complex, integrative cognitive
functions can be most completely understood from the perspective of network neuroscience.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

Mapping the neural substrates of human intelligence could better inform our understand-
ing of how brain development influences lifespan development across multiple functional
dominios, both in health and in disease (Furnham, 2008; Gottfredson, 2004; Schutte, 2014).
The concept of general intelligence—Spearman’s g, or simply “g”—was first postulated by
Spearman at the turn of the previous century (Lancero, 1904). Tests for cognitive performance

Citación: Ferguson, METRO. A., anderson,
j. S., & Spreng, R. norte. (2017). Fluid
and flexible minds: Inteligencia
reflects synchrony in the brain’s
intrinsic network architecture.
Neurociencia en red, 1(2), 192–207.
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00010

DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00010

Supporting Information:

Recibió: 25 Octubre 2016
Aceptado: 14 Marzo 2017

Conflicto de intereses: Los autores tienen
declaró que no hay intereses en competencia
existir.

Autor correspondiente:
Michael A. Ferguson
maf378@cornell.edu

Editor de manejo:
Danielle Bassett

Derechos de autor: © 2017
Instituto de Tecnología de Massachusetts
Publicado bajo Creative Commons
Atribución 4.0 Internacional
(CC POR 4.0) licencia

La prensa del MIT

Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain

share ubiquitous positive correlations, suggesting that a common “intelligence” factor may ex-
plain individual differences across a range of cognitive domains. Over decades of inquiry,
it has been demonstrated that individual differences in Spearman’s g indeed robustly predict
performance on laboratory tests of cognitive function, as well as adaptive behavior in real-
world contexts (Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, 2004; Lancero, 1927). More recently, con el
advent of functional neuroimaging methods, investigating the neural basis of “g” has been an
active area of inquiry (Deary, Penke, & Johnson, 2010; Duncan, 2005; Duncan et al., 2000;
Schultz & Col, 2016).

Por décadas, cognitive science has described intelligence as an emergent property of dis-
sociable processes (Iglesia, 1986; Gregory & Zangwill, 1987; Sternberg & Detterman,
1986). Yet efforts to identify neural markers of “g” have typically focused on individual brain re-
gions (Duncan, 2005; Duncan et al., 2000; Gray, Chabris, & Más valiente, 2003) or specific
networks of functionally connected brain regions (Col, Yarkoni, Repovs, Anticevic, & Más valiente,
2012; Lee et al., 2006; Song et al., 2008). Frontoparietal networks have received the widest at-
tention as a possible single-network solution to understanding the biological underpinnings of
individual differences in intellectual ability (Jung & Haier, 2007; Langeslag et al., 2013). Early
efforts to characterize the neural basis of cognitive functioning, which assumed a one-to-one
brain–behavioral mapping, have been increasingly challenged by models of whole-brain net-
work interactions, particularly with respect to complex, integrative cognitive capacities such
as intelligence (Bullmore et al., 2009; McIntosh, 1999; despreciar, Chialvo, Kaiser, & Hilgetag,
2004; van den heuvel, estampar, Kahn, & Hulshoff Pol, 2009). This suggests that a systems-level,
network-based approach may prove fruitful in identifying reliable neural markers of individual
differences in human intellectual function.

Early localization studies investigated brain–behavior relationships in circumscribed brain
regions because methodological constraints, including low temporal and spatial resolution,
as well restrictions in computational capacity necessitated a univariate analytical approach
(Duncan, 2005; Duncan et al., 2000).
These analyses identified specific brain regions
where the magnitude of activity varied as a function of performance on intelligence tasks
(Duncan et al., 2000). Advances in fMRI data acquisition and analytical methods have
dramatically improved the temporal and spatial resolution of neuroimaging protocols, habilitando
the measurement of regional interactions within distributed brain networks to characterize the
neural architecture of intelligence (Lee et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2013).

More recently, studies have begun to characterize multinetwork dynamics, or the network
architecture of the brain, as a neural marker of intelligence (Col, Ito, & Más valiente, 2015). Prop-
erties of large-scale, distributed networks observed in the brain during a wakeful resting state
have been identified as factors contributing to individual differences in intelligence (Finn et al.,
2015; Hearne et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2015). Building from these earlier studies, here we ap-
ply spectral decompositions to resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) from fMRI in order
to derive estimates of the brain’s spatially overlapping functional architecture that may be sensi-
tive to individual differences in “g.” Resting-state MRI is a powerful tool to detect and dissociate
functional brain networks from patterns of interregional correlations in neuronal variability, es-
timated by variations in BOLD signal and measured in the absence of explicit task demands
(Buckner, Krienen, Castellanos, Diaz, & yo, 2011; Fox et al., 2005). Previous studies have
identified distributed contributions to intelligence in the brain by several methods, incluido
the examination of pairwise connections without imposed network definitions (Hearne et al.,
2016; Smith et al., 2015), or the exploration of functional relationships both with and without
a priori network definitions (Finn et al., 2015). In these analyses, the functional correlations

193

Spectral decompositions:
Operations that determine the
orthogonal basis set of a matrix, p.ej.,
eigendecomposition, singular-value
decomposition, principal-component
análisis

Resting-state functional connectivity:
An fMRI method for mapping
correlated patterns in spontaneous
blood oxygen level dependent signal
across the brain during wakeful rest

Neurociencia en red

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

/

t

/

mi
d
tu
norte
mi
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

/

1
2
1
9
2
1
0
9
1
9
2
5
norte
mi
norte
_
a
_
0
0
0
1
0
pag
d

.

t

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain

Basis set:
Functions, represented as vectors,
whose combinations describe a
system in terms of its computational
and theoretical components

Intrinsic network architecture:
Large-scale patterns of resting-state
functional connectivity across
distributed anatomical regions

Fluid intelligence:
A component of general intelligence
that operationalizes pattern
recognition and completion in
cognitive testing

Cognitive flexibility:
The mental ability to switch between
different systems of representation,
operationalized by the Dimensional
Change Card Sort Task

Persistent synchrony:
Sustained functional covariance of
regiones del cerebro, or nodes, Dentro de un
network

between pairwise regions represent the edges of distributed, spatially nonoverlapping net-
obras (Finn et al., 2015; Hearne et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2015). Here we seek to identify a
basis set of spatially overlapping functional networks in the resting brain and to determine the
contributions of within-network and across-network interactivity to intelligence. We identified
spatially overlapping network maps in an effort to characterize the one-to-many functional or-
ganization of the human brain that we believe is essential for understanding the relationships
between cognition and neural systems (Bullmore et al., 2009; McIntosh, 1999; Sporns et al.,
2004; van den Heuvel et al., 2009).

To identify a basis set of functional networks in the resting brain, we introduced the appli-
cation of a multivariate statistical method, spectral decomposition (Alter, Marrón, & Botstein,
2000; Yeung, Tegner, & collins, 2002), to characterize an RSFC (es decir., intrinsic) network archi-
tecture in a large population of healthy young adults. This technique identifies the spec-
trum of spatially overlapping networks that covary across the duration of the resting-state
fMRI scan. Early brain-imaging studies had applied spectral decompositions to task-based
fMRI data (Bullmore et al., 1996; Friston, Frith, Fletcher, Liddle, & Frackowiak, 1996). To our
conocimiento, this is the first application of spectral analysis to RSFC data in order to describe the
functional dynamics of intrinsic brain networks and predict individual differences in cognitive
capacidad.

To evaluate whether individual differences in resting-state network dynamics may be a
marker of intellectual functioning, we drew upon two measures that have previously been used
to characterize intellectual capacity: fluid intelligence (es decir., control cognitivo) and cognitive
flexibility (Scott, 1962). In our study we investigated whether individual differences in these
core facets of human intelligence are associated with differences in the intrinsic functional ar-
chitecture of the brain, operationalized as the intrinsic synchrony or connectivity of functional
redes. We predicted that greater synchrony within and among functional networks at rest
would predict better performance on measures of fluid intelligence and of cognitive flexibility.

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

t

/

/

mi
d
tu
norte
mi
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

/

1
2
1
9
2
1
0
9
1
9
2
5
norte
mi
norte
_
a
_
0
0
0
1
0
pag
d

.

t

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

RESULTADOS

Spectral Decomposition for Multivariate Pattern Identification

Spectral decomposition was used to determine the synchrony and structure of intrinsic brain
redes. Spectral decompositions are a family of closely related analyses that describe the
dominant components of a complex system. These include principal-component analysis
(PCA), singular value decomposition, and eigendecompositions. Spectral decomposition iden-
tifies the spatial patterns of functional synchrony across brain regions, as well as their archi-
tecture or hierarchical organization, as determined by the synchrony strength, or persistent
synchrony, of these networks. The resulting principal components (PCs) are ranked numeri-
cally according to their prominence within the resting-state architecture (Figura 1A). Hetero-
geneity in the subject-level PC topology was inversely related to singular values (es decir., PC rank);
PC 1 was the most homogeneous across the population, and PC 10 the most heterogeneous
(Figura 1B). The whole-brain networks described by RSFC PCs show features of well-
characterized neurocognitive systems (Fox et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2009; Spreng, Sepulcro,
Tornero, stevens, & Schacter, 2013; Yeo et al., 2011). Por ejemplo, the group-mean RSFC
components show synchrony of sensory and motor cortices (PC 1), the default network (PC 2),
the salience network (atención ventral) relative to default regions (PC 3), the visual system
(PC 4), and the dorsal attention network (PC 5). PC 6 comprises motor and default regions.
PC 7 demonstrates a functionally independent mode of the left relative to right frontoparietal

Neurociencia en red

194

Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

t

/

/

mi
d
tu
norte
mi
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

/

1
2
1
9
2
1
0
9
1
9
2
5
norte
mi
norte
_
a
_
0
0
0
1
0
pag
d

t

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
7
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

Cifra 1. Principal components (PCs) of resting-state functional connectivity.
(A) Top ten PCs
(across-group FWE-corrected p < .05). Reliable positive and negative features are shown for each (B) Correspondence of the most homologous single- component. Color bars indicate t-values. subject PCs to the group-average PCs, shown for 600 subjects from the Human Connectome Project 900-subject release. The least variation across individuals exists in PC1, and variation across indi- vidual PC differences increases consecutively with PC order. control network. PCs 8–10 represent frontoparietal functional ensembles, including posterior attention regions as aspects of canonical intrinsic networks (Chen et al., 2013). All subcortical structures (thalamus, caudate, putamen, pallidum, hippocampus, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens) demonstrated bilateral positive associations with group-mean PCs 1 and 2. In both PCs 3 and 5, amygdala and hippocampus were functionally correlated and showed anticorrelation relative to all other subcortical structures. Subcortical functional asso- ciations between the hemispheres were largely symmetric across principal components, with the notable exception of PC 7. In PC 7, a left-lateralized subcortical configuration corre- sponded to left-lateralized patterns in cortical activity (Table 1). Eigenvalue: A singular-value representation for the amount of system variance explained by the corresponding eigenvector within a basis set of functions Eigenvalues and Behavior Correlation scores were calculated between cognitive measures and eigenvalues (i.e., mea- sures of network synchrony) for PCs 1–10 (n = 600). PC eigenvalues demonstrated significant correlations with numerous measures of cognitive performance when corrected for multiple comparisons (Figure 2). As predicted, fluid intelligence demonstrated the greatest correla- tions with functional network eigenvalues. All correlations between eigenvalues (measures of within-network synchrony) and fluid intelligence were positive, indicating that greater Network Neuroscience 195 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain Table 1. Principal-component (PC) subcortical associations Thalamus Caudate Amygdala Hippocampus Pallidum Nucleus accumbens Putamen PC 1 L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ PC 2 L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ R+ L+ PC 3 L+ R+ L+ R+ L– R– L– R– L+ R+ – PC 4 L– L– R– L– L+ R+ L– – L+ R+ L– R– PC 5 L– R– L– R– L+ R+ – L– R– L– R– L– R– PC 6 PC 7 PC 8 PC 9 PC 10 L+ R+ L+ – – L+ R+ R+ L+ R+ – L+ R+ L+ L+ L+ L+ L+ R+ – – L– R– L– R– – R– – – – – L+ – – – L– R– L– R– L– R– L– R– – – L– R– The table shows associations between PCs 1–10 and bilateral subcortical structures. Positive and negative functional relationships with left (L) and right (R) subcortical structures are shown only for significant regions. within-network synchrony across a range of networks is predictive of fluid intelligence (means [95% CIs]: PC 3, r = 0.08 [0.015, 0.15]; PC 4, r = 0.13 [0.06, 0.19]; PC 5, r = 0.12 [0.05, 0.18]; PC 6, r = 0.09 [0.02, 0.15]; PC 7, r = 0.12 [0.06, 0.19]; PC 8, r = 0.14 [0.06, 0.21]; PC 9, r = 0.13 [0.07, 0.20]; PC 10, r = 0.13 [0.07, 0.20]; see Figure 2). Cognitive flexibility demonstrated a negative correlation with the eigenvalue of PC 1, indicating that greater global synchrony is associated with lower cognitive flexibility (r = –0.09, 95% CI [–0.16, –0.03]; Figure 2). l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 2. Cognitive measures and PC eigenvalues. Spearman correlations were calculated between cognitive performance measures and unscaled eigenvalues for PCs 1–10 (n = 600). Correlations with 95% confidence intervals that do not cross 0 and that survive multiple- comparison correction are highlighted. The cognitive measures demonstrating the broadest cor- relations with PC eigenvalues are those for fluid intelligence. Because larger eigenvalues indicate stronger within-network synchrony, the results demonstrate that increased within-network syn- chrony for a broad range of PCs is positively correlated with fluid intelligence. Network Neuroscience 196 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain Scaled Eigenvalues and Behavior To investigate the relationship between intellectual functioning and individual differences in network hierarchies, we also correlated the cognitive performance measures with the scaled eigenvalues for PCs 2–10. (Because the scaled eigenvalue for PC 1 is by definition equal to 1, PC 1 is not included in the scaled-eigenvalue comparisons.) Cognitive flexibility demonstrated positive correlations with each of the scaled eigenvalues for PCs 2 to 10 (means [95% CIs]: PC 2, r = 0.09 [0.03, 0.16]; PC 3, r = 0.09 [0.03, 0.16]; PC 4, r = 0.10 [0.03, 0.16]; PC 5, r = 0.09 [0.03, 0.17]; PC 6, r = 0.10 [0.03, 0.16]; PC 7, r = 0.10 [0.04, 0.17]; PC 8, r = 0.10 [0.04, 0.17]; PC 9, r = 0.10 [0.03, 0.17]; PC 10, r = 0.10 [0.03, 0.17]; Figure 3). This indicates that cognitive flexibility is supported by persistent synchrony across a broad array of functional networks. In contrast, as we identified above, global synchrony, or the baseline level of sustained synchrony across the whole brain, is associated with lower cognitive flexibility. A similar pattern was observed for processing speed, suggesting that both aspects of cognitive functioning are enhanced by greater synchrony in specific networks but negatively impacted by higher levels of global synchrony. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 3. Cognitive measures and scaled eigenvalues for the PCs. Spearman correlations were calculated between cognitive performance measures and the scaled eigenvalues for PCs 2–10 (n = 600). Scaled eigenvalues were calculated by dividing each PC’s eigenvalue by the eigenvalue for PC 1 in each individual subject. Correlations with 95% confidence intervals that do not cross 0 and that survive multiple-comparison correction are highlighted. The cognitive measures demonstrating the broadest correlations with PCs are those for cognitive flexibility and processing speed. Larger scaled eigenvalues indicate stronger within-network synchrony relative to the global synchrony of an individual’s brain. Stronger within-network synchrony relative to the global synchrony (PC 1) is positively correlated with increased cognitive flexibility and more rapid processing speed across PCs 2–10. Network Neuroscience 197 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain Least absolute squares shrinkage operator (LASSO): A statistical learning method for selecting predictive features from large sets of independent variables through iterative regression Connectome: Graph theory model for connections across the brain; the connectome can be represented as layers of spatially overlapping functional networks Network Neuroscience Behavior/RSFC Network Interactions Using a LASSO regression determined from a training set of 600 participants, 33 of the 256 possible unique combinations of network interactions were used as predictors for fluid intel- ligence in an independent testing set (n = 230). The motivation for identifying combinations of networks related to intelligence is the hypothesis that individual differences in intellectual abilities may be represented better by the interactions of functional networks. A correlation score of r = 0.24 (p < .001) between the measured and predicted values of fluid intelligence was observed in the testing set (Figure 4A). For cognitive flexibility, 48 of the 1,024 possible unique combinations of network interactions were selected by LASSO regression as the best-fit model of cognitive flexibility from the PC eigenvalues. A correlation score of r = 0.07 (p = .28) was observed between the predicted and measured scores of cognitive flexibility (Figure 4B). However, predictions for cognitive flexibility based on eigenvalue products failed to reach sig- nificance, indicating that the eigenvalue magnitude for PC 1—that is, the strength of global synchrony—is a better predictor of cognitive flexibility than are models based on network in- teractions. Together, these results suggest that fluid intelligence and cognitive flexibility are associated with greater network stability or synchrony for the networks related to PCs 3–10. In contrast, cognitive flexibility, but not fluid intelligence, is negatively associated with overall synchrony, or stability, within the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture. DISCUSSION We investigated how individual differences in the intrinsic network architecture of the brain are associated with human intelligence. Building from previous studies (Finn et al., 2015; Hearne et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2013), here we applied spectral decomposition methods to large-scale functional brain networks to determine whether spatially overlapping patterns of synchrony within these networks corresponded with two core aspects of intellectual func- tioning: fluid intelligence and cognitive flexibility. Both fluid intelligence and cognitive flex- ibility were reliably associated with the functional architecture of intrinsic brain networks. Synchrony within multiple functional networks reliably predicted fluid intelligence. Greater relative synchrony of networks within the network architecture (i.e., scaled eigenvalues) was associated with greater cognitive flexibility, whereas greater whole-brain baseline synchrony levels were associated with reduced flexibility. These results are consistent with our prediction that systems-level descriptors of the intrinsic functional architecture of the brain can provide reliable markers of human intellectual functioning. Properties of the brain’s intrinsic neural architecture have been used to predict a range of cognitive capacities and may be a powerful predictor of more stable, or metacognitive, capaci- ties such as intelligence (Stevens & Spreng, 2014). Previous reports examining the associations between resting-state functional connectivity and intelligence have identified pairs of nodes (i.e., network edges) that are predictive of cognitive ability using multiple regressions for fea- ture selection (Finn et al., 2015; Hearne et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2015). Conceptually, our approach is consistent with these studies, in the sense that we characterize features of the brain’s functional architecture in order to identify large-scale distributed net- works that support intellectual abilities. Our approach also builds upon these previous studies in several key ways. First, we identified whole-brain distributed networks using spectral de- composition applied to the resting-state connectome (Figure 1), rather than using pairwise correlation values as the predictive features for our model. We then used the singular val- ues (i.e., eigenvalues) of the first ten principal components (eigenvectors), representing the persistence of within-network synchrony, to measure relationships between whole-network 198 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 4. Predicting aspects of intelligence from resting-state functional connectivity A least- absolute-squares shrinkage operator (LASSO) regression trained in a subsample of n = 600 pre- dicted fluid intelligence in an independent testing set (n = 230) with a correlation of r = 0.24 (p < 0.001; panel A). Using the same training set (n = 600) and testing set (n = 230) subsamples, LASSO regression predicted cognitive flexibility with a correlation of r = 0.07 (p = 0.28; panel B). The differential in predictive power between fluid intelligence and cognitive flexibility indicates that the spectral features associated with fluid intelligence represent more unique cognitive variance than do the spectral features associated with cognitive flexibility. synchrony and intelligence (Figures 2 and 3). This method supports the findings from previous network approaches to identify the predictors of intelligence (Finn et al., 2015; Hearne et al., 2016) and positive (or negative) real-life functioning (Smith et al., 2015). The present study corroborates these earlier reports that whole-brain network connectivity and interactivity are biomarkers of fluid intelligence (Finn et al., 2015; Hearne et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2015) and Network Neuroscience 199 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain supports the conclusion that its large-scale network architecture reflects individual differences in human intellectual functioning. As we noted above, singular values for PCs are interpreted as reflecting the temporal stabil- ity of brain networks. In simulations iterating brain activity from a set of initial conditions based on functional connectivity between brain regions (Ferguson & Anderson, 2012), the PCs of functional connectivity matrices by definition correspond to patterns of activity that are invariant to change in a Markov model based on functional connectivity between regions. We predicted that more temporally stable patterns of brain activity would be associated with higher intelligence across a broad range of brain networks. Consistent with this possibility, our results demonstrated that across-network interactions at rest are predictive of fluid intelligence more strongly than are correlations with the synchrony strength of any individual network (Figure 4). Cross-network interactions between the default network and frontoparietal networks have previously been reported to correspond positively with individual differences in fluid intelligence (Finn et al., 2015; Hearne et al., 2016). Our results are compatible with these findings of across-network interactions supporting fluid in- telligence, and they extend this cross-network interaction paradigm for fluid intelligence to include a diverse set of resting-state functional networks (see Figures 1 and 4). Although our findings do not exclude a disproportionate effect of some connections, such as frontoparietal region pairs, which may have influenced many of the PCs in our analysis, the data suggest that additional information contributes to fluid intelligence in across-network synchrony. As we demonstrated here, spectral decomposition of resting-state functional connectivity is consistent with a “one-to-many” functional architecture, with brain regions being implicated in multiple PCs, or networks. It is widely accepted that single brain regions can be recruited dur- ing multiple cognitive processes (McIntosh, 1999). One constraint of common decomposition methods such as spatial independence (Beckmann et al., 2005) or a greedy, winner-take-all algorithm (Yeo et al., 2011) is that they may obscure this one-to-many architecture. The PCA approach used here to identify patterns of functional synchrony (i.e., intrinsic brain networks) enables the identification of “promiscuous” brain regions that flexibly couple with multiple networks, providing the neural substrate for distributed parallel processing, a necessary con- dition for higher cognitive processes associated with intellectual functioning. Structural research on neural contributions to intelligence has indicated that greater efficiency of physical connections within networks is positively correlated with individ- ual differences in cognitive performance (Li et al., 2009; Pineda-Pardo et al., 2015). These findings may provide insight into the ways that individual differences in functional syn- chrony might arise from variations in anatomical connections. Integration of structural and functional data indicates that both efficiency and synchrony characterize intelligent brains (Pineda-Pardo et al., 2015). Greater efficiency in structural connectivity may directly con- tribute to more persistent synchrony within functional networks, in turn producing greater cognitive capacity. Future investigations, integrating multilayer maps that can incorporate functional synchrony, structural efficiency, and other physiological or genetic factors, will be necessary to identify truly systems-level biomarkers of human intelligence. A major challenge in identifying neural makers of intellectual functioning has been the vari- ability in research methodologies and in the operationalization of intelligence across studies (Sternberg, 2005). A prominent approach has involved the investigation of domain-specific processes associated with general intelligence, such as working memory capacity (Ackerman, et al., 2005; Conway et al., 2003; DeYoung et al., 2009; Edgin et al., 2010). However, Network Neuroscience 200 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain mounting neural evidence is challenging such domain-specific conceptualizations of intelli- gence and supporting the existence of a more unitary or “multiple-demand” account (Duncan, 2010; Duncan et al., 2000) of fluid intellectual functioning, implicating the frontal parietal control network (Vincent et al., 2008). Meta-analyses of fMRI studies provide partial sup- port for this idea. As was suggested by Duncan (2010), multiple brain regions are com- monly recruited during cognitive control tasks associated with fluid intelligence (Basten, 2015). However, this multidemand network flexibly couples with other brain regions depending on the specific task context, including perceptual, mnemonic, or motor output demands (e.g., Niendam et al., 2012). This hybrid model, combining cognitive control and fluid intellectual functioning more broadly, is consistent with our findings, which also suggest that multiple brain networks (PCs 3–10) couple with core frontoparietal brain regions, yet remain differentiated by network-specific recruitment of brain regions outside the frontoparietal control network. Patterns of persistent frontoparietal network synchrony are found in PCs 6, 7, and 10 and correspond to previous evidence that fluid intelligence, or “g,” may be preferentially dependent on the integrity of frontoparietal control networks and the underlying white-matter pathways (Penke et al., 2012, and see above). Cognitive control processes linked to fluid intelligence are necessary to form, reconfigure, or consolidate interpretive schema and are robustly linked with frontoparietal functional anatomy (Cole et al., 2015; Cole et al., 2013; Cole et al., 2012; Spreng et al., 2013). The positive correlations observed with the eigenvalues for these com- ponents suggest that fluid intelligence is supported by the cooperation of multiple spatially overlapping functional networks defined in our model, consistent with previous meta-analyses (e.g., Niendam et al., 2012). Furthermore, individuals with higher levels of intellectual func- tioning have greater brain “resilience,” such that those demonstrating more spatially distributed patterns of neural recruitment during cognitive tasks may be less susceptible to age-related brain changes or brain insult (Santarnecchi, Rossi, & Rossi, 2015). Our results here are also consistent with this idea, demonstrating that an intrinsic functional architecture comprising multiple, widely distributed brain networks, organized along a continuum of differentiated network synchrony, may provide the necessary neural foundation for the expression, and pre- sumably the preservation, of fluid intelligence. Interestingly, the magnitude of intrinsic global synchrony (PC 1) was negatively associated with cognitive flexibility (Figure 2). This may suggest, perhaps somewhat intuitively, that greater whole-brain synchrony—conceptually related to stability—is associated with lower mental flexibility. PC 1 is unique among the components in that it did not reflect some brain regions as positive and other brain regions as negative, so differences specific to PC 1 in our behav- ioral correlations may reflect global synchrony versus antagonistic or competing interactions between brain regions reflected in successive PCs. Cognitive flexibility, as such, appears func- tionally related to lower global synchrony and more network flexibility, a pattern of neural activity positively associated with learning and health (Bassett et al., 2011; Braun et al., 2015). We suggest that a flexible network architecture with interacting brain networks is necessary for flexible thought and behavior, and may be a critical element of adaptive real-world func- tioning. Investigating this association between network and behavioral flexibility and how it changes as a result of aging and brain disease will represent an important area of future re- search. However, this observation also highlights the importance of studying functional brain networks, not simply in isolation, but also in relation to other networks, as well as studying within-subject variability in global network characteristics, as we did here. By scaling individ- ual network synchrony values by each individual’s global network synchrony, we were able to Network Neuroscience 201 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain identify robust correlations between these scaled eigenvalues and cognitive flexibility for all identified networks (PCs 2–10 in Figure 3). Our findings suggesting that greater network synchrony is associated with higher fluid in- telligence are consistent with reports that the brain network configuration at rest is closely aligned with task-driven network configurations in individuals of higher intelligence Schultz, 2016. These resting-state (or intrinsic) connections are hypothesized to reflect the persistence of task-based synchrony patterns formed through recurrent coactivation of distributed brain regions occurring over the course of development (Stevens & Spreng, 2014). These patterns of coactivation may serve as the brain’s “ready state,” potentiating task-driven activity among regions that commonly work in concert (Fair et al., 2007). The functional network organization associated with higher cognitive functioning may position individuals in an optimized “ready stance” that is prepared to engage cognitive tasks. However, future research will be necessary to more directly investigate this relationship. These measures describe spectral components of RSFC as novel features of the brain’s func- tional architecture. Here we have shown that individual differences in intrinsic connectivity predict core aspects of human intelligence. Mapping intelligence in the human brain with RSFC offers a novel approach to investigating psychological functioning in health and disease. Alterations in the absolute and relative prominences of networks within the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture may help predict individual differences in normal cognitive function- ing, as was demonstrated here, or predict patterns of altered cognitive abilities in neurodevel- opment, normal aging, and brain disease. METHODS Resting-State Functional Connectivity The Human Connectome Project (HCP) is an initiative by the National Institutes of Health to generate large, open-access behavioral and fMRI datasets. RSFC was preprocessed and analyzed for 830 subjects (mean age = 28.8 years, SD = 3.9, range = 22–37; 465 women) from the Human Connectome Project (www.humanconnectome.org; HCP900 release). The subjects were selected on the basis of having four complete resting-state scans and com- plete behavioral metrics for the cognitive features of interest. The BOLD fMRI data were acquired in four 15-min blocks. For the analysis, we used data cleaned using the FIX soft- ware (Glasser et al., 2013; Griffanti et al., 2014; Moeller et al., 2010; Setsompop et al., 2012; Van Essen et al., 2013). The cerebral cortex was parcellated into 333 functionally defined regions (Gordon et al., 2014). Fourteen subject-specific subcortical regions were added using Freesurfer-derived seg- mentation (Fischl et al., 2002) of bilateral thalamus, caudate, putamen, amygdala, hippocam- pus, pallidum, and nucleus accumbens. Fourteen cerebellar regions were also added (Buckner et al., 2011). This combined parcellation scheme covering the full cortex, subcortical struc- tures, and the cerebellum comprised a total of 361 regions. BOLD time series for each region of interest (ROI) were extracted, and Fisher’s r-to-z transformed Pearson correlation coefficients were obtained for each pair of ROIs in each 15-min block for each subject. The resulting 361 × 361 matrices were averaged across the four blocks for each subject and subsequently averaged across all subjects to obtain a group-level functional connectivity matrix. Spectral decomposition of RSFC data produces functionally orthogonal principal compo- nents (PCs). These components are synonymous with eigenvectors of RSFC matrices and iden- tify covariance patterns in the functional brain data. As such, PCs from group-mean and Network Neuroscience 202 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain single-subject RSFC matrices represent a set of hierarchically organized intrinsic brain net- works, ranked by the amount of signal variance described by each component. A functional network thus defined is a set of multivariate patterns that explain persistent synchrony be- tween distributed brain regions during rest. In addition to identifying RSFC networks, spectral decomposition also calculates a singular value, alternatively called an eigenvalue, for each principal component vector. These are single numerical scores for the principal-component vector length and are proportional to the amount of total variance in the RSFC matrix accounted for by that component. Principal components were identified using singular value decomposition of the 361 × 361 functional connectivity matrices (corresponding to the cortical-and-subcortical parcellation scheme; see above). The first ten PCs were calculated from the group mean connectivity matrix and back-projected onto anatomical space (Figure 1A). The first 20 PCs were identified in the same manner for each subject. Spatial correlations between the individual and group PCs were calculated using Pearson correlations. The single-subject PCs with the highest spatial correlation to group PCs 1–10 were determined, in order to assess the homogeneity of the single-subject PCs across the population (Figure 1B). The homogeneity of PCs is an indicator of network stability and replicability across the study population. We selected the most stable, or homogeneous, PCs in our sample for the subsequent brain and behavioral analyses—that is, PCs 1 to 10. The reliability of the PC network architecture across the group was assessed by one-sample t tests (Figure 1A), family-wise error-corrected for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05). Network synchrony, the core measure of network architecture in the present study, is repre- sented by the eigenvalue of each PC. We also calculate a scaled eigenvalue for each PC, rep- resenting the relative dominance of that network within the network hierarchy. All scaled eigenvalues represent the connectivity strength of that network relative to the dominant com- ponent (PC 1). PCs with higher eigenvalues (and scaled eigenvalues) are more prominent within an individual’s resting-state network architecture. Greater prominence suggests that the regions that compose a functional network demonstrate more persistently synchronous activity across the resting-state time series and explain more of the variance of temporal fluctuations than do lower-ranked networks. PC 1 constitutes the highest-ranked component and is re- ferred to as the global synchrony component. As we noted above, all scaled eigenvalues are scaled relative to this global synchrony component and reflect individual differences in the organization, or relative strengths, of networks within the overall network hierarchy. Behavioral Data We used the HCP behavioral measures related to cognition for our analysis. The battery includes measures of cognitive flexibility (Gershon et al., 2013), continuous performance (Gur et al., 2001), delay discounting (Estle, Green, Myerson, & Holt, 2006), executive inhibi- tion (Gershon et al., 2013), working memory (Gershon et al., 2013), episodic memory (Gershon et al., 2013), language comprehension (Gershon et al., 2013), processing speed (Gershon et al., 2013), fluid intelligence (Bilker et al., 2012), language decoding (Gershon et al., 2013), spatial orientation (Gur et al., 2001), and verbal memory (Gur et al., 2001). These HCP behavioral measures largely consist of tests developed and validated for the NIH Toolbox (Gershon et al., 2013). A full description of these measures, including assessment protocols, normative data samples, and psychometric properties, may be found at www.healthmeasures. net/explore-measurement-systems/nih-toolbox). Additional measures were included in the HCP behavioral battery to further explore the dimensions of human social, emotional, Network Neuroscience 203 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / / t e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain and cognitive function. For full data on these additional measures, please see www. humanconnectome.org; HCP900 release. Behavioral data were collected from HCP partner sites, including Washington University and the University of Minnesota. Behavior/RSFC Eigenvalue and Scaled-Eigenvalue Correlations To investigate whether individual differences in resting-state functional synchrony reliably pre- dicted intellectual functioning, we divided the available 830 participants’ data into two sets: a training set (n = 600) and a testing set (n = 230). Spearman correlation coefficients were calculated between the individual subjects’ scores for behavior and the eigenvalues for PCs 1–10 in the training set (n = 600, Figure 2) to allow for nonnormal distributions in the behavior scores. As described above, we calculated the scaled eigenvalue, based on each individual’s global synchrony component, for each network component by dividing all the eigenvalues in an individual’s resting-state network profile by the eigenvalue of PC 1. The scaled eigenvalues for PCs 2–10 were correlated with the cognitive measures using Spearman correlation (n = 600, Figure 3). We report all correlations with 95% confidence intervals that did not include zero and with p-values that survived multiple-comparison correction (Figure 3). Behavior/RSFC Network Interactions Given our overall prediction that human intelligence may be best reflected in the dynamic net- work architecture of the brain, we hypothesized that fluid intelligence and cognitive flexibility may be more strongly associated with dynamic network interactions than with synchrony sim- ply within specific networks. As such, we undertook a secondary brain and behavioral analysis, using the products of individual eigenvalues, rather than a linear model, to predict cognitive performance. We used a least-absolute-squares shrinkage operator (LASSO) with the training set (n = 600) to optimize the selection of network interactions—that is, the eigenvalue product terms—to be used in the predictor model. The designation of 600 participants in the training set and 230 par- ticipants in the testing set was based on a 70%–30% partition scheme for machine-learning predictions (Weinberger, Blitzer, & Saul, 2005). We evaluated all possible combinations of networks for the RSFC principal components that correlated significantly with the cognitive measures (see above). Eight significant PC features met these criteria for fluid intelligence (Figure 2), and ten significant PC features met these criteria for cognitive flexibility (Figures 2 and 3). As such, there were 256 possible unique combinations of network interactions for networks covarying with fluid-intelligence features (28), and 1,024 possible combinations of network interactions for networks covarying with cognitive-flexibility features (210). LASSO regression (MATLAB, version 2012b, statistical toolbox) optimized the data-fitting between eigenvalue products and fluid-intelligence scores in the training set (n = 600) to 33 unique combinations of network interactions that could be used as predictor terms. Coefficients and intercepts from the training set for these 33 network interactions were used as predictors in the independent testing set (n = 230). Of the 1,024 possible network interactions between the cognitive-flexibility RSFC features, LASSO regression optimized the data-fitting in the training set to 48 unique combinations of network interactions that could be used as predictor terms. The coefficients and intercepts for these interactions were used to calculate predicted values for fluid intelligence and cognitive flexibility in the independent testing set (n = 230). The volume of an abstract, n-dimensional eigensubspace (a.k.a., a “hypervolume”) may be determined by the products of orthogonal eigenvector magnitude—that is, by multiplying eigenvalues. Ulti- mately, each of the various eigensubspace terms identified by LASSO may prove to represent Network Neuroscience 204 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain a discrete, dissociable neurobiological contribution to the emergent phenomenon of general intelligence. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS the NIH Blueprint Data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU–Minn Consortium (PIs: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; Grant No. 1U54MH091657), funded by the 16 NIH institutes for Neuroscience Research; and by the and centers that support McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. Support for the anal- ysis was provided by the National Institute of Mental Health (Grant No. K08 MH092697) and the Alzheimer’s Association (Grant No. NIRG-14-320049) to R.N.S. We are grateful to Elizabeth DuPre, Karen Spreng, and Gary R. Turner for helpful advice and discussion in the writing of the manuscript. AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS Michael A. Ferguson: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Software; Visualization; Writing – original draft Jeffrey S. Anderson: Data curation; Formal analysis; Methodology; Writing – review & editing R. Nathan Spreng: Conceptualization; Methodology; Supervision; Writing – review & editing REFERENCES Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Work- ing memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60. doi:10.1037/0033-2909. 131.1.30 Alter, O., Brown, P. O., & Botstein, D. (2000). Singular value de- composition for genome-wide expression data processing and modeling. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 10101–10106. Bassett, D. S., Wymbs, N. F., Porter, M. A., Mucha, P. J., Carlson, (2011). Dynamic reconfiguration of hu- J. M., & Grafton, S. T. man brain networks during learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 7641–7646. Basten, U., Hilger, K., & Fiebach, C. J. (2015). Where smart brains are different: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional and struc- Intelligence, 51, tural brain imaging studies on intelligence. 10–27. Beckmann, C. F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J. T., & Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 360, 1001–1013. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1634 Bilker, W. B., Hansen, J. A., Brensinger, C. M., Richard, J., Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (2012). Development of abbreviated nine- item forms of the Raven’s standard progressive matrices test. Assessment, 19, 354–369. doi:10.1177/1073191112446655 Braun, U., Muldoon, S. F., & Bassett, D. S. (2015). On human brain networks in health and disease. eLS, 1–9. doi:10.1002/ 9780470015902.a0025783 Buckner, R. L., Krienen, F. M., Castellanos, A., Diaz, J. C., & Yeo, (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum esti- Journal of Neuro- B. T. mated by intrinsic functional connectivity. physiology, 106, 2322–2345. doi:10.1152/jn.00339.2011 Bullmore, E., Barnes, A., Bassett, D. S., Fornito, A., Kitzbichler, M., Meunier, D., & Suckling, J. (2009). Generic aspects of complexity in brain imaging data and other biological systems. NeuroImage, 47, 1125–1134. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.032 Bullmore, E., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Morris, R. G., Williams, S. C., Gregory, L., Gray, J. A., & Brammer, M. J. (1996). Functional mag- netic resonance image analysis of a large-scale neurocognitive network. NeuroImage, 4, 16–33. doi:10.1006/nimg.1996.0026 Chen, A. C., Oathes, D. J., Chang, C., Bradley, T., Zhou, Z. W., Williams, L. M., . . . Etkin, A. (2013). Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and default-mode networks in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 19944–19949. doi:10.1073/pnas.1311772110 Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified sci- ence of the mind-brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cole, M. W., Ito, T., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Lateral prefrontal cortex contributes to fluid intelligence through multinetwork connec- tivity. Brain Connections, 5, 497–504. doi:10.1089/brain.2015. 0357 Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Multi-task connectivity reveals flex- ible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 1348–1355. doi:10.1038/nn.3470 Cole, M. W., Yarkoni, T., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2012). Global connectivity of prefrontal cortex predicts cog- Journal of Neuroscience, 32, nitive control and intelligence. 8988–8999. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0536-12.2012 Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 547–552. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10. 005 Network Neuroscience 205 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 201–211. doi:10.1038/nrn2793 DeYoung, C. G., Shamosh, N. A., Green, A. E., Braver, T. S., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Intellect as distinct from openness: Differences re- Journal of Personality and vealed by fmri of working memory. Social Psychology, 97, 883–892. doi:10.1037/a0016615 Duncan, J. (2005). Frontal lobe function and general intelligence: Why it matters. Cortex, 41, 215–217. Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 172–179. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.01. 004 Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., . . . Emslie, H. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457–460. doi:10.1126/science.289.5478.457 Edgin, J. O., Pennington, B. F., & Mervis, C. B. (2010). Neuro- psychological components of intellectual disability: The con- tributions of immediate, working, and associative memory. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 406–417. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01278.x Estle, S. J., Green, L., Myerson, J., & Holt, D. D. (2006). Differen- tial effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses. Memory & Cognition, 34, 914–928. doi:10. 3758/BF03193437 Fair, D. A., Schlaggar, B. L., Cohen, A. L., Miezin, F. M., Dosenbach, N. U., Wenger, K. K., . . . Petersen, S. E. (2007). A method for using blocked and event-related fMRI data to study “resting state” functional connectivity. NeuroImage, 35, 396–405. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.051 Ferguson, M. A., & Anderson, J. S. (2012). Dynamical stability of intrinsic connectivity networks. NeuroImage, 59, 4022–4031. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.062 Finn, E. S., Shen, X., Scheinost, D., Rosenberg, M. D., Huang, J., Chun, M. M., . . . Constable, R. T. (2015). Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain con- nectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 1664–1671. doi:10.1038/ nn.4135 Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., (2002). Whole brain segmen- Haselgrove, C., . . . Dale, A. M. tation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33, 341–355. Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Pro- ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 9673–9678. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504136102 Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Fletcher, P., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, (1996). Functional topography: Multidimensional scal- R. S. ing and functional connectivity in the brain. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 156–164. Furnham, A. (2008). Personality and intelligence at work: Explor- ing and explaining individual differences at work. London, UK: Routledge. Gershon, R. C., Wagster, M. V., Hendrie, H. C., Fox, N. A., Cook, K. F., & Nowinski, C. J. (2013). NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology, 80, S2–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e5f Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., . . . Consortium, W. U.-M. H. Fischl, B., Andersson, (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 105–124. doi:10.1016/ j.neuroimage.2013.04.127 J. L., Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Adeyemo, B., Huckins, J. F., Kelley, W. M., & Petersen, S. E. (2014). Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cerebral Cortex, bhu239. Gottfredson, L. S., & Deary, I. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 1–4. Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316–322. doi:10.1038/nn1014 Gregory, R. L., & Zangwill, O. L. (1987). The Oxford companion to the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Griffanti, L., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Beckmann, C. F., Auerbach, E. J., Douaud, G., Sexton, C. E., . . . Smith, S. M. (2014). ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage, 95, 232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034 Gur, R. C., Moberg, P. D., Turner, T. H., Bilker, W. B., Kohler, C., (2001). Computerized neurocogni- Siegel, S. J., & Gur, R. E. tive scanning: I. Methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychoparmacology, 25, 766–776. Hearne, L. J., Mattingley, J. B., & Cocchi, L. (2016). Functional brain networks related to individual differences in human intelligence at rest. Scientific Reports, 6, 32328. doi:10.1038/srep32328 Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integra- tion Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evi- dence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 135–154; discussion 154–187. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001185 Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic per- formance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 148–161. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.148 Langeslag, S. J., Schmidt, M., Ghassabian, A., Jaddoe, V. W., Hofman, A., van der Lugt, A., . . . White, T. J. (2013). Functional connectivity between parietal and frontal brain regions and intel- ligence in young children: The generation R study. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 3299–3307. doi:10.1002/hbm.22143 Lee, K. H., Choi, Y. Y., Gray, J. R., Cho, S. H., Chae, J. H., Lee, S., & Kim, K. (2006). Neural correlates of superior intelligence: Stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 29, 578–586. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.036 Li, Y., Liu, Y., Li, J., Qin, W., Li, K., Yu, C., & Jiang, T. (2009). Brain anatomical network and intelligence. PLoS Computational Biology, 5, e1000395. l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . / t / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d t . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 McIntosh, A. R. (1999). Mapping cognition to the brain through interactions. Memory, 7, 523–548. doi:10.1080/ neural 096582199387733 Moeller, S., Yacoub, E., Olman, C. A., Auerbach, E., Strupp, J., (2010). Multiband multislice GE-EPI Harel, N., & Ugurbil, K. Network Neuroscience 206 Aspects of intelligence reflected by network synchrony in the brain at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imag- ing with application to high spatial and temporal whole-brain fMRI. Magnetic Resonance Medicine, 63, 1144–1153. doi:10. 1002/mrm.22361 Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., & Carter, C. S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a super- ordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 241–268. Penke, L., Maniega, S. M., Bastin, M. E., Hernandez, M. V., Murray, C., Royle, N. A., . . . Deary, I. J. (2012). Brain white matter tract integrity as a neural foundation for general intelligence. Molec- ular Psychiatry, 17, 1026–1030. Pineda-Pardo, J. A., Martínez, K., Román, F. J., & Colom, R. (2016). Structural efficiency within a parieto-frontal network and cogni- tive differences. Intelligence, 54, 105–116. Santarnecchi, E., Rossi, S., & Rossi, A. (2015). The smarter, the stronger: Intelligence level correlates with brain resilience to systematic insults. Cortex, 64, 293–309. doi:10.1016/j.cortex. 2014.11.005 Schultz, D. H., & Cole, M. W. (2016). Higher intelligence is asso- ciated with less task-related brain network reconfiguration. Jour- nal of Neuroscience, 36, 8551–8561. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI. 0358-16.2016 Schutte, N. S., & Loi, N. (2014). Connections between emotional intelligence and workplace flourishing. Personality and Individ- ual Differences, 66, 134–139. Scott, W. A. (1962). Cognitive complexity and cognitive flexibility. American Sociological Association, 25, 405–414. Setsompop, K., Gagoski, B. A., Polimeni, J. R., Witzel, T., Wedeen, (2012). Blipped-controlled aliasing in par- V. J., & Wald, L. L. allel imaging for simultaneous multislice echo planar imaging with reduced g-factor penalty. Magnetic Resonance Medicine, 67, 1210–1224. doi:10.1002/mrm.23097 Smith, S. M., Beckmann, C. F., Andersson, J., Auerbach, E. J., Bijsterbosch, J., Douaud, G., . . . Glasser, M. F. (2013). Resting- state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 144–168. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039 Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., (2009). Correspon- . Beckmann, C. F. Mackay, C. E., dence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 13040–13045. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905267106 . . Smith, S. M., Nichols, T. E., Vidaurre, D., Winkler, A. M., Behrens, T. E., Glasser, M. F., . . . Miller, K. L. (2015). A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demo- graphics and behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 1565–1567. Song, M., Zhou, Y., Li, J., Liu, Y., Tian, L., Yu, C., & Jiang, T. (2008). Brain spontaneous functional connectivity and intelligence. NeuroImage, 41, 1168–1176. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008. 02.036 Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence,” objectively deter- mined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–292. Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London, UK: Macmillan. (2004). Sporns, O., Chialvo, D. R., Kaiser, M., & Hilgetag, C. C. Organization, development and function of complex brain net- works. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 418–425. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2004.07.008 Spreng, R. N., Sepulcre, J., Turner, G. R., Stevens, W. D., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Intrinsic architecture underlying the re- lations among the default, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neu- roscience, 25, 74–86. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00281 Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. K. (1986). What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Stevens, W. D., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). Resting-state functional connectivity MRI reveals active processes central to cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5, 233–245. doi:10.1002/wcs.1275 van den Heuvel, M. P., Stam, C. J., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 7619–7624. doi:10. 1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-09.2009 Van Essen, D. C., Smith, S. M., Barch, D. M., Behrens, T. E., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K., & the WU–Minn H.C.P. Consortium. (2013). The WU–Minn Human Connectome Project: An over- view. NeuroImage, 80, 62–79. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013. 05.041 Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 3328–3342. doi:10.1152/jn.90355.2008 Weinberger, K. Q., Blitzer, J., & Saul, L. K. (2006). Distance met- ric learning for large margin nearest neighbor classification. In Y. Weiss, B. Schoelkopf, & J. Platt (Eds.), Advances in neural in- formation processing systems (pp. 1473–1480). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., . . . Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional con- nectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 1125–1165. doi:10. 1152/jn.00338.2011 Yeung, M. K., Tegner, J., & Collins, J. J. (2002). Reverse engineering gene networks using singular value decomposition and robust re- gression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 6163–6168. doi:10.1073/pnas.092576199 Network Neuroscience 207 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . t / / e d u n e n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / / 1 2 1 9 2 1 0 9 1 9 2 5 n e n _ a _ 0 0 0 1 0 p d . t f b y g u e s t t o n 0 7 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3imagen de INVESTIGACIÓN
imagen de INVESTIGACIÓN
imagen de INVESTIGACIÓN
imagen de INVESTIGACIÓN
imagen de INVESTIGACIÓN

Descargar PDF