La representación neuronal de los acontecimientos está dominada por

La representación neuronal de los acontecimientos está dominada por
Elements that Are Most Reliably Present

Konstantinos Bromis1*, Petar P. Raykov1*, Leah Wickens1,
Warrick Roseboom1,2, and Chris M. Bird1

Abstracto

■ An episodic memory is specific to an event that occurred at a
particular time and place. Sin embargo, the elements that constitute
the event—the location, the people present, and their actions
and goals—might be shared with numerous other similar
events. Does the brain preferentially represent certain elements
of a remembered event? En ese caso, which elements dominate its neu-
ral representation: those that are shared across similar events, o
the novel elements that define a specific event? We addressed
these questions by using a novel experimental paradigm com-
bined with fMRI. Multiple events were created involving conver-
sations between two individuals using the format of a television
chat show. Chat show “hosts” occurred repeatedly across multi-
ple events, whereas the “guests” were unique to only one event.

Before learning the conversations, participants were scanned
while viewing images or names of the (famous) individuals to
be used in the study to obtain person-specific activity patterns.
After learning all the conversations over a week, Participantes
were scanned for a second time while they recalled each event
multiple times. We found that during recall, person-specific
activity patterns within the posterior midline network were rein-
stated for the hosts of the shows but not the guests, y eso
reinstatement of the hosts was significantly stronger than the
reinstatement of the guests. These findings demonstrate that
it is the more generic, familiar, and predictable elements of an
event that dominate its neural representation compared with
the more idiosyncratic, event-defining, elementos. ■

INTRODUCCIÓN

Our lives progress through a series of unique events.
When we remember these events, we reactivate their neu-
ral representations. Although the events themselves are
unique, defined by the combination of what happened
where and when, many will share common elements, semejante
as the same people or location. It is not known how such
repeated elements are activated during memory recall. Do
the predictable and repeated elements of an event domi-
nate its neural representation? En ese caso, this might provide a
structure to retrieve less predictable, more idiosyncratic
elementos. Alternativamente, are the unique elements that dis-
tinguish similar events represented more robustly? El
current study aimed to address these questions.

When humans experience or recall an event, un
event-specific pattern of brain activity—as measured using
fMRI—is elicited within regions of the cortex (Raykov,
Keidel, Oakhill, & Bird, 2021; Chen et al., 2017; Oedekoven,
Keidel, Berens, & Bird, 2017; Sotavento & kühl, 2016; Bird,
Keidel, Ing, Horner, & Burgess, 2015; St-Laurent, Abdi,
& Buchsbaum, 2015; kühl & Chun, 2014). Estos
effects are observed most frequently in the brain’s poste-
rior medial network (Cooper & Ritchey, 2020; Ranganath

1escuela de psicologia, University of Sussex, 2School of Engi-
neering and Informatics, University of Sussex
*Co-first authorship.

& Ritchey, 2012), and although they reflect the neural rep-
resentation of an event, it is unclear what drives them.
Under the view that episodic memory is holistic in nature
(Tulving, 1983), it might be expected that all elements that
comprise an event contribute equally. Some recent evi-
dence speaks against that because the spatial context that
an event takes place in has been shown to be a major fac-
tor in determining its neural representation (Robin,
Buchsbaum, & Moscovitch, 2018). en este estudio, nosotros
wanted to broadly compare nonspatial elements that
were repeated, familiar, and more predictable with those
that were event-unique, unfamiliar, and less expected.

Elements that are repeatedly encountered across events
inevitably become more familiar to us and also more pre-
dictable. Any viewer of the TV show “Friends” will become
familiar with the six main characters and would expect
them to feature in any new episode. Predictive coding
accounts of perception argue that we generate predictions
that serve to “explain away” variance in incoming sensory
información (clark, 2013). De este modo, during perception, el
activity of neuronal populations that code the predictable
features of an event are suppressed, whereas those that
code the unexpected or novel features are enhanced
(Sohoglu & davis, 2020; Aitchison & Lengyel, 2017;
Friston, 2005). En tono rimbombante, the enhanced elements of
our perceptual experience are also encoded better into
memory, consistent with the view that “prediction errors”

© 2022 Instituto de Tecnología de Massachusetts. Published under a
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Internacional (CC POR 4.0) licencia.

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva 34:3, páginas. 517–531
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01802

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

drive new learning (Quent, Henson, & Greve, 2021; NVI &
Schoenbaum, 2008). Given that the brain appears to prior-
itize unfamiliar and unexpected information, Podríamos
expect that when we recall an event, it is the less predict-
able elements that dominate our representation of the
evento. Por ejemplo, if we recall a specific episode of
“Friends” that involved the unexpected arrival of one of
the sisters of one of the main characters, our representa-
tion of this individual might be more robust than the
otros, as her presence is a key identifying element of this
particular episode.

Memory recall has also been argued to play a role in
updating our internal model, by the “off-line” generation
of fictive prediction errors (barrón, Auksztulewicz, &
Friston, 2020; see also Hinton, Dayán, Frey, & Neal,
1995). More generally, memory recall has long been
thought to involve reinstating both the processes and rep-
resentations that were active during encoding (Nyberg,
Habib, McIntosh, & Tulving, 2000; morris, Bransford, &
Franks, 1977). It is therefore plausible that memory
recall—similarly to memory encoding—might be biased
toward reinstating representations of the more unex-
pected elements of events (see Wittkuhn, Chien, Sala-
McMaster, & Schuck,2021).

Sin embargo, it has been argued that recent repeated expe-
riences are most useful in predicting future experiences
because they are more likely to be encountered again
(anderson & Milson, 1989). Por el contrario, idiosyncratic
experiences are poor for making generalizations about
the future (sherman & Turk-Browne, 2020). Behaviorally,
it is well established that prior knowledge exerts a strong
influence over what aspects of an event are recalled
(Popov & Reder, 2020; Herrero, Hasinski, & Montaña de cedro,
2013; Poppenk, Köhler, & Moscovitch, 2010; Cervecero &
Treyens, 1981). Respectivamente, we may expect that it is those
elements of an event that are more reliably present that
will dominate its neural representation.

More generally, it is well established that repeated expo-
sure to the same material is beneficial for memory ( Van
Strien, Hagenbeek, estampar, Rombouts, & Barkhof, 2005;
Glenberg, Herrero, & Verde, 1977). Sin embargo, the situation
is less clear-cut when the same items are repeatedly
encoded in different contexts. Aquí, although the items
may become more familiar and are better recognized,
their associations with the contexts that they were experi-
enced in can weaken (Sievers, Bird, & Renoult, 2019; Yassa
& Reagh, 2013).

Although repetition can result in better memory, es
also the case that more novel and distinctive items are also
better remembered (Hunt, 1995). This memory advantage
for more distinct items affects both recollection- y
familiarity-based recognition judgments (Kishiyama &
Yonelinas, 2003). Además, novelty can act at the level
of a stimulus or whether a particular stimulus is novel
within a specific context (Ranganath & Rainer, 2003).
Tomados juntos, there is substantial evidence that two
broad factors can determine the degree to which elements

of an event are more memorable. Por un lado, allá
are items that are reliably present and predictable—often
as a result as having been frequently encountered in the
same context. Por otro lado, there are items that
are more idiosyncratic and unexpected—perhaps only
occurring once in a context and therefore uniquely identi-
fying an event. These factors have different effects on
memory in different situations, indeed there are situations
where memory is superior both for highly unexpected and
highly expected events (Quent, Henson, & Greve, 2021).
Our focus in this study is how these factors affect the neu-
ral representation of complex naturalistic events. Specifi-
cally, if a complex memory can be accurately recalled, es
it those elements that are most reliably present that dom-
inate the neural representation or is it the less familiar
events that are uniquely associated with an event?

We first recorded fMRI patterns of activity elicited when
participants viewed images or read names of famous celeb-
rities. Following this, participants learned nine fictional
conversations between two celebrities set within a televi-
sion “chat show” format. The repeated elements were the
hosts of the shows (three in total), whereas the unique ele-
ments were the guests (nine in total). After they had learned
all of the conversations, participants then repeatedly
recalled them in a second fMRI session. By using the
person-specific patterns of activity from Session 1, nosotros
were able to examine whether the hosts or the guests
were more robustly reinstated when participants recalled
the conversations. All analyses were performed on ROIs
from the posterior midline (PM) network identified in
a previous study of event memory (Robin, 2018).

MÉTODOS

Participantes

Thirty-one (21 women, 10 hombres) participants took part
in the experiment. Participants were aged 19–30 years
(mean = 24 años, DE = 3.54 años) and did not have a his-
tory of any psychiatric or neurological disorders. All partic-
ipants were right-handed and were fluent English
speakers. Four participants were excluded from any anal-
yses: two did not complete the experiment, one because
of excessive movement in the scanner, and one for failing
to recall three of the guests during the memory screening
prueba. Por lo tanto, data from 27 participants were analyzed.
Note three participants had corrupted audio recording
and were not included in reports of the memory screening
prueba. This sample size is consistent with previous studies
examining fMRI pattern effects about stories or person
decoding (Raykov, Keidel, Oakhill, & Bird, 2020; di Oleggio
Castello, Halchenko, Guntupalli, Gors, & Gobbini, 2017;
Zadbood, Chen, Leong, Norman, & Hasson, 2017).
Informed consent was obtained from all participants
before the experiment, and they were reimbursed £40
for their time. This project was approved by the Brighton
and Sussex Medical School Research Governance and
Ethics Committee.

518

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva

Volumen 34, Número 3

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

We also collected follow-up data from a group of sepa-
rate participants who completed a similar task online. Este
group comprised 37 Participantes (20 women, 17 hombres)
with a mean age of 24.61 años (±4.94 years). We excluded
one participant who failed to learn the conversations after
five learning sessions. Por lo tanto, 36 participants from the
online study were included in the analyses. Participantes
received payment through online recruitment platform
Prolific (https://www.prolific.co/) for each of the learning
sessions. The project was approved by the University of
Sussex Cross School Research Ethics Committee.

Estímulos

We ran a pilot study to select 12 celebrities who would be
familiar to our sample (see https://osf.io/zpcv3/). Forty-
eight pictures of 12 (six men, six women) famous individ-
uals were used in Session 1. Four different pictures
sourced from Google Images were used for each
famous individual. The pictures only showed the famous
individual and were converted to grayscale using Adobe
Photoshop CC19. Además, five pictures of nonfamous
individuals were used in the experiment.

Nine short fictitious conversations were written by the
research team (see https://osf.io/zpcv3/). These were
learned after Session 1 and recalled in the scanner in
Session 2. The conversations were relatively short (162.5 ±
16.1 words on average) and took the form of chat show
conversations using a question-and-answer structure.
The conversations involved two people, a guest, y un
host, taken from the 12 famous celebrities. De término medio,
a similar number of words were “spoken” by the hosts
(mean = 80.7 palabras) and the guests (mean = 81.7).
The topic of each conversation was unique (p.ej., donación
to charity, redes sociales; see https://osf.io/zpcv3/ for the
complete transcripts of all conversations). Each conversa-
tion was associated with a particular day (Monday,
Wednesday, or Friday) and week ( Week 1, 2, o 3). Partic-
ipants were required to learn when each conversation
took place, as this was how memory would be cued in
the scanner. Each of the three hosts was associated with
three conversations that occurred on a specific day of
the week (Host 1, Mondays; Host 2, Wednesdays; y
Host 3, Fridays; ver figura 1). The guests were only asso-
ciated with one of the nine conversations. A pilot experi-
ment with an independent group of participants ensured
that there were not substantial differences between the
conversations in how memorable and interesting they
eran (see https://osf.io/zpcv3/). A picture was created
using Photoshop to illustrate the context of each conver-
sation and to make them appear more plausible. El
picture presented the host and the guest sitting in a TV
studio (see Figure 1C). Three background pictures of TV
studios were used overall, which were consistent for the
three hosts (p.ej., Studio 1 for Mondays, Studio 2 para
Wednesdays). Three counterbalancing lists were created

where the identity of hosts and guests was varied across
Participantes.

Procedimiento

fMRI Study

Prescanning. Before taking part in the fMRI experiment,
participants completed a short online questionnaire
establishing their knowledge of the famous individuals
used in the experiment (see https://osf.io/zpcv3/). Participe-
pants rated the following from 1–5: how familiar they are
with the person, how well they can imagine them, cómo
much they know about their career and/or personal life,
and whether they like the person. Los participantes fueron
selected if they responded to be familiar (p.ej., responded
arriba 3) con el 12 celebrities used in the experiment.
Participants were also encouraged to learn more about
these celebrities before the experiment. Participants took
part in two fMRI sessions spaced approximately 7 días
apart (ver figura 1).

Scanning Session 1

In Session 1, images and names of 12 famous individuals
were presented in a blocked fMRI design. There were five
runs in total. Each run contained 24 blocks of trials, com-
prising one image and one text block for each of the 12
celebrities. In an image block, participants saw the four dif-
ferent images of the same individual (p.ej., Daniel Craig).
Each of the four pictures were presented twice within a
single block, resulting in eight images per block (ver
Cifra 1). The presentation order within a block was ran-
domized. Each picture was presented for 800 msec with a
200-msec gap, during which a white fixation cross was
mostrado. Each block lasted 8 segundo. In a text block, Participantes
saw multiple presentations of the name of 1 del 12
famous individuals. Each text presentation was 800 mseg,
and there was a 200-msec ISI. To increase engagement
with each stimulus, the font color and text position for
each name presentation was varied. Each image and text
name were presented on a black background and presen-
tation of identities across blocks was randomized.

In between picture and text blocks, participants per-
formed an odd–even number judgment task. This served
as an active baseline task. Aquí, participants saw a
sequence of four numbers randomly selected from the
range 1–98. Each number was presented for 1.8 sec and
was followed by a white fixation cross for 700 mseg. Allá-
delantero, a block of the odd–even task lasted 10 segundo. The odd–
even task was followed by a red fixation cross presented
para 400 msec signaling the upcoming identity block. Par-
ticipants performed an odd-ball detection task on the
image blocks. In each run, there was an additional image
block that included a picture of an unfamiliar individual
embedded in the sequence of pictures of a famous indi-
individuales. These odd-ball blocks could appear at any point in

Bromis et al.

519

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

Cifra 1. Study design.
Participants took part in two
fMRI sessions spaced a week
apart. (A) In Session 1,
participants viewed pictures and
names of famous individuals in
a blocked design (block length =
8 segundo). Había 12 unique
identidades, and within each run,
participants saw one picture and
one name block for each
identity. There were five runs in
total. (B) After Session 1,
participants were provided with
nine brief conversations to study
at their own leisure. Cada
conversation included a host
and a guest discussing a
particular topic and was
associated with day and week.
The hosts were common to
three conversations, mientras
the guests were always unique.
(C) An image of the host and
guest in a studio was provided to
help participants visualize the
conversaciones. Conversations
were shown in the format of a
script (see https://osf.io/zpcv3/).
(D) In Session 2, Participantes
recalled the conversations
following a date cue. Cada
conversation occurred once in
each run, and there were six runs
en total.

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

the run apart from the first image block and were not
included in the main analyses. Identities were presented
in a randomized order.

Learning Phase

After completing scanning Session 1, participants were
provided with a list with the nine conversations involving
el 12 celebrities (ver figura 1). Participants were asked
to learn each conversation and were encouraged to visual-
ize each one, using the images provided. Participantes
were instructed to learn the conversations well and were
told that their memory for the conversations would be
tested before being allowed to take part in the second
scanning session. They were given 4–6 days to learn the
conversaciones.

Following this, participants underwent a screening
test to ensure they had learned the conversations
before proceeding to the second scanning session. El
test was carried out at least 1 day before the scanning ses-
sión. Participants freely recalled all of the nine conversa-
tions in a random order, cued by the week and the day.
The experimenter provided feedback on parts of the con-
versations that the participants had failed to recall, y
all participants were asked to review the conversations
again before scanning.

Immediately before the second fMRI session, participar-
pants also provided subjective ratings on their memory
for the conversations. They rated on a scale of 0–100
how vividly and confidently they could remember the
conversaciones. They also rated how engaging they found
the conversations.

520

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva

Volumen 34, Número 3

Scanning Session 2

In Session 2, while in the scanner, participants were asked
to recollect the nine conversations they had previously
learned. There were six runs in total, and within each
run, participants recollected all nine of the conversations.
Participants were presented with the time information
(Monday first week) to cue their memory for the specific
conversation. The cue was presented for 3 sec and was
followed by a 15-sec recollection period. Los participantes fueron
asked to remember the conversation in as much detail
as possible for the 15 segundo. Participants were provided with
the option to press a button if they failed to recollect the
conversation during that particular memory trial (p.ej.,
debido a la distracción mental). All such events were
removed from the main analyses. In between memory
bloques, participants performed the odd–even number
judgment task (described above) para 15 segundo, which served
as active baseline. A red fixation cross followed the
number judgment task and signaled the upcoming recall
block (ver figura 1). Within each run, conversation
memory trials were randomized.

Online Study

In a follow-up online study, we asked a separate group of
participants to learn the nine conversations used in the
fMRI experiment and provide additional subjective ratings
about the conversations.

Similarly to the fMRI experiment, we initially pre-
screened participants to be familiar with the 12 celebrities
used in the experiment. Participants who were not familiar
with any of the 12 celebrities were not included in the
main experiment and were not included in the further
learning sessions.

Participants completed five learning sessions over a
week, each session released on a separate day. During
the learning sessions, participants read each of the nine
conversations at their leisure. The event cue (p.ej., Monday
primero) and the photoshopped studio picture showing
the host and the guest were presented to participants as
they read the conversations during each session. Después
completing the reading session, participants answered
multiple-choice questions about the conversations. El
multiple-choice questions differed on each learning
session and aimed to help participants learn the conversa-
ciones. The questions concerned the identities of the hosts
and guests, what they were talking about, and details from
the conversations. After the fifth learning session, participar-
pants provided subjective ratings on how confidently and
vividly they could remember each of the conversations.
Participants also rated how confidently they could remem-
ber the host and the guest for each conversation and how
important they perceived each one was for the conversa-
ción. Además, participants were asked to guess who
the host and the guest on a hypothetical conversation
happening on Week 4 (Monday, Wednesday, and Friday)

would be. This was an open-ended question, and partici-
pants were instructed to make a guess or write they do
not know.

MRI Acquisition

All images were acquired on a 3-T Siemens Prisma scanner
with a 32-channel head coil. To minimize head movement,
soft cushions were inserted into the head coil. Funcional
images were acquired with a gradient-echo EPI sequence
with multiband acceleration factor of 8 with the following
parámetros (repetition time = 0.8 segundo, echo time =
33.1 mseg, 52° flip angle, campo de visión = 208 × 180 mm,
72 slices with sliced thickness of 2 mm and isotropic
2 mm voxels). Two SpinEcho fieldmap runs with reversed
phase-encode blips in both anterior to posterior and poste-
rior to anterior were acquired with the same parameters as
the functional images. Separate field maps were acquired
for Sessions 1 y 2. A high-resolution T1-weighted image
was acquired with 3-D MPRAGE sequence (repetition
time = 2.4 segundo, echo time = 2.14 segundo, 8° flip angle, campo
of view = 224 × 224mm and 0.8 mm isotropic voxels).

Image Preprocessing

SPM 12 ( Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience)
was used to preprocess all images, except for the field
maps. For each session, we first spatially realigned the
functional images to the mean session image. Session-
specific field maps were estimated with command line
functions from FSL (Smith et al., 2004) and were applied
to the motion-corrected data to correct for image distor-
ciones (Hutton et al., 2002). The anatomical image was
segmented into gray, white, and cerebrospinal fluid using
tissue probability maps, and was coregistered to the mean
functional image. The segmented images were used to
estimate deformation fields, which were applied to the
functional data to transform them to Montreal Neurologi-
cal Institute space. A 3-mm smoothing FWHM Gaussian
kernel was applied to the data as recommended by
previous work, showing that a small amount of smoothing
can improve sensitivity of multivoxel pattern analyses
(Hendriks, Daniels, Pegado, & Op de Beeck, 2017;
Gardumi et al., 2016).

Análisis de los datos

Datos de comportamiento
In both scanning sessions, participants completed an odd–
even number judgment task that acted as an active base-
line. We analyzed accuracy and RTs from both sessions to
ensure participants were paying attention throughout the
main tasks.

We carried out a post hoc analysis of data from the
screening test before the second scanning session. Este
analysis used the well-established procedure for scoring

Bromis et al.

521

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

performance on tests of prose recall (p.ej., Wechsler,
1945). The script for each conversation was divided
into discrete “idea units,” and a point was allocated for
successfully recalling each unit. For each conversation,
we scored units spoken by the host and the guest sepa-
rately. Examples of the full script divided into units and
participants’ recalled conversations are available online
(https://osf.io/zpcv3/).

During the second scanning session, participants could
press a button to indicate that they could not retrieve the
conversation on this trial. These trials were not included
into further analyses. Participants provided subjective
ratings on a scale of 0–100 on how vividly and confidently
they could recall the conversations. Participants also rated
how familiar and engaging they found the conversations.
These ratings were averaged across participants separately
for each conversation.

In the online study, a separate group of participants pro-
vided subjective confidence and vividness ratings about
the same conversations. Participants also provided subjec-
tive ratings in their confidence to remember the host and
the guest for each conversation and their importance.

MRI Data

We note that data from the last run of Session 2 de 3 afuera
del 27 participants were lost because of a technical
issue. Por lo tanto, these participants had only five runs
rather than six from Session 2. MRI data were analyzed
with SPM 12, the CosMoMVPA toolbox (Oosterhof,
Connolly, & Haxby, 2016), and custom scripts written in
MATLAB ( Version 2017b, The MathWorks, Cª). All analy-
ses were conducted on Montreal Neurological Institute
normalized images. The BrainNet Viewer toolbox was
used for visualizing the ROIs (see Figure 5A), y
bspmview toolbox was used for visualizing the whole-
brain parcellations reported in supplementary materials.

ROI Definition

We carried out our analyses on regions associated with the
PM network (see Introduction). The ROIs were taken from
a previous study of multielement event recall by Robin
et al. (2018). We used their multifeature ROI, which is a
set of regions comprising voxels that could classify
between different aspects of events—locations, gente,
and objects (see Figure 5A). This comprises five different
regiones: posterior medial cortex (PMC), dorsal medial pFC
(MPFC), left and right superior lateral occipital cortex
(LOC) extending to the angular gyrus in the lateral parietal
corteza (referred to as angular gyrus by Robin and col-
ligas), and left and right parahippocampal gyrus.

General Linear Models

To estimate activation patterns for later use in the repre-
sentational similarity analyses (RSAs), we used general

linear models (GLMs). In each run from Session 1, a
separate regressor for each block was included, semejante
that picture and text blocks were modeled separately
(es decir., 24 regressors per run). All trial regressors were
entered in a single first-level model as the least squares
all method described in Mumford, Tornero, Ashby, y
Poldrack (2012). The patterns (t maps) from picture
and text blocks for the same identity within each run
were then averaged. Por lo tanto, from all five runs, allá
eran 60 patrones (12 per run). After averaging the pic-
ture and text blocks, this resulted in five patterns for
each of the 12 famous individuals included in the exper-
mento. In Session 2, each retrieval trial block was modeled
with a separate regressor (nine per run) in a least squares
all approach. This resulted in 54 patterns for the nine con-
versations used (six per conversation). Separate regres-
sors of no interest for the six motion parameters, a session
constant term, and a high pass filter with cutoff of 1/128 Hz
were included in all GLMs.

RSA

We used the GLM estimated t maps as inputs to our RSAs.
A series of RSAs were carried out to examine whether
repeated features are more strongly represented during
recollection. Contrast matrices for each analysis are shown
En figura 4. All similarity matrices were estimated using
Correlación de Pearson, and all correlation values were Fisher
transformed before computing further contrasts. Group-
level one-sample t test against zero was used to examine
the significance of the RSA contrasts (un = .05).

Before investigating whether the hosts and/or the
guests were represented in the ROIs during memory
retrieval, we first established which ROIs distinguished
the identities of the celebrities as well as the conversa-
ciones. If an ROI could not discriminate between the celeb-
rities when presented in isolation, then it would not make
sense to seek evidence for identity-specific reactivation of
these patterns during memory retrieval. Similarmente, if an ROI
could not discriminate between the conversations them-
selves during retrieval, then it would not make sense to
look for reactivation of the identities of the people taking
part in the conversations (see Supplementary Figure 5,
available online at https://osf.io/zpcv3/). Por lo tanto, el
ROIs that we report showing a significant effect of “host”
or “guest” reinstatement had to not only pass the signifi-
cance threshold for these specific analyses but also show
significant effects in two additional independent analyses
(of “identity” and “conversation,” see below).

Identity. Primero, we examined which regions show reliable
identity-specific patterns (see Figure 4A). For this analysis,
only patterns from Session 1 were used. For each ROI, spa-
tial patterns of activity (t maps) for each identity were
extraído, vectorized, and used to construct an RSA matrix.
Patterns for the same identity in odd numbered runs and
even numbered runs were separately averaged. Este

522

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva

Volumen 34, Número 3

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

resulted in 12 identity patterns estimated from the odd
runs and 12 identity patterns estimated from the even runs.
The pairwise similarities between all of these patterns were
used to produce a 12 × 12 correlation matrix. La resultante
RSA matrix represents the neural similarity between the 12
identidades. The diagonal values represent the matching
identities across runs, y el 132 off-diagonal values rep-
resent the similarity between nonmatching identities. A
examine which regions show reliable identity-specific
patrones, the mean average similarity between matching
identities versus the mean average similarity between
nonmatching identities was compared. ROIs that did
not show reliable identity-specific patterns were not
included into further analyses. Results from all ROIs are
available online at https://osf.io/zpcv3/.

Segundo, we examined whether the
Conversation.
remaining ROIs would exhibit reliable conversation-
specific effects. Using a similar logic, patterns for each
conversation across odd and even numbered runs were
separately averaged. Their similarity was then computed
using Pearson correlation, which resulted in a 9 × 9 similar-
ity matrix. Diagonal values represented similarity between
patterns of matching conversations, whereas off-diagonal
values represented values of mismatching conversations.
The mean matching similarity with the mean nonmatch-
ing similarity across conversations was compared. ROI
that did not show reliable conversation-specific patterns
were also not included into further analyses.

In the host RSA, the patterns from Session 1 para
Hosts.
the host identities were averaged across all runs. Similarmente,
all conversation patterns, from Session 2, were averaged
across all runs. The correlation between the host patterns
and the conversations was computed. The correlation
matrix was constructed such that the diagonal values rep-
resented the correlation between host identity (p.ej.,
Jennifer Aniston pattern from Session 1) and the con-
versations with matching host. The off-diagonal values
represented correlation between host identity and mis-
matching conversations (p.ej., where Jennifer Aniston was
not the host; see Figure 4C). Note that the host patterns
were repeated within the correlation matrix, y ahí-
delantero, some off-diagonal values were not included into
the analyses. The contrast between matching host to con-
versation patterns and the mismatching patterns was
computed. This was done to examine whether host pat-
terns were reinstated during retrieval. ROIs that did not
show a significant host-specific reinstatement were not
removed from further analyses, as it was possible that they
would show a guest-specific reinstatement.

Guests. Después, the guest identity-specific patterns
from Session 1 were extracted and averaged them over
all runs. The similarity between guest patterns from Ses-
sión 1 and the conversation patterns from Session 2 era
computed. This resulted in a 9 × 9 correlation matrix

(see Figure 4D). The diagonal values represent similarity
between matching guest and conversation patterns (p.ej.,
Michelle Obama Session 1 pattern and conversation where
Michelle Obama was the guest—Monday first). El
off-diagonal values that were used as a contrast repre-
sented the similarity between a mismatching guest and
conversation. To keep the number of contrast values sim-
ilar to the host analysis, described above, we focused on
the values representing mismatch between guest and con-
versation, coming from the same show. Por ejemplo, el
mismatch values for Michelle Obama who was a guest on
Monday first week were conversations where she was not
the guest, but still happened on Mondays and had the same
host (p.ej., Monday second week and Monday third week).
To examine guest-specific reinstatement, the mean match-
ing guest to conversation similarity to the mean of the mis-
matching guest to conversation similarity was computed.

Simulations of Host and Guest RSAs

The RSAs for the hosts involve repeatedly correlating the
pattern of activity from Session 1 for each of the hosts with
three different conversations from Session 2. Por el contrario,
the RSAs for the guests involve unique pairwise correla-
tions between the pattern of activity for each guest and
their respective conversation. We wanted to check that
this procedure did not bias the analyses to find greater evi-
dence for reinstatement of either the hosts or the guests.
We therefore generated simulated patterns for the hosts
and guests and modeled the situation where both the host
and guest patterns were equally present in the pattern for
each conversation.

We first simulated 12 random patterns of the same
length as our PMC ROI. These patterns acted as the 12
celebrity patterns from Session 1. Three patterns were
taken to represent the hosts, and the other nine were
taken to represent the guests. We then simulated nine
conversation patterns that were linear combinations of
the host and guest patterns as well as noise (the correla-
tion between each conversation pattern and each of its
constituent “host” and “guest” pattern was assigned to
ser 0.2). In the first simulation, we added white noise
to the simulated conversations patterns. This allowed us
to control the similarity between the simulated identity
patterns and the simulated conversation patterns. A
match our design, three of the simulated conversation pat-
terns were associated with the same host (but a unique
guest). We then ran our planned “host” and “guest” RSA
comparisons on the simulated data (as illustrated in
Cifra 4). We ran these simulations 100,000 times each.

Próximo, we ran a further simulation using fMRI data to rule
out the possibility that correlated noise in the data would
bias the analyses to find greater evidence of reinstatement
for either the hosts or the guests. For each subject’s fMRI
datos, we averaged the conversation patterns across the dif-
ferent runs of Session 2 and constructed a correlation
matrix between the different conversation patterns. Nosotros

Bromis et al.

523

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

then Fisher-transformed these correlation matrixes and
created an average correlation matrix across subjects that
represented the similarity between the nine conversations
participants were remembering during Session 2. We used
this average correlation matrix across conversations to add
correlated noise to the simulated conversations. Specifi-
cally, aquí, we modeled the conversations as combinations
of simulated host and guest patterns. Además, nosotros
added simulated noise patterns that were drawn from a
multivariate Gaussian distribution with mean zero and cor-
relation matrix being equal to the empirical average corre-
lation matrix across conversations. We ran each simulation
100,000 veces.

Hosts versus guests. To examine which regions show dif-
ferential reinstatement for host and guest identities during
retrieval of conversations, we performed a paired t test.
Específicamente, we contrasted for each subject their host-
specific reinstatement effect with their guest-specific rein-
statement effect.

Time period analyses. We additionally ran post hoc
analyses to examine the time course of the host and guest
RSA effects. This examined whether any effects observed
in the main analyses evolve over time, and in particular,
whether the host effects are stronger during the initial
period of recall. To examine the time course of the host
and guest analyses, we ran three additional GLMs. Nosotros
modeled the first, middle, and last third of each of the con-
versations in separate GLMs. This allowed us to estimate
conversation patterns of brain activity that were specific
for the beginning, middle, and end of the 15-sec recall
período. We ran the host and guest analyses as described
previamente, but separately for each of the three patterns.
We also ran the conversation-specific analysis (ver
Figura 4B) that tested whether we could observe reliable
conversation-specific patterns.

Additional Analyses

En línea (https://osf.io/zpcv3/), we report two additional
fMRI analyses. In the first, we ran exploratory RSAs to
investigate reinstatement of the hosts and guests during
the conversations within a whole-brain parcellation (ver
Supplementary Figures 6 y 7 at https://osf.io/zpcv3/).
The main purpose of this analysis was to establish whether
any regions outside our predefined ROIs showed evidence
for reinstatement of the guests during recall of the conver-
sations. We therefore report results for the four main anal-
yses (ver figura 4) de 200 functionally defined regions
using a parcellation reported in Schaefer et al. (2018). Nosotros
also ran the four main analyses in a bilateral hippocampal
ROI (constructed using parcellations from Ritchey,
Montchal, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2015, and deposited
on https://neurovault.org/). Segundo, we used intersubject
pattern analysis to investigate whether the topics of the
conversations were represented in patterns of activity,

regardless of the identities of the hosts and guests. Este
analysis addresses the issue of how narrative information
is represented in the brain (see Supplementary Figure 9
at https://osf.io/zpcv3/).

RESULTADOS

Datos de comportamiento

fMRI Study

Before taking part in the experiment, participants were
asked to rate how familiar they are with the 12 celebrities
and how easily they could imagine them (rating data were
missing from three participants). Participantes, on average,
were familiar with the celebrities included in the experi-
mento (mean = 4.07, DE = 1.02, max = 5). One participant
initially reported relatively low familiarity with the celebri-
corbatas (mean = 2.66) and was asked to learn about each of
the celebrities by watching videos of them over the course
of a week.

In the first scanning session, participants were pre-
sented with an additional image block per run that
included a single picture of an unfamiliar person embed-
ded in a series of pictures of a famous celebrity. Participe-
pants, on average, identificado 38% (DE = 19%) of these
oddball blocks. A coding error prevented us from record-
ing responses to the oddball blocks that occurred after
a block has finished, so this may be an underestimate of
the true percentage of oddball blocks detected. Between
picture and text blocks, participants performed an odd–
even number judgment task that served as an active base-
line and attention check. De término medio, participants made
the odd–even judgments accurately at 97.6% (DE = 0.02,
RT = 790 mseg, DE = 101). Highly similar performance on
the odd–even judgment baseline task was also found
during the second scanning session (97%, DE = 0.03,
RT = 768 mseg, DE = 105).

The memory screening test before the second scanning
session revealed that all of the participants had learnt the
nine conversations well. All participants correctly identi-
fied both the host and the guest for all conversations
(one participant failed to identify the guest for three of
the conversations and their data were excluded from the
estudiar). Participants identified 6.04 (DE = 2.29) idea units
spoken by the hosts and 7.03 (DE = 2.04) idea units spo-
ken by the guests, a difference that is highly significant,
t(24) = −8.18, pag < .001 (see Figure 2). Thus, even though the conversations were constructed so that the hosts and guests spoke the same amount and both asked and responded to questions, participants recalled more dia- logue that was spoken by the guests than by the hosts. Immediately before scanning, participants rated that they could vividly and confidently remember the conver- sations. Furthermore, they rated them as being familiar and engaging (data available at https://osf.io/zpcv3/). During the second scanning session, participants could indicate if they did not recall a conversation in a specific trial. Participants rarely reported that they could not recall 524 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 34, Number 3 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 4 3 5 1 7 1 9 8 4 8 5 8 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 8 0 2 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 sample, we did not observe any significant differences in host versus guest confidence (93.55 ± 11.15 vs. 92.69 ± 12.42), t(416) = 0.78, p = .43, but the guests were still per- ceived to be more important than the hosts within each conversation (72.03 ± 22.30 vs. 67.96 ± 23.38), t(416) = −2.65, p = .008 (see Figure 3B). Last, when asked to guess who the host and guests would be on a following week, participants guessed 72.9% of the times that a following week will involve the same hosts that were repeated in the previous weekdays. For the guests, participants responded that they did not know who the guest would be for 73.83% of the future conversations and 25.2% guessed that the guest on Week 4 would be a celebrity that was not one of the previously seen celebrities. Only on 1% of trials did one participant guess that a previously seen guest would reappear on Week 4. Therefore, according to this measure, the hosts were more “predictable” than the guests. Figure 2. Objective memory was higher for information said by the guests in the fMRI study. Per subject average conversation units recalled separately for hosts and guests were shown. On average, participants remembered more of what the guest spoke. fMRI Data RSAs the specific conversation on a given trial. On average, participants had 1.71 (SD = 1.35) discarded trials out of 54, or 3.16%. Online Study A separate group of participants learned the same conver- sations online and provided behavioral ratings about their memory. Before completing the learning, sessions partic- ipants were screened to be familiar with each of the 12 celebrities. Participants completed five learning sessions. After the fifth session, participants answered multiple- choice questions about the conversations and provided subjective ratings about their memory. Participants showed overall good learning of the conver- sations after five sessions, with average accuracy being 95.38% (±5.72). Furthermore, participants had achieved ceiling performance for the host (100% ± 0%) and near ceiling performance for the guest (94.7% ± 9%) memory questions. Participants rated that they could confidently (75.75 ± 19.47) and vividly (75.03 ± 21.42) remember the conversations. When examining all participants, we observed that participants were more confident in their memory for the hosts (94.01 ± 10.41) compared with the guests (89.06 ± 16.92), t(603) = 4.33, p < .001. Inter- estingly, participants rated the guests as playing a more important role than the hosts in the conversations (71.96 ± 21.77 vs. 67.24 ± 21.84), t(603) = −3.68, p < .001 (see Figure 3A). To further investigate these effects and to provide a better comparison to the participants who took part in the main scanning experiment (who identified both the host and the guest of every conversa- tion), we analyzed data from participants who were able to identify all of the hosts and the guests (25 in total). In this Identity. We first examined which regions would show reliable identity-specific patterns in Session 1 when the celebrities were presented in isolation as both pictures and names. See https://osf.io/zpcv3/ for results from all ROIs. Reliable identity-specific decoding was observed Figure 3. Similar average confidence and importance ratings for host and guest in online study. (A) Memory confidence was slightly higher in the whole group that completed the online study. This included people who did not learn guests to a ceiling-level performance. (B) No memory confidence difference was seen for hosts and guests in participants who learned all conversations, similarly to the participants included in the fMRI study. Nonetheless participants still rated the guest as playing a more important role in the conversation. Bromis et al. 525 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 4 3 5 1 7 1 9 8 4 8 5 8 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 8 0 2 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 4 3 5 1 7 1 9 8 4 8 5 8 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 8 0 2 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 Figure 4. Contrast matrices demonstrating the different RSAs. Red indicates positive and blue indicates negative contrast weight. (A) Analysis of Session 1 to examine which regions show reliable identity-specific patterns. (B) We examined Session 2 data to demonstrate regions that would show reliable conversation-specific patterns. (C) We correlated patterns from Session 1 to Session 2 to examine host-specific reinstatement during the conversations. (D) Examined guest reinstatement in the conversations. in the left LOC, t(26) = 3.52, p = .001; PMC, t(26) = 2.47, p = .02; right LOC, t(26) = 2.54, p = .01; and right fusi- form cortex, t(26) = 3.62, p = .001 (see Figure 5B). The other regions, left parahippocampal cortex, t(26) = −0.59, p = .55, and MPFC, t(26) = 0.88, p = .38, did not show significant identity-specific patterns. Therefore, the regions taken forward to the next analysis were the PMC, the left and right LOC, and the right fusiform cortex. Conversations. We next examined which of the four regions taken forward from the previous analyses would show reliable conversation-specific patterns. See https:// osf.io/zpcv3/ for results from all four ROIs. The left LOC, t(26) = 6.10, p < .001; PMC, t(26) = 4.04, p < .001; and right LOC, t(26) = 4.06, p < .001, showed reliable conversation-specific patterns (see Figure 5B). However, the right fusiform cortex ROI did not show significantly higher reliability for matching conversations, t(26) = −0.28, p = .78. Therefore, the regions that were taken for- ward for further analysis were the PMC and the left and right LOC. Simulations of host and guest RSAs. We ran simulations to ensure that our analyses were not statistically biased to find stronger effects for the host identities because the hosts were repeated across three conversations. We simu- lated conversation patterns that had an equal contribution from both simulated host and simulated guest patterns. The first simulation added white noise to the simulated conversation patterns and ran the same analyses for the hosts and guests as the ones ran in the main analyses. The second simulation added noise that had the same cor- relational structure as the conversation patterns in the fMRI data (see Supplementary Figure 4 at https://osf.io/zpcv3/). Importantly, we did not see any differences in the simu- lated effects for host and guest analysis (simulation with correlated noise: host mean = 0.202, SD = 0.005; guest mean = 0.202, SD = 0.005), t(99999) = −0.58, p = .56. Hosts. We next examined which ROIs would show host- specific reinstatement. We compared the similarity between brain patterns from the Session 1 to Session 2 conversations. We took the patterns of identities in 526 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 34, Number 3 Session 1 that would be hosts in Session 2 and computed the similarity between host identities with the matching conversations. The left LOC, t(26) = 2.34, p = .02; PMC, t(26) = 3.27, p = .003; and right LOC, t(26) = 2.02, p = .05, showed host-specific reinstatement during the conversa- tions (see Figure 5B). Guests. We next examined whether these regions would also show guest-specific reinstatement. None of the ROIs showed a reliable guest-specific reinstatement: left LOC: t(26) = −1.56, p = .12; PMC: t(26) = −1.61, p = .12; right LOC: t(26) = −1.62, p = .11. Surprisingly, these nonsignif- icant effects are in the opposite direction to that which would show evidence for reinstatement (see Figure 5B). Hosts versus guests. We then performed a direct contrast between the host-specific reinstatement and guest-specific reinstatement effects. We observed higher host reinstate- ment in the left LOC, t(26) = 2.90, p = .007; PMC, t(26) = 3.41, p = .002; and right LOC, t(26) = 2.46, p = .02. In a post hoc analysis, we exam- Time course analysis. ined the time course of the host and guest effects reported above. Specifically, we ran three separate GLMs to esti- mate the first, second, and third part of the conversations separately. This allowed us to estimate patterns for the conversations for the beginning, middle, and end of the recall period. We ran the host and guest analyses as described above separately for each part of the recall period. We further ran the conversation-specific analysis in the three parts separately to ensure that we had enough power to distinguish between the conversations when modeling only a third of the duration (see Figure 6). Throughout the beginning, middle, and end of the events Figure 5. RSA comparisons within ROIs. (A) ROIs taken from Robin et al. (2018): These ROIs were identified to equally represent multiple features (location, people, and object) of events. The ROIs included PMC, LOC, MPFC, and parahippocampal cortex (PHC). (B) RSA results of ROIs showing reliable identity and conversation patterns, as well as reliable host reinstatement effects. The x-axis represents the different analyses (see Figure 3). The y-axis represents the correlation contrast between the diagonal and off-diagonal values. The asterisk indicates significance at p < .05. Each diamond represents the correlation contrast value for a given subject in one of the three ROIs. Figure 6. Reinstatement effects for host across time. Here, we show results for the conversation-specific host and guest analysis separately for the first, second, and third part of the recall duration. Bar plot represents means, and error bar shows bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. Asterisk indicates significance at p < .05. Bromis et al. 527 l D o w n o a d e d f r o m h t t p : / / d i r e c t . m i t . e d u / j / o c n a r t i c e - p d l f / / / / 3 4 3 5 1 7 1 9 8 4 8 5 8 / j o c n _ a _ 0 1 8 0 2 p d . f b y g u e s t t o n 0 8 S e p e m b e r 2 0 2 3 we observed conversation specific patterns. We observed host reinstatement in the middle and end of recall periods, but not during the beginning of events. In contrast, we did not observe guest reinstatement during any point of the recall period. We found that when we modeled only the beginning of the conversations, we could reli- ably detect conversation-specific patterns in all ROIs (left LOC: t(26) = 3.98, p < .001; PMC: t(26) = 3.14, p = .004; right LOC: t(26) = 2.25, p = .03), but could not observe any reinstatement of the hosts or guests in any of the three ROIs (all ps > .05). When modeling the middle part of the
recordar, we again found conversation-specific patterns in all
ROI: left LOC: t(26) = 5.47, pag < .001; PMC: t(26) = 6.06, p < .001; right ROC: t(26) = 3.86, p < .001. We also found host reinstatement (left LOC: t(26) = 2.71, p = .01; PMC: t(26) = 3.21, p = .003; right LOC: t(26) = 1.98, p = .058), but not guest reinstatement (all ps > .05). When modeling
only the last 30% of the recall, we again found conversation-
specific patterns (left LOC: t(26) = 4.28, pag < .001; PMC: t(26) = 4.04, p < .001; right LOC: t(26) = 4.06, p < .001), host reinstatement in PMC (left LOC: t(26) = 1.73, p = .09; PMC: t(26) = 2.23, p = .03; right LOC: t(26) = 1.87, p = .07), but no significant guest reinstatement (all ps > .05).

DISCUSIÓN

Rich and detailed episodic memories comprise many
individual elements: Some of these elements are common
to several different memories, whereas others are unique to
uno. Using a novel paradigm involving recall of com-
complejo, naturalistic events (custom written “chat show” con-
versations), we found that the neural representation of
the events was dominated by the repeated and more pre-
dictable elements (the “hosts”) rather than the more idio-
syncratic, yet event-defining elements (“the guests”). Estos
effects were found in PMC as well as lateral parietal/occipital
corteza. En tono rimbombante, analyses of behavioral data showed that
participants actually had better memory for dialogue spo-
ken by the guests compared with the hosts and also rated
the guests as being more important to the conversations.
This shows that the fMRI effects we report are not driven
by better memory for the hosts compared with the guests.
Many studies have shown that episodic memories tend to
be recalled in a holistic manner, with the retrieval of one ele-
ment being dependent on retrieval of others ( Joensen,
Gaskell, & Horner, 2020; Ngo, Horner, Newcombe, &
Olson, 2019; Horner & Burgess, 2014). Además,
retrieval of one element tends to result in reactivation of
the representation of other elements (Horner, Bisby, Arbusto,
lin, & Burgess, 2015), suggesting that no particular element
dominates in the overall representation of the event. Cómo-
alguna vez, in these studies, the events are all unique combina-
tions of elements. Our results suggest that when we recall
memories of particular events, it is the repeated and pre-
dictable elements that serve to frame or scaffold the rep-
resentation of the memory. It has already been shown that
the spatial context of an episode can dominate the

representation of episodic memories (Robin et al., 2018),
but in the current study, we specifically chose nonspatial ele-
mentos (well-known people) to be the repeated or novel ele-
mentos. We therefore speculate that when we recall detailed
episodic memories, those elements that are reliably present
serve as a form of “context” within which to retrieve the less
predictable details that are specific to a particular occasion.
Our study focused on regions of the brain’s PM network,
and we used ROIs that were identified in a previous study of
multielement event recall (Robin et al., 2018). The regions
where we found that hosts were represented more strongly
than guests—the PMC and lateral occipital/parietal
cortex—have been shown to support event-specific pat-
terns of activity in many previous studies (Masís-Obando,
Norman, & Baldassano, 2021; Raykov et al., 2021; Reagh &
Ranganath, 2021; Bird, 2020; Chen et al., 2017; Oedekoven
et al., 2017; Bird et al., 2015; St-Laurent et al., 2015; kühl &
Chun, 2014). This brain network has been argued to pre-
dominantly represent contextual information in a broad
sense—not only spatial contexts but also temporal and
social contexts (Ranganath & Ritchey, 2012). Similarmente, él
is associated with integrating multimodal information over
long timescales to build mental models of an overarching
situation ( Yeshurun, Nguyen, & Hasson, 2021; Hasson,
Chen, & Honey, 2015). Sin embargo, there is a large body
of evidence that these regions also support representa-
tions of known individuals and more basic semantic con-
cepts (Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013). Aquí, we show that
although these regions do support representations of
famous individuals, when two individuals occur together
in an event, it is the person who occurs most reliably
within that context who is represented most robustly.

The aim of this study was to contrast those elements that
are more generic, familiar, and reliably present with those
that are more idiosyncratic, unexpected, and event-
defining. Our behavioral results revealed that it was actually
the parts of the conversations spoken by the guests that
were more memorable. Además, a follow-up study found
that participants judged the guests to be more important to
the conversations compared with the hosts. Given this and
the research summarized in the Introduction, fue
unclear which elements would dominate the neural repre-
sentation of the conversations. Nosotros, por lo tanto, feel that the
finding that the host representations were consistently
stronger than the guest representations is an important
step to understanding how recalled episodic memories
are represented by the brain. Our design does not allow
us to further tease apart which factors contribute to the
representations of the hosts being dominant. Por ejemplo,
the number of repetitions of the hosts across the conversa-
tions is confounded with their predictability. Además, es
possible that chat show “hosts” are associated with a qual-
itatively different structural or social representation.
Future work should aim to deconfound the effects of
number of repetitions and predictability as well as verify
that our results generalize to elements of an episode that
do not have strong preexisting contextual associations.

528

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva

Volumen 34, Número 3

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

Episodic memory recall is thought to comprise an initial
period of memory search and construction followed by a
period of memory elaboration (Addis, Wong, & Schacter,
2007; Conway, Pleydell-Pearce, & Whitecross, 2001). Es
possible that, en nuestro estudio, the initial search period of epi-
sodic recall was dominated by the representations of the
hosts. This is because the hosts were reliably associated
with a particular day of the week across three separate con-
versations (p.ej., all Fridays), whereas the guests were only
associated with one unique combination of day and week
(p.ej., Monday Week 3). Sin embargo, we found no evidence
that the representation of the hosts was preferentially
active at the beginning of the recall period or that the
activation of the representation of the guest became
stronger as the recall phase progressed. Además, nosotros
observed conversation-specific patterns of activity in all
ROIs included in the main analyses. En efecto, nosotros también
observed conversation-specific patterns shared across
participants who learned the conversations with different
host and guests (see https://osf.io/zpcv3/). Tomados juntos,
these results and our behavioral memory findings suggests
that participants were unlikely to be remembering only the
hosts during the whole retrieval period.

The guests in the conversations were all well-known
celebrities and key elements in each of the events. Never-
theless, the representational similarity between the guest-
related patterns of activity in Session 1 and recall of the
conversations in Session 2 was not reliably above chance
in any of the ROIs. Además, an exploratory analysis
de 200 cortical regions and the hippocampus did not reveal
any locations where guest-related patterns of activity were
reliably activated when recalling the conversations (ver
https://osf.io/zpcv3/). By modeling our analyses, we verified
that they were not biased toward detecting reactivation
effects in the hosts rather than the guests. It remains a pos-
sibility that brain regions supporting representations of the
guests during retrieval did not correspond to shape of the
parcellated brain regions used in the exploratory analysis.
It has been argued that a function of episodic memory is to
enable the prediction of future events (Barron et al., 2020;
Lu, Hasson, & Norman, 2020; Schacter, Benoit, & Szpunar,
2017; see also Sun, Advani, Spruston, sajonia, & Fitzgerald,
2021). Predictive processing accounts of perception and
cognition typically emphasize the role of unexpected infor-
mation in driving attention and new learning (NVI &
Schoenbaum, 2008; Friston, 2005). Since memory offers
an opportunity for “off-line” learning (Hinton et al., 1995),
we might expect that the less typical aspects of an event
would be overrepresented when the event is recalled.
Sin embargo, we found the opposite effect—the repeated
and more predictable elements of the event—dominated
its neural representation.

En cambio, our results are compatible with the view that
information that has been encountered more frequently
is likely to be most useful to us in the future (anderson
& Milson, 1989). When recalling an event, activating robust
representations of the elements that are most reliably

present may help us to generalize our experiences to
new situations (Gershman, 2017). To reconcile our find-
ings with predictive coding accounts of cognition, it may
be that, during perception, prediction errors drive the
learning of any unexpected information, whereas memory
recall offers the opportunity to improve our internal
model of the world by selectively enhancing those ele-
ments that are likely to be encountered again and remov-
ing noisy or idiosyncratic components that are unlikely to
be repeated (Barron et al., 2020; see also Sun et al., 2021).
En resumen, our results show that the elements of an
event are not represented equally in memory. Those
elements that are most reliably present across similar
events—being both more frequently encountered and
more predictable—are the ones that are most robustly acti-
vated when recalling these events. The brain may use these
elements to provide context and scaffold the representa-
tion of a remembered event within the PM network. Este,
Sucesivamente, may facilitate the activation of the more idiosyn-
cratic elements that are unique to one specific occasion.

Expresiones de gratitud

We thank Ediz Sohoglu and Sam Berens for helpful discussions
about the study. We would like to thank Jessica Robin for pro-
viding us with the multifeature ROI and Charlotte Sutherland
for helping to score the behavioral data.

Reprint requests should be sent to Petar P. Raykov, School of
Psicología, University of Sussex, Falmer BN1 9QH, Reino Unido, or via
e-mail: P.Raykov@sussex.ac.uk.

Contribuciones de autor

Petar P. Raykov: Conceptualización; Análisis formal; Método-
ology; Administración de proyecto; Visualización; Writing—
Original draft; Writing—Review & edición. Konstantinos
Bromis: Conceptualización; Análisis formal; Methodol-
ogia; Administración de proyecto. Leah Wickens: Curación de datos;
Administración de proyecto. Warrick Roseboom: Conceptuali-
zación. Chris M. Bird: Conceptualización; Funding acquisi-
ción; Metodología; Administración de proyecto; Supervisión;
Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review & edición.

Información de financiación

This project has received funding from the European
Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s
Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme
(https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100010663), conceder número:
819526 to C. METRO. B. PAG. PAG. R. was additionally supported by
an Economic and Social Research Council studentship
(https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000269), grant num-
ber: ES/J500173/1 and fellowship: ES/ V012444/1.

Diversidad en las prácticas de citas

Análisis retrospectivo de las citas en cada artículo publicado.-
publicado en esta revista de 2010 a 2021 revela una persistente
patrón de desequilibrio de género: Although the proportions

Bromis et al.

529

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

of authorship teams (categorized by estimated gender
identification of first author/last author) publishing in
the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience ( JoCN ) durante
this period were M(un)/m = .407, W.(Omán)/m = .32,
M/M = .115, y P/P = .159, the comparable propor-
tions for the articles that these authorship teams cited
were M/M = .549, W/M = .257, M/M = .109, y P/P =
.085 (Postle and Fulvio, JoCN, 34:1, páginas. 1–3). Consecuencia-
frecuentemente, JoCN encourages all authors to consider gender
balance explicitly when selecting which articles to cite and
gives them the opportunity to report their article’s gender
citation balance.

REFERENCIAS

Addis, D. r., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. l. (2007). Recordando
the past and imagining the future: common and distinct
neural substrates during event construction and elaboration.
Neuropsicología, 45, 1363–1377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016, PubMed: 17126370
Aitchison, l., & Lengyel, METRO. (2017). With or without you:
Predictive coding and Bayesian inference in the brain.
Opinión actual en neurobiología, 46, 219–227. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.08.010, PubMed: 28942084
anderson, j. r., & Milson, R. (1989). Human memory: Un

adaptive perspective. Revisión psicológica, 96, 703–719.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.703

barrón, h. C., Auksztulewicz, r., & Friston, k. (2020). Prediction

and memory: A predictive coding account. Progress in
Neurobiología, 192, 101821. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.pneurobio.2020.101821, PubMed: 32446883

Bird, C. METRO. (2020). How do we remember events? Actual

Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 32, 120–125. https://doi.org
/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.01.020

Bird, C. METRO., Keidel, j. l., Ing, X. l. PAG., Horner, X. A. J., & Burgess,
norte. (2015). Consolidation of complex events via reinstatement
in posterior cingulate cortex. Revista de neurociencia, 35,
14426–14434. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1774-15
.2015, PubMed: 26511235

Cervecero, W.. F., & Treyens, j. C. (1981). Role of schemata in
memory for places. Psicología cognitiva, 13, 207–230.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(81)90008-6

Chen, J., Leong, Y. C., Honey, C. J., Yong, C. h., Norman, k. A., &
Hasson, Ud.. (2017). Shared memories reveal shared structure in
neural activity across individuals. Neurociencia de la naturaleza, 20,
115–125. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4450, PubMed: 27918531

clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated
agents, and the future of cognitive science. conductual
and Brain Sciences, 36, 181–204. https://doi.org/10.1017
/S0140525X12000477, PubMed: 23663408

Conway, METRO. A., Pleydell-Pearce, C. w., & Whitecross, S. mi.

(2001). The neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A
slow cortical potential study of autobiographical memory
retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 493–524.
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2781

Fairhall, S. l., & Caramazza, A. (2013). Brain regions that
represent amodal conceptual knowledge. Diario de
Neurociencia, 33, 10552–10558. https://doi.org/10.1523
/JNEUROSCI.0051-13.2013, PubMed: 23785167

Friston, k. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Serie B:
Ciencias Biologicas, 360, 815–836. https://doi.org/10.1098
/rstb.2005.1622, PubMed: 15937014

Gardumi, A., Ivanov, D., Hausfeld, l., Valente, GRAMO., Formisano, MI.,
& Uludağ, k. (2016). The effect of spatial resolution on
decoding accuracy in fMRI multivariate pattern analysis.
Neuroimagen, 132, 32–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2016.02.033, PubMed: 26899782

Gershman, S. j. (2017). Predicting the past, remembering

the future. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17,
7–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.05.025,
PubMed: 28920071

Glenberg, A., Herrero, S. METRO., & Verde, C. (1977). Type I rehersal:
Maintenance and more. Journal of Memory and Language,
16, 339–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80055-8
Hasson, Ud., Chen, J., & Honey, C. j. (2015). Hierarchical process
memory: Memory as an integral component of information
Procesando. Tendencias en Ciencias Cognitivas, 19, 304–313.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.006, PubMed: 25980649
Hendriks, METRO. h. A., Daniels, NORTE., Pegado, F., & Op de Beeck, h. PAG.
(2017). The effect of spatial smoothing on representational
similarity in a simple motor paradigm. Frontiers in Neurology,
8, 222. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2017.00222, PubMed:
28611726

Hinton, GRAMO. MI., Dayán, PAG., Frey, B. J., & Neal, R. METRO. (1995). El
“wake–sleep” algorithm for unsupervised neural networks.
Ciencia, 268, 1158–1161. https://doi.org/10.1126/science
.7761831, PubMed: 7761831

Horner, A. J., Bisby, j. A., Arbusto, D., lin, W.-J., & Burgess, norte. (2015).
Evidence for holistic episodic recollection via hippocampal
pattern completion. Comunicaciones de la naturaleza, 6, 1–11.
https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8462, PubMed: 26136141
Horner, A. J., & Burgess, norte. (2014). Pattern completion in

multielement event engrams. Biología actual, 24, 988–992.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.012, PubMed: 24746796

Hunt, R. R. (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von

Restorff really did. Boletín psiconómico & Revisar, 2, 105–112.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03214414, PubMed: 24203592

Hutton, C., Bork, A., Josephs, o., Deichmann, r., Ashburner, J.,
& Tornero, R. (2002). Image distortion correction in fMRI: A
quantitative evaluation. Neuroimagen, 16, 217–240. https://doi
.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.1054, PubMed: 11969330

Joensen, B. h., Gaskell, METRO. GRAMO., & Horner, A. j. (2020). United we

caer: All-or-none forgetting of complex episodic events.
Revista de Psicología Experimental: General, 149, 230.
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000648, PubMed: 31305093

Kishiyama, METRO. METRO., & Yonelinas, A. PAG. (2003). Novelty effects on
recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. Memoria
& Cognición, 31, 1045–1051. https://doi.org/10.3758
/BF03196125, PubMed: 14704019

kühl, B. A., & Chun, METRO. METRO. (2014). Successful remembering

elicits event-specific activity patterns in lateral parietal cortex.
Revista de neurociencia, 34, 8051–8060. https://doi.org/10
.1523/JNEUROSCI.4328-13.2014, PubMed: 24899726

Cooper, R. A., & Ritchey, METRO. (2020). Progression from feature-

Sotavento, h., & kühl, B. A. (2016). Reconstructing perceived and

specific brain activity to hippocampal binding during episodic
encoding. Revista de neurociencia, 40, 1701–1709. https://doi
.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1971-19.2019, PubMed: 31826947
di Oleggio Castello, METRO. v., Halchenko, Y. o., Guntupalli, j. S., Gors,
j. D., & Gobbini, METRO. I. (2017). The neural representation of
personally familiar and unfamiliar faces in the distributed
system for face perception. Informes Científicos, 7, 12237. https://
doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-12559-1, PubMed: 28947835

retrieved faces from activity patterns in lateral parietal cortex.
Revista de neurociencia, 36, 6069–6082. https://doi.org/10
.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-15.2016, PubMed: 27251627

Lu, P., Hasson, Ud., & Norman, k. A. (2020). Learning to use

episodic memory for event prediction. BioRxiv.

Masís-Obando, r., Norman, k. A., & Baldassano, C. (2021).

Schema representations in distinct brain networks support
narrative memory during encoding and retrieval. BioRxiv.

530

Revista de neurociencia cognitiva

Volumen 34, Número 3

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3

morris, C. D., Bransford, j. D., & Franks, j. j. (1977). Levels of
processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Diario de
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9

Mumford, j. A., Tornero, B. o., Ashby, F. GRAMO., & Poldrack, R. A.
(2012). Deconvolving BOLD activation in event-related
designs for multivoxel pattern classification analyses.
Neuroimagen, 59, 2636–2643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2011.08.076, PubMed: 21924359

Ngo, C. T., Horner, A. J., Newcombe, norte. S., & Olson, I. R.
(2019). Development of holistic episodic recollection.
ciencia psicológica, 30, 1696–1706. https://doi.org/10.1177
/0956797619879441, PubMed: 31672085

NVI, y., & Schoenbaum, GRAMO. (2008). Dialogues on prediction

errores. Tendencias en Ciencias Cognitivas, 12, 265–272. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.006, PubMed: 18567531

Nyberg, l., Habib, r., McIntosh, A. r., & Tulving, mi. (2000).
Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during
memory retrieval. Actas de la Academia Nacional de
Ciencias, EE.UU., 97, 11120–11124. https://doi.org/10.1073
/pnas.97.20.11120, PubMed: 11005878

Oedekoven, C. S. h., Keidel, j. l., Berens, S. C., & Bird, C. METRO. (2017).
Reinstatement of memory representations for lifelike events
over the course of a week. Informes Científicos, 7, 14305. https://
doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13938-4, PubMed: 29084981
Oosterhof, norte. NORTE., Connolly, A. C., & Haxby, j. V. (2016).

CoSMoMVPA: Multi-modal multivariate pattern analysis of
neuroimaging data in Matlab/GNU octave. Fronteras en
Neuroinformatics, 10, 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2016
.00027, PubMed: 27499741

Popov, v., & Reder, l. METRO. (2020). Frequency effects on memory:
A resource-limited theory. Revisión psicológica, 127, 1–46.
https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000161, PubMed: 31524424

Poppenk, J., Köhler, S., & Moscovitch, METRO. (2010). Revisiting the

novelty effect: When familiarity, not novelty, enhances
memory. Revista de Psicología Experimental: Aprendiendo,
Memoria, and Cognition, 36, 1321–1330. https://doi.org/10
.1037/a0019900, PubMed: 20804299

Quent, j. A., Henson, R. NORTE., & Greve, A. (2021). A predictive
account of how novelty influences declarative memory.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 179, 107382. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107382, PubMed: 33476747
Ranganath, C., & Rainer, GRAMO. (2003). Neural mechanisms for

detecting and remembering novel events. Reseñas de naturaleza
Neurociencia, 4, 193–202. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1052,
PubMed: 12612632

Ranganath, C., & Ritchey, METRO. (2012). Two cortical systems for

memory-guided behaviour. Naturaleza Reseñas Neurociencia, 13,
713–726. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3338, PubMed: 22992647
Raykov, PAG. PAG., Keidel, j. l., Oakhill, J., & Bird, C. METRO. (2020). El
brain regions supporting schema-related processing of people’s
identidades. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 37, 8–24. https://doi.org
/10.1080/02643294.2019.1685958, PubMed: 31710265

Raykov, PAG. PAG., Keidel, j. l., Oakhill, J., & Bird, C. METRO. (2021).

Activation of person knowledge in medial prefrontal cortex
during the encoding of new lifelike events. Corteza cerebral.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab027, PubMed: 33866362
Reagh, z. METRO., & Ranganath, C. (2021). A cortico-hippocampal

scaffold for representing and recalling lifelike events. BioRxiv.
Ritchey, METRO., Montchal, METRO. MI., Yonelinas, A. PAG., & Ranganath, C.
(2015). Delay-dependent contributions of medial temporal
lobe regions to episodic memory retrieval. eVida, 4, e05025.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05025, PubMed: 25584461
Robin, j. (2018). Spatial scaffold effects in event memory and

imagination. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Ciencia cognitiva,
9, e1462. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1462, PubMed: 29485243

Robin, J., Buchsbaum, B. r., & Moscovitch, METRO. (2018). El
primacy of spatial context in the neural representation of

events. Revista de neurociencia, 38, 2755–2765. https://doi
.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1638-17.2018, PubMed: 29440386
Schacter, D. l., Benoit, R. GRAMO., & Szpunar, k. k. (2017). Episodic
future thinking: Mechanisms and functions. Current Opinion
in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.cobeha.2017.06.002, PubMed: 29130061

Schaefer, A., kong, r., gordon, mi. METRO., Laumann, t. o., Zuo,
X. NORTE., holmes, A. J., et al. (2018). Local-global parcellation of
the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional
connectivity MRI. Corteza cerebral, 28, 3095–3114. https://
doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179

sherman, B. MI., & Turk-Browne, norte. B. (2020). Statistical

prediction of the future impairs episodic encoding of the
present. procedimientos de la Academia Nacional de Ciencias,
EE.UU., 117, 22760–22770. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas
.2013291117, PubMed: 32859755

Sievers, C., Bird, C. METRO., & Renoult, l. (2019). Predicting memory
formation over multiple study episodes. Aprendiendo & Memoria,
26, 465–472. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.049791.119, PubMed:
31732707

Herrero, t. A., Hasinski, A. MI., & Montaña de cedro, PAG. B. (2013). El

context repetition effect: Predicted events are remembered
mejor, even when they don’t happen. Diario de
Psicología experimental: General, 142, 1298. https://doi.org
/10.1037/a0034067, PubMed: 23957285

Herrero, S. METRO., Jenkinson, METRO., lana rica, METRO. w., beckman, C. F.,

Behrens, t. mi. J., Johansen-Berg, h., et al. (2004). Avances en
functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation
as FSL. Neuroimagen, 23, S208–S219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2004.07.051, PubMed: 15501092

Sohoglu, MI., & davis, METRO. h. (2020). Rapid computations of
spectrotemporal prediction error support perception of
degraded speech. eVida, 9, e58077. https://doi.org/10.7554
/eLife.58077, PubMed: 33147138

St-Laurent, METRO., Abdi, h., & Buchsbaum, B. R. (2015). Distributed
patterns of reactivation predict vividness of recollection.
Revista de neurociencia cognitiva, 27, 2000–2018. https://
doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00839, PubMed: 26102224

Sol, w., Advani, METRO., Spruston, NORTE., sajonia, A., & Fitzgerald, j. mi. (2021).
Organizing memories for generalization in complementary
learning systems. BioRxiv.

Tulving, mi. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford:

prensa de la Universidad de Oxford.

Van Strien, j. w., Hagenbeek, R. MI., estampar, C. J., Rombouts,

S. A. R. B., & Barkhof, F. (2005). Changes in brain electrical
activity during extended continuous word recognition.
Neuroimagen, 26, 952–959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.neuroimage.2005.03.003, PubMed: 15955505

Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for clinical
usar. Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1080
/00223980.1945.9917223

Wittkuhn, l., Chien, S., Hall-McMaster, S., & Schuck, norte. W..
(2021). Replay in minds and machines. Neurociencia &
Revisiones de biocomportamiento, 129, 367–388. https://doi.org/10.1016
/j.neubiorev.2021.08.002

Yassa, METRO. A., & Reagh, z. METRO. (2013). Competitive trace theory: A
role for the hippocampus in contextual interference during
retrieval. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 107.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00107, PubMed: 23964216
Yeshurun, y., Nguyen, METRO., & Hasson, Ud.. (2021). The default mode
network: Where the idiosyncratic self meets the shared social
world. Naturaleza Reseñas Neurociencia, 22, 181–192. https://doi
.org/10.1038/s41583-020-00420-w, PubMed: 33483717

Zadbood, A., Chen, J., Leong, Y. C., Norman, k. A., & Hasson, Ud..

(2017). How we transmit memories to other brains:
Constructing shared neural representations via
comunicación. Corteza cerebral, 27, 4988–5000. https://doi
.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx202, PubMed: 28922834

Bromis et al.

531

yo

D
oh
w
norte
oh
a
d
mi
d

F
r
oh
metro
h

t
t

pag

:
/
/

d
i
r
mi
C
t
.

metro

i
t
.

mi
d
tu

/
j

/

oh
C
norte
a
r
t
i
C
mi

pag
d

yo

F
/

/

/

/

3
4
3
5
1
7
1
9
8
4
8
5
8

/
j

oh
C
norte
_
a
_
0
1
8
0
2
pag
d

.

F

b
y
gramo
tu
mi
s
t

t

oh
norte
0
8
S
mi
pag
mi
metro
b
mi
r
2
0
2
3The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image
The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image
The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image
The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image
The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image
The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image
The Neural Representation of Events Is Dominated by image

Descargar PDF