Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., &

Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., &
Konrad, k. (2023). Successful Modulation of Temporoparietal Junction Activity and Stimulus-Driven
Attention by fNIRS-based Neurofeedback – a Randomized Controlled Proof-of-Concept Study. Imaging
Neurociencia, Publicación anticipada. https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00014

Successful Modulation of Temporoparietal Junction Activity and
Stimulus-Driven Attention by fNIRS-based Neurofeedback – a
Randomized Controlled Proof-of-Concept Study

Simon H. Kohl1,2*, Pia Melies2, Johannes Uttecht2, Michael Lührs3,4, Laura Bell2,5, David M.. A.
Mehler6,7, Surjo R. Soekadar8, Shivakumar Viswanathan9, Kerstin Konrad1,2

1JARA-Institute Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging (INM-11), Forschungszentrum Jülich, 52425 Jülich,

Alemania

2Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy,

Faculty of Medicine, RWTH Aachen University, 52074 Aachen, Alemania

3Brain Innovation B.V., Research Department, 6229 EV Maastricht, Los países bajos
4Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 EV

Maastricht, Los países bajos

5Audiovisual Media Center, Medical Faculty, RWTH Aachen University, 52074 Aachen, Alemania
6Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Faculty of Medicine, RWTH Aachen University, 52074

Aachen, Alemania

7Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Alemania
8Clinical Neurotechnology Laboratory, departamento. of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, Charité Campus Mitte (CCM), Charité –

Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Charitéplatz 1, 10117 Berlina, Alemania

9Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine – Cognitive Neuroscience (INM-3), Forschungszentrum Jülich, 52425 Jülich,

Alemania

*Autor correspondiente: Simon H. Kohl, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Wilhelm-Johnen-Strasse, 52425 Jülich, Alemania;
si.kohl@fz-juelich.de, simon.h.kohl@gmail.com ; ORCID: 0000-0003-0949-6754

© 2023 Instituto de Tecnología de Massachusetts. Publicado bajo una atribución Creative Commons 4.0 Internacional

(CC POR 4.0) licencia. 1

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

Graphical abstract

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

2

Abstracto

The right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) is a core hub in neural networks associated with reorienting

of attention and social cognition. Sin embargo, it remains unknown whether participants can learn to

actively modulate their rTPJ activity via neurofeedback. Aquí, we explored the feasibility of

functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS)-based neurofeedback in modulating rTPJ activity and

its effect on rTPJ functions such as reorienting of attention and visual perspective taking. en un

bidirectional regulation control group design, 50 healthy participants were either reinforced to up- o

downregulate rTPJ activation over four days of training.

Both groups showed an increase in rTPJ activity right from the beginning of the trainingbut only the

upregulation group maintained this effect, while the downregulation group showed a decline from the

initial rTPJ activation. This suggests a learning effect in the downregulation exclusively, making it

challenging to draw definitive conclusions about the effectiveness of rTPJ upregulation training.

Sin embargo, we observed group-specific effects on the behavioral level. We found a significant group x

time interaction effect in the performance of the reorienting of attention task and group-specific

cambios, with decreased reaction times (RTs) in the upregulation group and increased RTs in the

downregulation group across all conditions after the neurofeedback training. Those with low baseline

performance showed greater improvements. In the perspective-taking task, sin embargo, only time

effects were observed that were non-group-specific.These findings demonstrate that fNIRS-based

neurofeedback is a feasible method to modulate rTPJ functions with preliminary evidence of

neurophysiologically specific effects, thus paving the way for future applications of non-invasive

rTPJ modulation in neuropsychiatric disorders.

Palabras clave: Neuromodulation, Neurofeedback, Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS),
Temporoparietal Junction, Atención, Social Cognition

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

3

Highlights

the right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) as a core hub for attentive and socio-cognitive

functions is a promising target for neuromodulatory interventions

first single-blinded, randomized controlled study demonstrates feasibility and

effectiveness of the fNIRS-based neurofeedback training of the rTPJ in healthy adults

 subjects are able to regulate the rTPJ with different learning characteristics

first evidence of a neurophysiologically specific effect on stimulus-driven attention

findings have important implications for clinical translation of neurofeedback

interventions targeting the rTPJ

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

4

1 Introducción

The right temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is considered a central hub of the human brain being

involved in diverse mental functions. Theoretical models stress its involvement in stimulus-driven

attention and social cognition and discuss its essential role in detecting violations of expectations,

contextual updating, mental state shifting, and sense of agency (Corbetta et al., 2008; Decety &

Lamm, 2007; Geng & Vossel, 2013; Krall et al., 2015; van Overwalle, 2009). Due to its diverse

anatomical and functional connections, the TPJ is also considered an important brain region for

communication with neighboring, partially overlapping networks, forming a potential hub where

multiple networks converge and interact (Carretero & Huettel, 2013; Mars et al., 2012).

Neuromodulation of such high degree network hubs or control points may result in greater changes in

neural networks and associated behaviors and cognitive functions than neuromodulation of low

degree nodes. Por lo tanto, they are considered hot spots for targeted brain-based interventions

(Murphy & bassett, 2017).

Además, targeting such high degree hubs using non-invasive neuromodulation, como

neurofeedback, is interesting from a translational perspective. Testing the causal role of the hub in the

network by neuromodulation followed by observation of its behavioral/functional consequences may

inform therapeutic interventions for brain disorders associated with this hub, p.ej., autism spectrum

disorder (ASD), depression and schizophrenia (Kana et al., 2015; Penner et al., 2018). Sucesivamente, pruebas

these potential interventions will increase our understanding of this neural network hub and its role in

the respective disorder.

Previous neuromodulation studies mostly relied on neurostimulation techniques such as

transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) or transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to disrupt

or enhance TPJ functions while neurofeedback was utilized to a lesser extent.

TMS studies have demonstrated a decrease in spatial attention performance when disrupting

activation in the right TPJ (rTPJ; Krall et al., 2016; Mengotti et al., 2022). En cambio, a study

conducted by Roy et al. (2015) used tDCS to enhance activation in the right posterior parietal cortex,

which includes parts of the rTPJ, resulting in improved attention re-orienting following stimulation.

Regarding socio-cognitive abilities such as visual perspective taking (vPT) and imitation control, el

evidence for potential enhancement through tDCS is promising but mixed (Santiesteban et al., 2012,

2015; Nobusako et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2020).Sin embargo, tDCS studies have reported no significant

enhancing effects on other complex socio-cognitive abilities, including theory of mind (ToM;

5

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

Santiesteban et al., 2015), empathy, emotion recognition, and joint attention (Pereira et al., 2021). En

hecho, inhibitory tDCS for ToM and empathy (Mai et al., 2016), as well as inhibitory TMS for ToM

(Krall et al., 2016), have shown disruptive effects.

Juntos, these studies provide first evidence that neuromodulation of the rTPJ can be used to

improve reorienting of attention and certain facets of socio-cognitive abilities, such as vPT.

Por lo tanto, the rTPJ may also be a promising target for neurofeedback interventions, offering

potentially new treatment options for neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by deficient TPJ

functions such as ASD (Esse Wilson et al., 2018; Salehinejad et al., 2021)

Neurofeedback based on functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is similar to

neurostimulation a causative neuromodulation technique for modulating activation of circumscribed

neocortical brain regions, although likely with less specific and more global effects on brain

networks than neurostimulation. By providing real-time feedback of hemodynamic correlates of

neural activity (p.ej., changes in oxyhemoglobin), participants can learn to regulate the brain activity

of specific target regions. En particular, fNIRS-based neurofeedback offers several advantages when

it comes to clinical translation. It is an easy-to-use, non-invasive, and endogenous form of

neuromodulation, which allows long-term learning through the reinforcement of neural activity and

cognitive strategies with therapeutic potential. Además, it is safe and well tolerated, and is therefore

associated with fewer ethical concerns than other neuromodulation techniques (Kohl et al., 2020;

Soekadar et al., 2021). Across different studies, preliminary but compelling evidence suggests that

the activation of a neural network, including the TPJ, can be successfully modulated by

neurofeedback based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (resonancia magnética funcional; Harmelech et al., 2015;

Emmert et al., 2016; Direito et al., 2019, 2021; Pamplona et al., 2020). Sin embargo, behavioral effects

and specificity of findings are less clear, and no study has yet targeted rTPJ activity using fNIRS-

based neurofeedback.

En el estudio actual, we aimed to fill this gap and investigated the feasibility and effectiveness

of fNIRS-based neurofeedback training employing social/monetary reward (Mathiak et al., 2015) a

control rTPJ activity in healthy participants. We conducted a randomized, controlled proof-of-

concept study employing a bidirectional-regulation control group design, which allows for the

detection of neurophysiologically specific effects (Sorger et al., 2019). More specifically, we aimed

to explore three aspects: (1) Can participants learn to increase/decrease the activity of the rTPJ using

fNIRS-based neurofeedback and how is their learning behavior over the course of the training

characterized? (2) Are there any specific behavioral effects in stimulus-driven attention and vPT

6

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

following the neurofeedback training? (3) What are potential predictors of behavioral improvements?

We hypothesized that healthy adult participants could gain control over activation of the rTPJ with

fNIRS-based neurofeedback and that successful upregulation would be accompanied by improved

performance in a reorienting of attention task and a vPT task. A diferencia de, we assumed that

downregulation would be associated with either decreased performance or no change in performance.

Based on previous findings on specific traits associated with rTPJ function, p.ej., empathy and autistic

traits (Donaldson et al., 2018; Kana et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2020), we tested predictors of behavioral

change on an exploratory level. Due to the scarcity of neurofeedback studies targeting rTPJ

activación, we identified a rather broad set of potential predictors without stating directed hypothesis

for each of them (see methods section).

2 Métodos

2.1 Participantes

Fifty right-handed healthy participants (edad 18-30 años) were recruited via flyer and social media

announcements. Participants were screened during a telephone interview prior to participating in the

study and were excluded if they had a history of psychiatric or neurological diseases, drug or alcohol

abuso, or if they were undergoing current psychopharmacological or psychotherapeutic treatments.

Participants were informed about the study procedure and signed an informed consent document. En

the end of the study, they received a financial compensation of at least 60€ for attending all sessions,

along with an additional monetary reward depending on the success of the neurofeedback training.

The study protocol was approved by the local ethics committee (I 148/18) and conducted in

accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013), with the exception

that it was not pre-registered on a publicly accessible database.

The participants were randomly allocated to the study groups, which were balanced out in terms of

gender and the order of task assessments. Twenty subjects were allocated to the downregulation

group and ten more (30 Participantes) to the upregulation group in order to provide higher statistical

power for later subgroup analyses in this group.

After the first eleven participants, we noticed an error in the online preprocessing script

(motion correction algorithm), which led to small deviations of the feedback displayed during the

neurofeedback training. We simulated the feedback signal of these participants using the corrected

script and calculated the accordance with the original feedback signal. Five participants (3 en el

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

7

upregulation group, 2 in the downregulation group) showed an accordance below 90% and were

therefore excluded from further analysis.

No a priori power analysis was conducted. Sin embargo, according to a sensitivity analysis, a

mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) incluido 45 participants was sufficiently powered (80%) a

detect a group x time interaction effect of at least f = 0.43 (assuming no violation of sphericity and a

correlation among repeated measure of 0.8) o 0.77 for an independent t-test.

2.2 Study design

We applied a single-blinded, randomized controlled between-subject design. Los participantes fueron

blinded to group assignments, but experimenters were not. We followed the recently published best

practices for fNIRS publications (Yücel et al., 2021) and the consensus on the reporting and

experimental design of clinical and cognitive-behavioral neurofeedback studies (CRED-nf checklist

(Ros et al., 2020;see Supplementary Material 2)).

Cifra 1. Study design

Procedimiento

All participants took part in four appointments, including a pre- and post-assessment session with one

additional short neurofeedback training session (día 1 and day 4) as well as two longer

neurofeedback training sessions (día 2 and day 3; ver figura 1). The four appointments were

scheduled within one week (m = 6.71 ± 2.23 días) and the pre- and post-assessment sessions at the

same time of day. The procedure on each day was as follows:

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

8

Day 1: To evaluate self-efficacy as a potential mechanism of neurofeedback effects and address

group differences, participants completed the German version of the general self-efficacy scale

(Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995). Además, a questionnaire was administered to assess the

Participantes’ expectations and motivation towards the neurofeedback training, offering further

controls for non-specific psychological mechanisms. After a short practice session, the pre-

assessment of the (1) reorienting of attention task and (2) perspective-taking task took place. El

order of these two tasks was counterbalanced across participants within both groups. Participantes

subsequently received specific instructions about the neurofeedback training and underwent two runs

of neurofeedback training.

Days 2 y 3: On days 2 y 3, participants underwent neurofeedback sessions with four runs each.

To assess pre-post changes in mood states and resting state brain activity, we assessed the German

short version of the Profile of Mood States (McNair et al., 1981) and recorded a 10 min resting-state

fNIRS measurement (to be reported elsewhere) before the training started on day 2 and after the

training was completed on day 3. Between days 2 y 3, participants completed standardized

questionnaires to account for variations in socio-cognitive traits among groups and predict

neurofeedback effects. These traits, including autistic traits, empathy, cognitive styles, así como

ADHD symptoms have the potential to impact rTPJ functioning (Barman et al., 2015; Donaldson et

Alabama., 2018; Kana et al., 2014). All traits were assessed dimensionally using the German version of the

Social Responsiveness Scale (Bölte, 2012), the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale v1.1 (Kessler et al.,

2005), the German version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI;davis, 1983), the autism-

spectrum quotient (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), the systemizing quotient (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003),

and the empathy quotient (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004).

Day 4: Participants underwent a short neurofeedback training session of two runs, followed by the

post-assessment of the reorienting of attention and perspective-taking task. At the end of the session,

participants filled in the general self-efficacy scale again as well as a debriefing questionnaire to

further assess feasibility and unspecific mechanisms. This questionnaire included items assessing

participants’ evaluation of the neurofeedback training, for example “I believe the training helped to

improve my attention”, “I enjoyed the training”, “The experimenter was trustworthy”, etc..

Además, they were asked to guess the group condition they had been randomly assigned to.

The fNIRS system was set up at the beginning of each day, except for day 1, which began

with comprehensive instructions and practice runs. For all tasks, stimuli were presented on a 24-inch

LCD screen (1920 X 1080 píxeles) using the Psychtoolbox on Matlab 2017a (The Mathworks Inc,

9

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

Natick, MAMÁ) and being run on a Windows PC. Participants viewed the screen at a distance of

approximately 50cm. Responses were acquired using a standard keyboard.

Neurofeedback training

Participants were blinded to their group assignment and were told that, depending on their group

assignments, the goal of the training was to increase or decrease activation of a specific brain region.

Irrespective of group assignment, participants in both groups received the same instructions.

All participants received standardized information and instructions about the neurofeedback

training (see Supplementary Material 1) based on Greer et al. (2014). They were instructed not to use

any respiratory or motor strategies but to remain still, breathe regularly, and only rely on mental

strategies to regulate their brain activity. The training took place on all four days and comprised 12

runs in total: two runs on days 1 y 4 (~12 minute/day), and four runs on days 2 y 3 (~25

minutes/day). Each neurofeedback run consisted of six blocks. Each block started with a 25s/30s no-

regulation condition followed by a 30s regulation condition, and the block ended with a 2s reward

presentación (ver figura 1). We varied the durations of the no-regulation condition to avoid

synchronization with physiological confounds, such as breathing patterns and Mayer waves, durante

the task and to increase design efficiency (Kinoshita et al., 2016; Yücel et al., 2021). On each block,

the face of a human avatar was continuously displayed on the screen. We used DAZ Studio 4.9 (DAZ

Productions, Cª, EE.UU) to create a modified version of the stimuli validated by Hartz et al. (2021).

Eleven pictures of the avatar with different levels of smiling were created for the visualization of the

feedback signal.

During the no-regulation condition, participants were instructed to passively look at the

avatar, which maintained a neutral facial expression. During the regulation condition, real-time

feedback of rTPJ activity was presented visually on a screen using a smiling avatar (social reward).

Participants were instructed to regulate and make the avatar smile, which was modulated in real time

by their rTPJ activation.

To foster motivation, participants received a monetary reward for successful regulation. En

each regulation trial, the participant received 0.01€ per second exceeding a certain individual

límite (see real-time fNIRS data processing (online analysis)). Whenever participants exceeded

this reward threshold, a green frame appeared around the feedback display, indicating that their

regulation was earning an incentive. The total amount earned on each trial was presented on the

screen at the end of the trial. This threshold was adapted according to individual regulation

actuación (ver 0. for a detailed description).

10

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

In neurofeedback training, providing explicit mental strategies is not necessary but initially

seems to facilitate learning (Scharnowski et al., 2015). Por lo tanto, we provided some example

strategies that could be helpful to regulate rTPJ activity (p.ej., strategies related to ToM, empathy,

thinking, imagination of positive events, counting, etc.; see Supplementary Material 1). Sin embargo,

participants were encouraged to find their own individual successful strategy by trial and error. Después

each neurofeedback run, we asked participants to verbally report which strategies they used and how

successful they rated this strategy (Likert scale ranging from 1 a 5). After each session, nosotros también

assessed participants’ motivation to continue participating in the training and their beliefs about

being able to control their brain activity.

Reorienting of attention task

Reorienting of attention is defined as the capacity to alter the focus of attention to unexpected,

external stimuli while expecting another task/situation. We assessed the reorienting of attention using

a modified version of the Posner paradigm (Krall et al., 2016; posner, 1980; Vossel et al., 2009). En

this task (ver figura 2), a central diamond (fixation point) was displayed between two horizontally

arranged boxes. For each trial, a central cue was presented for 200ms indicating whether a target

would appear on the right or the left side of the screen (brightening of the diamond to the right or left,

respectivamente). After a variable cue-target interval of 400ms or 700ms, the target (white diamond)

appeared for 100ms with a certain probability at the cued (valid cueing) or at the non-cued location

(invalid cueing) and the participant had to indicate on which side it appeared by pressing a button

using his/her right hand. The target-cue stimulus onset asynchrony was either 1000ms or 1300ms. Todo

stimuli were presented on a black background. Since fNIRS was assessed during the task, the trials

were presented in a blocked design. The task consisted of a total of twelve blocks, with six invalid

blocks and six valid blocks. Each invalid block comprised twelve valid and eight invalid trials and

each valid block comprised 20 valid trials only. Por eso, the overall distribution of valid trials (192 de

240) and invalid trials (48 de 240) era 80% vs. 20%. The blocks were presented in a randomized

order to mitigate anticipatory effects. Participants were told that the cue was not always informative,

but they were not informed about the different blocks beforehand. The task blocks with a 40s

duration were separated by 20s or 25s rest periods in which the same visual stimuli, but no cues or

objetivos, were presented.

Visual perspective-taking (vPT) tarea

vPT refers to the ability to infer spatial relationships between objects from different viewing angles.

We assessed vPT with the widely used Director paradigm according to Dumontheil et al. (2010) y

11

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

Symeonidou et al. (2016), as this task has been successfully used to assess the effects of tDCS

stimulation of the TPJ (Santiesteban et al., 2012, 2015). en esta tarea, participants saw a visual scene

con un 4 X 4 set of shelves containing eight different objects (ver figura 2) and were instructed to take

the perspective of a “director” standing behind the shelves and giving them auditory instructions to

move certain objects on the shelves by clicking a mouse on the respective target object. En tono rimbombante,

some of the objects were occluded from the view of the director, which participants had to take into

account in order to respond correctly in the perspective taking (PT) condición. This can be seen in

Cifra 2 where the “director” refers to the football instead of the large basketball (distractor), cual

is occluded from his view. In the control condition (non-perspective-taking (NPT) condición), el

distractor is replaced by an irrelevant object. For a more detailed description of this task, ver

Dumontheil et al. (2010) and Symeonidou et al. (2016). Each block consisted of four trials. The PT

and NPT blocks were presented in a pseudo-randomized order in such a way that no more than two

blocks of the same condition were presented consecutively. The task blocks (24s) were separated by

a rest period with a duration of 20s or 25s. RTs were recorded from the onset of the auditory

instruction to the participant’s mouse click response.

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

12

Cifra 2. Illustration of experimental tasks used for pre-post measurements. The reorienting of

attention task (izquierda; adopted from Krall et al., 2016) and the visual perspective-taking task (bien;

adopted from Symeonidou et al., 2016) were used to measure the effects of the neurofeedback

training. [Note that in the perspective-taking task (bien), the speech bubbles are only shown for

illustration. Auditory instructions were provided to the participants by the director (see text).]

2.3 fNIRS acquisition

We used the ETG-4000 continuous wave system (Hitachi Medical Corporation, Tokio, Japón) a

measure changes in oxy-(HbO) and deoxyhemoglobin (HbR) concentrations at a rate of 10Hz with

two wavelengths (695nm and 830nm). Two 3 × 5 probe sets (2 × 22 measurement channels) eran

placed bitemporally on the participant’s head to cover temporal and frontal brain regions and were

attached using electroencephalography (EEG) caps (Easycap GmbH, Herrsching, Alemania). El

interoptode distance was 3cm. The probe sets were placed on the participants’ heads in such a way

that the second most posterior optode of the lowest row was placed over T3/T4 of the EEG 10-20

13

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

sistema (Jaspe, 1958) and the most anterior optode of the lowest row was placed approximately over

the eyebrow (ver figura 3). If necessary, hair was moved away from optode holders in order to

increase the quality of the signal. Además, we instructed participants each day to stay relaxed,

breathe regularly, and keep the movement of their heads to a minimum.

To select the best channel for the feedback processing that covers the rTPJ, prior to the

estudio actual, we conducted digitizer measurements using a Patriot 3D Digitizer (Polhemus,

Colchester, Vermont) in a separate sample of five pilot participants wearing an fNIRS optode

arrangement available from a previous study. In all five subjects, the same channel corresponded to

anterior parts of the rTPJ (ver figura 3). To confirm the anatomical specificity of this channel, nosotros

also conducted digitizer measurements in all participants of the current study after each experimental

session. Anatomical locations of the optodes in relation to standard head landmarks (nasion, inion,

Cz, and preauricular points) were assessed. Cortical sensitivities of all channels were estimated

through Monte Carlo photon migration simulations (1,000,000 photons) using AtlasViewer

implemented in Homer v2.8 (Aasted et al., 2015; Huppert et al., 2009). Montreal Neurological

Instituto (MNI) coordinates for each subject and session were extracted and averaged for each

partícipe. In total, 5% of data (es decir., 10 del 200 samples obtained from 50 participants and 4

sessions) were excluded from this analysis due to errors during the digitizer measurements, cual

resulted in implausible estimations of MNI coordinates. The average MNI coordinate of the feedback

channel (x = 56 ± 6.4, y = -49 ± 4.6, z = 18 ± 6.9) corresponded to anterior parts of the rTPJ,

previously reported in a meta-analysis for reorienting of attention and theory of mind contrasts (Krall

et al., 2015).

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

14

Cifra 3. fNIRS optode arrangement and sensitivity profile for the feedback channel. El

feedback channel corresponds to anterior parts of the rTPJ (MNI: x = 56 ± 6.4, y = -49 ± 4.6, z = 18

± 6.9).

2.4 Real-time fNIRS data processing (online analysis)

Participants received feedback in real time about the instantaneous HbO activity at one single

channel placed over the rTPJ (see section on fNIRS acquisition). The procedure to convert the HbO

activity into feedback (updated every 100ms) involved several steps as described below (ver figura

4).

Cifra 4. Real-time fNIRS data processing. Correlation-based signal improvement (CBSI)

algoritmo (Cui et al., 2010).

The raw signal was first preprocessed by the ETG-4000 using a high-pass filter of 0.01Hz and

a low-pass filter of 1Hz and a moving average of 5s. This preprocessed HbO signal was then sent in

real time to an external computer where it was further processed using a customized Matlab script.

15

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

Motion artifacts in the signal were then removed using the correlation-based signal

mejora (CBSI) algoritmo (Cui et al., 2010). This algorithm calculates the corrected signal as a

linear combination of HbO and HbR scaled by their standard deviations, based on the assumption

that HbO and HbR are highly negatively correlated. Además, the algorithm assumes that the

signal has been offset-corrected to have zero mean.

Using the CBSI algorithm, the corrected HbO signal at time t (denoted as Xcorr (t)) era

obtained using the following expression:

where X(t) and Y(t) are the measured values of the HbO and HbR values, respectivamente, at time t (después

offset correction), and α is the ratio of the noise amplitude in the HbO and HbR signals. To estimate

the noise amplitude ratio α and perform the offset correction, we used the HbO and HbR signals from

the last 30s of the no-regulation period (es decir. from the period [–30s, 0s] relative to the start of the

regulation period at 0s) como sigue:

This preprocessed and motion-corrected signal Xcorr (t) was then normalized relative to the

HbO signal from the last five seconds of the no-regulation period (base) using the following

formula:

The feedback signal was further smoothed using linear interpolation over 1s. The final step

was to convert the feedback value into visual feedback. This was implemented by mapping the

feedback value onto a scale that ranged from 0 a 10, based on the following expression:

To receive positive feedback on downregulation, this value was multiplied by –1 for the

downregulation group. In this expression, the maximum feedback value (nivel 10) was defined as a

porcentaje (k) of a threshold value (t) that was determined for each participant based on their rTPJ

activation during the reorienting of attention and perspective-taking tasks at pre-assessment. Similar

to the calculation of the feedback signal during the neurofeedback task, the rTPJ activation during

valid/invalid and perspective-taking/non-perspective-taking blocks was estimated and averaged over

16

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

bloques. The mean between the contrasts of invalid vs valid (reorienting of attention task) y

perspective taking vs non-perspective taking (vPT task) was calculated using the following formula:

If this value was negative, only the positive contrast was used. If both contrasts were

negative, the initial threshold was set to the default value T = 1. To scale the feedback signal, nosotros

defined level 10 of the feedback value as k = 0.25 (es decir., 25% of the threshold value T). The feedback

value at each time was used to update the visual display in two ways. Each level of this scale from 0

a 10 was associated with eleven images of the avatar smiling to different extents with 10 being the

largest smile. Por lo tanto, the feedback value was used to update the image of the avatar displayed on

the screen. Además, whenever the participant exceeded a feedback level of 5, a green frame

appeared around the feedback display indicating a monetary reward (1 cent/sec).

To maintain the difficulty of the task across runs, the value of k was incremented by 0.25 para

the next run if the participant exceeded level 5 para 75% of the time on each run.

2.5 Data processing and analysis

Statistical methods and software

The additional fNIRS offline analyses were carried out using Homer v2.8 (Huppert et al.,

2009)(Huppert et al., 2009) and in-house Matlab scripts (Matlab 2018b; The Mathworks Inc, Natick,

MAMÁ). Statistical analyses were performed using R (R Core Team, 2021). To assess the effects of the

neurofeedback training, we calculated linear mixed models using the R packages lme4 (Bates et al.,

2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017). The models were fitted using REML. En el caso de

non-normal residuals, robust nonparametric analysis of longitudinal data in factorial designs was

carried out using the nparLD package (Noguchi et al., 2012), and ANOVA-type statistics (ATS) eran

reported. Además, we calculated paired and Welch’s unequal variances t-test and Mann-

Whitney/Wilcoxon tests for comparisons of mean values. Spearman’s rank correlation was used to

assess relationships between neurofeedback regulation success, behavioral effects, and psychosocial

factores. To explore predictors of behavioral improvements, we calculated stepwise multiple

regression models and applied an Akaike information criterion (AIC) stepwise model selection

algoritmo (akaike, 1974) to select the best models. Data are presented as means ± standard deviation

(Dakota del Sur) unless indicated otherwise. For all analyses, a p-value below 0.05 was considered significant.

Bonferroni correction was applied for the correlational analyses. We calculated Cohen’s d for mean

comparisons or the correlation coefficient after non-parametric tests and partial eta-squared (p²) para

linear mixed models.

17

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

Neurofeedback regulation success

Further preprocessing

To analyze neurofeedback regulation success, we analyzed the time series of the feedback signal

based on the online analysis adding further steps for artifact removal. Primero, we detected and

automatically removed noisy channels by calculating coefficients of variation (CoV) and excluding

channels with a CoV > 10% in HbO or HbR or channels with a variation difference between the

chromophores of over 5%. Además, channels in which we identified a flat line of at least 1s were

removed (Bell et al., 2020). If the channel covering the rTPJ (COI) was detected as a noisy channel,

we visually inspected the raw and preprocessed time series of the respective channel, and reincluded

the channel if the high CoV was driven by spikes or drifts that could be removed by our

preprocessing pipeline. The removed values were replaced by the average activation of the six

neighboring trials. Segundo, outliers were removed if they exceeded 3SDs from the mean on-trial level

and replaced by the last observation.

Additional robustness checks

Since short channel measurements were unfortunately not available for our system, we carried out an

additional stepwise offline analysis approach to further test the robustness of the observed effects and

to rule out the possibility that the neurofeedback signal change was driven by systemic physiological

signals. For the offline robustness checks we used raw fNIRS signals of the same data sets as for the

online analysis and carried out the same preprocessing and analysis steps as in the online analysis

(bad channel removal, outlier detection, interpolación, bandpass filter, 5s moving average filter, y

CBSI). Además, we applied a more stringent bandpass filter (0.01-0.09 Hz), cual es

recommended by Pinti et al. (2019) and should remove most of the systemic physiological signals

(first robustness check). In the second, more conservative robustness check, we applied the common

average reference (CAR) using the average time series of the 22 channels placed over the left

hemisphere and subtracting it from the feedback channel time series. The CAR is considered to be a

viable approach when short channel measurements are not available (Yücel et al., 2021), albeit a

suboptimal one, since there is a risk of overcorrecting the signal or inducing additional effects

depending on network activity during the task (Hudak et al., 2018; Klein et al., 2022; Kohl et al.,

2020).

For all three analysis approaches, we calculated the median and standard deviation of the

feedback signal time series for each of the 30s trials, normalized to the last five seconds of the no-

18

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

regulation period (see formula for the calculation of the feedback signal). Mean values were

calculated for each neurofeedback run and used for further group-level analyses.

Neurofeedback success measures

The main goal of the study was to test for successful control of rTPJ activation. Sin embargo, there is no

consensus on how to define neurofeedback regulation success (Kohl et al., 2020; Paret et al., 2019),

and there is evidence of insufficient reporting quality in the field of fNIRS-based neurofeedback

(Kohl et al., 2020), making it difficult to assess the effectiveness of a newly developed

neurofeedback training protocol. Por lo tanto, here we report several different neurofeedback success

measures on the group and on the individual level, each of which has implications for the conclusion

of the successful control of rTPJ activation (ver tabla 1). Besides measure of signal amplitude, nosotros

also include measures of signal variability in our analysis as they might be indicative of learning as

Bueno (see Kohl et al., 2020).

Primero, we tested whether a participant is able to (1) activate the target region in the desired

direction and maintain it over the course of the training compared to a within-baseline condition

(neurofeedback performance as compared to baseline – maintenance of activation). On the group

nivel, we used one-sample t-tests to test for a regulation effect against baseline for both groups

separately. For the analysis on the individual level, we followed an exploratory approach according

to Haugg et al. (2021) and Auer et al. (2015) to classify successful participants who maintained up-

or downregulation throughout the training, independent of a learning effect. A run was classified as

successful if the medians of the feedback signal over trials were positive in the upregulation group

and negative in the downregulation group. Participants who demonstrated more than 50% successful

neurofeedback runs were then classified as “successful”, and participants below 50% como

“unsuccessful”. The numbers of successful participants are reported. Además, we report on the

numbers of successful runs per participant (see Table S1 and S2).

These measures only provide necessary evidence for the control of baseline brain activation,

but they alone are insufficient to draw conclusions about the successful regulation of brain activation

through neurofeedback. This limitation arises because the activation of the target region could be

influenced by factors inherent to the experimental paradigm, such as stimulation from the

experimental stimuli, or the use of mental strategies, rather than solely attributable to the effects of

neurofeedback.

Stronger evidence for control would be if a participant showed a voluntary change in (2)

amplitudes and (3) variability of the feedback signal over time compared to a within-baseline

19

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

condición (neurofeedback improvement or learning). On the group level, we tested for a time effect of

rTPJ regulation using linear mixed models or non-parametric ANOVAs for both groups separately.

On the individual level, the neurofeedback improvement of each participant was calculated based on

the slope of the linear regression over all neurofeedback runs. Aquí, a participant was classified as

“successful” if he or she showed a slope larger than 0 in the upregulation group and smaller than 0 en

the downregulation group. Además, since learning does not necessarily follow a linear trajectory,

we compared the regulation success of the last session with the first session. On the group level, nosotros

used the paired t-test for both groups separately to compare rTPJ activation in the last session

compared to the first session. On the individual level, we classified a participant with a

positive/negative value as “successful” and vice versa.

Por último, we tested for a specific effect of regulation (specific evidence for control) por

comparing measures 1-3 with the between-group control condition. para hacerlo, we calculated linear

mixed models or non-parametric ANOVAs and tested for a significant group effect (1) y un

significant group × time interaction (2-3).

Mesa 1. Neurofeedback success measures

Necessary evidence for control:
Neurofeedback performance as
compared to baseline

Stronger evidence for control:
Neurofeedback improvement or
aprendiendo

Individual level
>50% successful runs
(positive/negative median)

Group level
(de)activation over all runs (prueba t)

Difference between last and first
session or slope ><0 change from first to last session (t- test), slope (mixed model) Specific evidence for control: Significant group effects N>1000 EM, como

well as incorrect key presses were excluded from the analysis. Harmonic means of valid and invalid

trials of the invalid blocks were calculated and analyzed. The harmonic mean, as recommended for

RT analysis by Ratcliff (1993),is a more unbiased estimator of the central tendency of RTs than the

arithmetic mean, which also reduces the effects of outliers while remaining high power. Además,

RTs for invalid trials were subtracted from valid trials to estimate the costs of shifting attention from

the cued position to a non-cued target (reorienting effect). Two participants, one from each group,

had to be excluded due to a technical error or not understanding task instructions. For the vPT task,

20

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

harmonic means of the RTs and mean accuracies were analyzed. One participant had to be excluded

from this analysis due to a technical error. For both tasks, linear mixed models or non-parametric

ANOVAs were calculated with the task condition and measurement time as within-subject factors

and the group as the between-subject factor. According to our hypotheses, we expected to see a

significant group × time interaction as well as a significant within-group time effect in the

upregulation group for both tasks.

To further confirm the specificity of the behavioral effects and control for unspecific

contributions of psychosocial factors, we calculated four sets of correlational analyses:

1-2) Observed behavioral effects in the attention task/vPT task were correlated with three different

neurofeedback success measures for both groups separately as well as across group resulting in 27

pruebas (3 conditions x 3 success measures x 3 grupos) for each task.

3-4) Changes in RTs of the attention task/changes in accuracies of the vPT task across conditions

were correlated with eleven results of questionnaires assessing psychosocial factors (p.ej.,

expectations toward the training, subjective evaluation of the training, etc.) Resultando en 33 pruebas (11

questionnaire results x 3 grupos) for each task.

We applied the Bonferroni correction separately for the four different sets of analyses, cada

involving a distinct number of tests (es decir., 27, 27, 33, y 33).

Predicting behavioral improvements

As neurofeedback represents a potentially useful tool for application in clinical populations exploring

how subclinical symptoms, personality traits, and baseline task performance are related to specific

behavioral neurofeedback, effects in healthy samples can inform clinical translation. In terms of TPJ

functioning, these include ASD symptoms (Kana et al., 2014, 2015, 2016) and measures of empathy

as well as baseline cognitive and socio-cognitive performance data.

We only found a specific effect in the reorienting of attention task and no specific effect in the vPT

tarea. Por lo tanto, we conducted an analysis for the effects in the reorienting of attention task using

absolute RTs across conditions as a dependent variable of a multiple regression model. We used the

results of questionnaires assessing autism-related traits and empathy (AQ, EQ, SQ, SRS, IRI) también

as baseline task performance (RTs across conditions of the reorienting of attention task and

accuracies in PT trials of the vPT task) as predictor variables (7 en total). To avoid overfitting,

stepwise multiple regression models were calculated and the AIC stepwise model selection algorithm

(akaike, 1974) was used to select the best model.

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

21

Mental strategies underlying neurofeedback regulation

Based on a content analysis of the strategy reports, we identified 20 different categories of strategies

that participants employed to regulate their brain activity. Cifra 8 in the results section shows the

different categories and their distribution. We classified the reported strategies into the different

categories, calculated how many strategies were used by each subject and how many participants

reported to have used a particular strategy. The mean number of strategies used, and the frequencies

of the different strategies were compared between groups.

3 Resultados

3.1 Baseline characteristics

There were no baseline differences between the two groups, es decir., neither in the questionnaire data nor

in the reaction times and accuracies in the (1) reorienting of attention task and (2) vPT task (all p >

0.05; ver tabla 2, Cifra 6, and Table S1-2 for more detailed baseline characteristics and

questionnaire results). Además, the thresholds for the feedback signal as determined by rTPJ

activation during the pre-assessments did not significantly differ between the groups (upregulation

group = 2.19 ± 1.45, range 0.45 – 6.2, downregulation group = 2.76 ± 1.84, Range 0.03 – 6.92).

These results demonstrate that our randomization procedure was successful. Seven participants (5 en

the upregulation and 2 in the downregulation group) showed negative contrasts for both tasks.

Por lo tanto, their initial threshold was set to a default value of 1.

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

22

Upregulation

Downregulation

(M ± SD)

(M ± SD)

27 (13 femenino)

18 (9 femenino)

p-value

norte

Age (años)

pre RTs attention task – invalid

pre RTs attention task – valid

24.22 ± 3.03

497 ± 69 EM

452 ± 61 EM

pre accuracies attention taskinvalid

0.98 ± 0.02

pre accuracies attention task – válido

0.99 ± 0.02

24.22 ± 2.71

508 ± 90 EM

468 ± 94 EM

0.98 ± 0.05

0.99 ± 0.03

pre RTs vPT task – PT

3667 ± 327 EM

3630 ± 302 EM

pre RTs vPT task – NPT

3611 ± 286 EM

3620 ± 272 EM

pre accuracies vPT task – PT

pre accuracies vPT task – NPT

pre rTPJ thresholds

AQ total

EQ total

SQ total

IRI total1

SRS: total

POMS: depression/anxiety

POMS: vigor

POMS: fatiga

POMS: hostility

0.946 ± 0.071

0.981 ± 0.035

2.19 ± 1.45

15.30 ± 6.14

45.19 ± 9.76

0.926 ± 0.091

0.971 ± 0.044

2.76 ± 1.84

13.94 ± 4.49

45.17 ± 8.28

29.96 ± 10.35

32.11 ± 13.75

56.78 ± 11.78

39.26 ± 19.36

0.40 ± 0.52

3.43 ± 1.01

1.61 ± 0.99

0.54 ± 0.93

52.39 ± 8.83

37.94 ± 12.94

0.42 ± 0.58

3.40 ± 0.90

1.53 ± 1.12

0.52 ± 0.77

General self-efficacy

30.59 ± 2.50

31.83 ± 3.57

Expectations

Motivation

2.42 ± 0.56

3.70 ± 0.40

2.67 ± 0.59

3.63 ± 0.50

Mesa 2. Baseline characteristics and questionnaire results

0.935

0.816

0.703

0.169

0.112

0.701

0.966

0.634

0.230

0.270

0.399

0.995

0.577

0.161

0.862

0.716

0.920

0.814

0.737

0.211

0.123

0.695

1 according to Cliffordson (2001) and Paulus (2012)Cliffordson (2001) and Paulus (2012); AQ, Autism Spectrum Quotient;
ASRS, Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale; EQ, Empathy Quotient; IRI, Interpersonal Reactivity Index; POMS, Profile of Mood
Estados; PT, perspective taking; NPT, non-perspective taking; SQ, Systemizing Quotient; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale;
vPT, visual perspective taking.

3.2 Regulation behavior and rewards

Both groups were able to regulate the signal in the desired direction and remained above their

individual threshold. De término medio, the upregulation group remained above the threshold in each trial

for a longer period (M = 16±3.25 of 30s) than the downregulation group (M = 9.41±2.9 of 30s), pero

23

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

only the downregulation group improved over time (see Supplementary Material 3.1 and Figure S1

for detailed results). Como resultado, the upregulation group also received significantly more monetary

rewards than the downregulation group (M = 12.80±2.33€ vs M = 7.86€±2.16€; t(38,4) = 7.29, pag < 0.001, d = 2.35). 3.3 Neurofeedback regulation success Table 3 shows the results for the different neurofeedback success measures and Figure 5 shows grand averages of HbO changes of the feedback signal for all four neurofeedback training days (sessions) and box plots of average feedback performance based on the online analysis for all twelve neurofeedback runs. Tables S5-6 show the individual results of neurofeedback regulation success. Table 3. Neurofeedback regulation success Upregulation group NF performance - compared to baseline NF improvement (slope) NF improvement (last vs first) Downregulation group Online analysis (amplitudes) Online analysis (variability) M±SD 2.84±2.2 0.01±0.34 0.02±3.7 n 26/27 14/27 15/27 p-value < 0.001* M±SD N/A n N/A 0.84 0.97 0.00±0.17 -0.06±1.69 17/27 17/27 p-value N/A 0.595 0.86 M±SD 2.39±1.89 n 4/18 p-value 1 M±SD N/A n N/A p-value N/A NF performance - compared to baseline 0.143 15/18 NF improvement (slope) NF improvement (last vs first) 0.004* 15/18 Neurofeedback regulation success according to different success measures for both groups. The p-values reflect the results of the group analysis as described in 2.5 “Data processing and analysis”; NF, neurofeedback. Regulation success (amplitudes) -0.11±0.17 -1.37±1.76 -0.22±0.35 -2.36±3.6 15/18 15/18 0.09 0.01* In the upregulation group, we observed high rTPJ activation that was sustained over the course of the training. In contrast, the downregulation group unexpectedly showed the same effect (activation instead of deactivation), which, however, disappeared over the course of the training. One-sample t- tests revealed a significant main effect of regulation over all runs in the upregulation group (M = 2.84±2.2, t(26) = 6.72, p < 0.001, d = 1.32) and the downregulation group (M = 2.39±1.89, t(17) = 5.6, p < 0.001, d = 1.36), meaning that on average, rTPJ activity also increased in the downregulation group. Paired-sample t-tests, however, only revealed a significant decrease between the last and the first session in the downregulation group (Mdiff = -2.36±3.6, t(17) = 2.79, p = 0.01, d = 0.68), but no significant increase in the upregulation group (Mdiff = 0.02±3.7, p > 0.98, re = -0.01). The non-

parametric ANOVA only revealed a non-significant time trend in the downregulation group (FATS

24

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

(5.85, ∞) = 1.86, pag = 0.09) and no effect in the upregulation group. No specific group effect or

significant group × time interaction was found.

The analysis on the individual level revealed that in the upregulation group, 96.30% del

Participantes (26 de 27) were successfully upregulating rTPJ activity (>50% successful runs; m = 9.63

± 2.27), 51.85% (14 de 27) showed an improvement of regulation performance over runs as indicated

by a positive slope, y 55.56% (15 de 27) showed a higher regulation performance in the last session

compared to the first session. In the downregulation group, solo 22.22 % (4 de 18) were successfully

downregulating rTPJ activity (>50% successful runs; m = 3 ± 2.66), 83.33% (15 de 18) showed an

improvement of regulation performance over runs as indicated by a negative slope and a higher

regulation performance in the last compared to the first session.

Regulation success (variabilidad)

For the variability of the neurofeedback performance over time, similar results compared to the main

analysis of regulation success (analysis based on signal amplitudes) were observed. Paired-sample t-

tests also revealed a difference between the last and the first session in the downregulation group

(Mdiff = -1.37±1.76, t(17) = 3.29, pag = 0.004, re = 0.8) but not in the upregulation group (Mdiff =

0.06±1.69, pag = 0.86, re = 0.03). The non-parametric ANOVA only revealed a non-significant time

trend in the downregulation group (FATS (7.27, ∞) = 1.55, pag = 0.143). No specific group effect or

significant group × time interaction was found.

The individual analysis revealed that in the upregulation group, 62.96% of the participants

(17 de 27) showed decreasing standard deviations over runs, as indicated by a negative slope of the

regression and lower values in the last session compared to the first session. Por otro lado, en el

downregulation group, 15 out of 18 Participantes (83.33%) showed decreasing standard deviations

over runs, as indicated by a negative slope of the regression and lower values in the last session

compared to the first session.

Robustness checks

Robustness check 1 successfully confirmed the results of the online analysis. Sin embargo, none of the

effects survived the more conservative robustness check 2 (see Supplementary Material 3.3 para

detailed results).

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

25

Cifra 5. Neurofeedback regulation performance. The first row shows the grand averages of the

changes in HbO of the feedback channel for the four neurofeedback training days (sessions). El

second row shows box plots of the average feedback performance as assessed by the standardized

median change of rTPJ activation averaged over participants for all twelve neurofeedback runs

(sessions color-coded) based on the online analysis. The third row shows box plots of the standard

26

Descargado de http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 Septiembre 2023

deviations of feedback performance as assessed by the standardized median change of right TPJ

activation averaged over participants for each run. The regression lines of the linear models are

depicted in red. Paired-sample t-tests comparing the last session (run 11 y 12) with the first session

(run 1 y 2) revealed significant effects and the ANOVA over all neurofeedback runs only revealed

non-significant time trends in the downregulation group only. No time effect was observed in the

upregulation group (see main text).

3.4 Primary behavioral outcomes

Cifra 6. Primary behavioral outcomes. Results of the reorienting of attention task (upper panel) y

visual perspective-taking task (lower panel). For detailed descriptive statistics, see Table S2.

Boxplots show interquartile range ± 1.5 (whiskers). Asterisks denote the significance for the group ×

time interaction and within-group time effects across task conditions; *** pag < 0.001; * p < 0.05. Reorienting of attention task As expected, we found a significant main effect of condition for RTs (F(1,123) = 111.21, p < 0.001, p² = 0.47), and accuracy data (FATS (1, ∞) = 17.18, p < 0.001), reflecting a significant reorienting 27 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 effect in both groups across time points (mean RTs valid = 456 ± 77ms, mean RTs invalid = 499 ± 80ms). The hypothesized three-way interaction of group × time × condition was not observed, i.e., no significant time effects or group × time interaction effects were observed for the reorienting effect. However, we found a significant group × time interaction (F(1,123) = 17.17, p < 0.001, p² = 0.12), and a significant main effect of time in both groups (upregulation group: (FATS (1, ∞) = 6.20, p = 0.013), downregulation group: (FATS (1, ∞) = 4.42, p = 0.036), indicating a group-specific effect of the training on RTs across conditions. The pre-post comparisons revealed that after the neurofeedback training, reaction times across conditions decreased in the upregulation group (pre = 474 ± 68ms, post = 457 ± 57ms, d = 0.51) and increased in the downregulation group across conditions (pre = 488 ± 93ms, post = 503 ± 108ms, d = -0.56). No other main effects or interactions were found (see Figure 6). If we included trials from the valid only blocks, results did not change, but the time effect in the downregulation group (FATS (1, ∞) = 3.25, p = 0.071) failed to reach significance (see Supplementary Material 4). vPT task Contrary to our hypothesis, the three-way interaction of group × time × condition was neither observed for RTs nor for accuracies in the vPT task. RTs decreased in both groups (F(1,126) = 55.58, p < 0.001, , p² = 0.31) irrespective of condition (pre = 3630 ± 270ms, post = 3500 ± 295ms, d = 0.83). No other main effects or interactions were significant. Accuracies increased in both groups (pre = 95.8 ± 3.1%, post = 98.3 ± 6.5%, d = 0.58), as indicated by a significant time effect (FATS (1, ∞) = 11.91, p < 0.001) and a significant condition effect (FATS (1, ∞) = 10.75, p < 0.005), but no interaction effect occurred. However, a ceiling effect was observed in this task. The majority of the participants responded with 100% accuracy in this task during the pre-assessment (29 in the NPT and 18 in the PT condition) and during the post-assessment (34 in the NPT and 29 in the PT condition; see Figure 6)). 3.5 Mental strategies, secondary outcomes, and unspecific psychological effects Mental strategies underlying neurofeedback regulation The downregulation group used significantly more different strategies (M = 8.66±2.47) during the neurofeedback training compared to the upregulation group (M = 6.26±3.24; t(42.11) = 2.82, p = 0.007, d = 0.87). Figure 7 shows the distribution of strategies as reported by the participants of both 28 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 groups. Fisher’s exact Chi-square test revealed no significant association between the group and reported strategies (p = 0.982), indicating that similar strategies were used for both upregulating and downregulating TPJ activity. Table S8 shows the percentages of strategies relative to the total number of strategies reported per group and their mean success rating. In total, most strategies were reported to be more successful in the upregulation group (mean success rating: 3.35) than in the downregulation group (mean success rating: 2.74), and socio-cognitive strategies and positive mental imagery were reported most frequently in both groups (see Supplementary Material 5). Figure 7. Strategies as reported by participants for each group. Motivation, self-control beliefs, self-efficacy, and mood Motivation to take further part in the neurofeedback training as assessed after each session was high in both groups (Mdn = 9, on a 10-point rating scale), but decreased slightly in the upregulation group over the course of the sessions. There was a significant time effect (FATS (2.34, ∞) = 3.11, p = 0.04), which was driven by a simple main effect of time in the upregulation group (FATS 29 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 (2.54, ∞) = 3.48, p = 0.02). No time effect was observed in the downregulation group. Post hoc comparisons of the last session with the first session confirmed a slight, but significant, decrease of motivation in the upregulation group (first session, Mdn = 9, last session, Mdn = 8, p = 0.02, r = 0.491) and no effect in the downregulation group (first session, Mdn = 9.5, last session, Mdn = 9.5, p = 0.43, r = 0.124; see Table S4). For the general self-efficacy scale, we found a significant time effect (F(1,43) = 4.93, p = 0.03, p² = 0.10). Although the group × time interaction failed to reach significance (F(1,43) = 2.39, p = 0.129, p² = 0.05), this effect seemed to be driven by an increase in the upregulation group from pre- (M = 30.59 ± 2.5) to post-assessments (M = 32.04 ± 3.16). This was indicated by a simple main effect of time, which, however, failed to reach significance (F(1,52) = 3.48, p = 0.07, p² = 0.06). No time effect was observed in the downregulation group (F(1,34) = 0.02, p = 0.89, p² = 0; see Table S3). Participants’ beliefs of how well they could control the neurofeedback signal was lower in the downregulation group at the beginning of the training, but increased to the level of the upregulation group towards the end of the training, as indicated by a significant time effect (F(3,128.3) = 3.36, p = 0.02, p² = 0.07), group effect (F(1,42.98) = 11.88, p = 0.001, p² = 0.26) as well as a significant group  time interaction (F(3,128.2) = 6.17, p < 0.001, p² = 0.12). A simple main effect of time was only observed in the downregulation group (F(3,68) = 4.75, p = 0.005, p² = 0.15). Post hoc t-tests indicated that there was a group difference in the first neurofeedback session (upregulation group = 7.19±1.42, downregulation group = 4.44±1.82, t(30.53) = 5.37, p < 0.001, d = 1.95) and the second neurofeedback session (upregulation group = 6.92±1.5, downregulation group = 5.72±1.64, t(34.46) = 2.48, p = 0.02, d = 0.87), which disappeared in the third session (upregulation group = 7.08±1.35, downregulation group = 6.56±1.82) and the last session (upregulation group = 6.88±1.82, downregulation group = 6.36±2.11) see Table S4). The neurofeedback training showed no significant effect on mood states, as assessed with the POMS (see Table S3). Expectations and evaluations of the training No differences were found between the groups with respect to the expectation towards the neurofeedback training and the subjective evaluation of the training (believed efficacy, joy, and experimenter). The debriefing questionnaires revealed, however, that 71.11% of the participants (80.77% in the upregulation and 55.56% in the downregulation group) guessed the group assignment correctly, although most participants reported that they were not confident about their judgement. 30 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 3.6 Correlations of behavioral outcomes with regulation performance and psychosocial factors We found a significant negative correlation between changes in RTs in the valid trials of the reorienting of attention task and neurofeedback performance, as assessed by the number of successful runs (rho = -0.47, p = 0.045, Bonferroni corrected), indicating higher improvements of RTs in participants with more successful runs in both groups. Subgroup analysis revealed no significant effect after Bonferroni correction. For the perspective-taking task, we found a significant correlation between neurofeedback improvement (slopes) and improvements in the accuracies of NPT trials across groups (rho = -0.49, p = 0.02), indicating greater performance improvements in participants who were more successful in learning downregulation over the course of the training. This significant correlation was only observed in the downregulation group (rho = -0.71, p = 0.039, Bonferroni corrected). None of the psychosocial factors correlated significantly with behavioral outcomes after Bonferroni correction. For more details including significant correlations on the uncorrected level, see Supplementary Material 6. 3.7 Predicting behavioral improvements For the neurophysiologically specific improvements observed in the attention task, we found that IRI total scores and baseline performance in the attention task predicted changes in performance (see Table 4). The subgroup models revealed that baseline attentional performance and EQ scores only predicted behavioral improvements in the upregulation group, thus indicating greater improvements in participants with lower baseline performance and higher EQ scores. In the downregulation group, IRI scores and baseline vPT performance predicted decreased performance in the attention task after the training. Table 4. Summary statistics of the stepwise multiple linear regression model predicting behavioral improvements. Model summary – both groups R2 0.377 Coefficients Step Intercept IRI total pre RTs attention task Adjusted R2 0.346 Residual SE 0.022 Beta -0.102 0.001 0.17 SE 0.027 0.0004 0.047 F(2,39) 11.82 t-value -3.836 2.143 3.651 p-value <0.001*** p-value <0.001*** 0.038* <0.001*** Model summary – Upregulation group 31 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 R2 0.513 Coefficients Step Intercept EQ pre RTs attention task Adjusted R2 0.471 Residual SE 0.026 Beta 0.169 -0.001 -0.271 SE 0.039 0.001 0.086 F(2,23) 12.12 t-value 4.308 -2.311 -3.170 p-value <0.001*** p-value <0.001*** 0.03* 0.004** Adjusted R2 0.38 Model summary – Downregulation group R2 0.463 Coefficients Step Intercept IRI total pre accuracy vPT task Note that we used absolute DRTs in the reorienting of attention task in the model including both groups but the real DRTs in the subgroup models. p-value 0.008** 0.017* 0.021* t-value -3.106 2.752 2.634 Beta -0.403 0.003 0.286 SE 0.13 0.001 0.108 Residual SE 0.023 p-value 0.018* F(2,13) 5.596 4 Discussion This is the first study demonstrating the feasibility and effectiveness of neurofeedback training of the rTPJ based on fNIRS. We demonstrated successful activation of the rTPJ compared to baseline (necessary evidence for control) within the first training session (2 neurofeedback runs) in the upregulation group. Only one of 27 participants in this group failed to activate (<50% successful trials). However, we observed no significant effect of neurofeedback improvement; almost half of the participants (13 of 27) failed to show a positive slope. Successful downregulation, on the other hand, required at least four sessions (12 neurofeedback runs) or more. Most participants failed to successfully downregulate, but a significant neurofeedback improvement effect was observed in this group and only three of 18 participants failed to show such an effect. Surprisingly, participants in the downregulation group were also activating their rTPJ at the beginning of the training but learnt to downregulate or at least to not activate it anymore towards the end of the training. This can be interpreted as strong evidence for control in the downregulation group. While only unspecific improvements were observed for vPT, specific up/down-regulatory effects on stimulus-driven attention were observed in the reorienting of attention task, providing evidence for a neurophysiological specific effect of rTPJ regulation on stimulus-driven spatial attention, although not specifically related to the reorienting process of attention (as indicated by a reduced invalidity effect). Neurophysiological specificity was further confirmed by the fact that non- specific psychological mechanisms and mental strategies did not differ between groups and therefore cannot explain the group effect. The training was well received by the young and healthy participants 32 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 with no dropouts as well as high levels of motivation and feelings of control reported throughout the training. 4.1 Neurofeedback regulation success While we demonstrate the feasibility and effectiveness of a neurofeedback training of the rTPJ, the specific results of different neurofeedback success measures, in conjunction with the findings of the behavioral effects, yield a complex picture. As both groups showed high activation of the rTPJ from the beginning of the training and only the downregulation group showed a learning effect we cannot derive definitive conclusion regarding the effectiveness of a neurofeedback upregulation training. The initial high activation of the rTPJ might be explained by the contribution of general neurofeedback regulation mechanism by a neurofeedback controller network. Such a separate controller network involves neural populations of the TPJ (Emmert et al., 2016; Sitaram et al., 2017) possibly related to the integration of visual feedback as well as other feedback-related processes such as prediction processing (Bzdok et al., 2013). Therefore, there might have been an overlap of the neural populations of the neurofeedback controller network with the neurofeedback target region, meaning that the measured activity at rTPJ could have been a combination of the two. This potential overlap complicates the interpretation of activity changes and the relevance of feedback. One may speculate that during the regulation period, the controller network initially increased activity at rTPJ, but extended learning led to changes in the network, potentially reducing its activity and leading to complex effects on the measured upregulation and downregulation conditions. In such a scenario decreased activity in the controller network counteracted a potential increase over time in the target region, diminishing an observable learning effect. While the baseline period served as a control for stimuli-evoked activity, it did not account for baseline activity specifically related to the controller network, which was only engaged during the regulation period. However, we acknowledge the speculative nature of this account, which can only be confirmed through fMRI studies employing more fine-grained measures and estimations of the neurofeedback controller network. Furthermore, the complexity of the social neurofeedback stimuli as well as the instructions used in our design may explain the initial high activation of the rTPJ. The rTPJ involves parts of the posterior STS, an area which has been attributed to the face processing network, and a subregion of the STS closely located to anterior parts of the rTPJ which has been associated with biological motion as well as emotional face processing (Beauchamp, 2015; Müller et al., 2018). Although we used digitizer measurements to ensure the correct placements of the feedback channels over anterior parts of the rTPJ, given the spatial resolution of fNIRS and the variability in optode placements we 33 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 cannot exclude the possibility that the feedback channel captured the activation of this subregion of the STS – at least in some of the participants. The activation of the feedback channel might therefore have been partly induced by the feedback stimuli when the avatar started smiling or even by participants paying more attention to the facial stimuli during the regulation condition. Lastly, this effect might be explained by the fact that both groups received the same strategy instructions and as a result relied heavily on socio-cognitive strategies associated with rTPJ activation (Bzdok et al., 2013). The absence of a learning effect in the upregulation group made it also difficult to detect, and may explain, the absence of a significant group x time interaction effect in the current study (specific evidence for control). Nevertheless, the observed neurofeedback learning effect in the downregulation group, along with the specific effects on the behavioral level, provides interesting and encouraging findings as they indicate a neurophysiologically specific mechanism of rrTPJ regulation on stimulus-driven attention. These results have important implications for future study designs and clinical translation we discuss below (see section 4.2 and 4.5). The first robustness check further confirmed the results of the online analysis, but the second, more conservative robustness check did not. This should be interpreted with caution, since given the limited spatial resolution and coverage in our study the CAR approach involves the risk of overcorrecting the signal or inducing additional effects depending on network activity during the task (Hudak et al., 2018; Klein et al., 2022; Kohl et al., 2020). In particular, the first approach (bandpass filter of 0.01-0.09 Hz) is capable of removing most of the frequencies associated with systemic physiology, including heart rate (~1Hz), breathing rate (~0.3Hz), and Mayer waves (~0.1Hz; Pinti et al., 2019). Moreover, we took care to keep the contribution of systemic physiological changes in our experimental paradigm at a low level by using variable stimulus onsets and instructing our young and healthy participants to calm down before the experiment, breathe regularly, and avoid unnecessary movements. Therefore, we can assume that it is very unlikely that the observed effects were driven by systemic physiology, but instead by the real neural activation of the rTPJ. 4.2 Primary behavioral outcomes The upregulation group showed increased performance and the downregulation group decreased performance in the reorienting of attention task across conditions. Our single-blinded, bidirectional- regulation control group design allowed us to properly control for neurofeedback non-specific or general non-specific effects and demonstrate neurophysiological specificity of the observed 34 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 behavioral effects. It is unlikely that differences in the employed strategies can explain the behavioral effects, since although the downregulation group showed a higher variation of strategies, both groups relied on the same strategies to regulate their brain activity. The absence of group-specific correlations of regulation success with behavioral outcomes and the missing group-specific effect in the vPT task, together with other non-significant correlations between behavioral improvements and psychosocial factors, underline the role of other non-specific psychosocial mechanisms such as reward, control beliefs, and expectations in explaining the behavioral effects. However, given the strength of our study design, the observed dissociation in the reorienting of attention task provides evidence for a specific neurophysiological effect of rTPJ regulation on stimulus-driven attention. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find a specific effect on the reorienting of attention, i.e., a specific improvement in invalid trials in the upregulation group. This is surprising given the assumed specific role of the rTPJ in reorienting of attention (Krall et al., 2015), which has been supported by previous neurostimulation studies (Krall et al., 2016; Roy et al., 2015). However, neurofeedback is different from neurostimulation. Instead of passively receiving neurostimulation, neurofeedback training requires the active participation of the participant and the skill of neural regulation to be successfully learned, which involves the recruitment of additional neural networks throughout the training (Emmert et al., 2016; Sitaram et al., 2017) that may have induced additional behavioral effects. Possible downstream effects of rTPJ regulation on other brain activities may have also resulted in additional behavioral effects (Kvamme et al., 2022). Furthermore, the specific role of the rTPJ in early stimulus-driven attentional reorienting has been questioned by Geng and Vossel (2013), who suggest a rather general role in post-perceptual contextual updating and adjustments of top-down expectations. We found increased RTs after downregulation training indicating decreased performance in stimulus-driven attention, which is an interesting albeit preliminary finding. To date, only a few studies have applied a bidirectional control group approach and demonstrated group-specific changes, also including decreases in performance, for example sustained attention and response inhibition in the study of Yamashita et al. (2017). Future studies should test the robustness of these effects, assess potential long-term effects, and explore if decreased performance after downregulation can also be observed in other cognitive domains. If so, the bidirectional control group approach would be an interesting tool for cognitive neuroscience studies, since it is more efficient and the demonstration of such a dissociation provides stronger (causal) evidence than just an upregulation effect. However, caution is advised with respect to long-term effects and when such designs are applied to clinical populations. 35 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 Only unspecific improvements were found for vPT. The observed improvement might have been the result of a retest effect or a ceiling effect, which was observed for most of the participants and may have masked a group-specific effect in this task. Beneficial effects of rTPJ stimulation were demonstrated by tDCS studies (Santiesteban et al., 2012, 2015), but the evidence is mixed (Nobusako et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2020) with most of the studies applying a between-subject design lacking a baseline control. Moreover, accuracies were substantially lower than in our study, particularly after sham or occipital stimulation, which left more room for improvement in these samples than in ours. Therefore, investigating whether clinical or subclinical samples that are characterized by decreased perspective-taking performance may benefit from neurofeedback of the rTPJ should be addressed in future studies. Lastly, more difficult perspective-taking tasks need to be designed to avoid ceiling effects in participants with high cognitive performance. 4.3 Secondary outcomes and non-specific mechanisms Self-efficacy improved after the training, and, although we did not find a significant interaction effect, this effect seemed to be slightly more pronounced in the upregulation group. A number of neurofeedback studies have demonstrated improvements in domain-specific or general self-efficacy in different clinical samples and have discussed improvements of self-efficacy as a psychological mechanism mediating the effect of neurofeedback training (Hershaw et al., 2020; Ko & Park, 2018; Markiewicz et al., 2021; Mehler et al., 2018; Schmidt & Martin, 2016, 2020). We were unable to find significant correlations between changes in self-efficacy and behavioral improvements in cognitive tasks. Self-efficacy might therefore be a psychological mechanism that mediates the effects on symptom improvement in clinical samples, but this was not observed in the current sample and thus cannot be responsible for the cognitive improvement observed in the reorienting of attention task in young and healthy participants. Regarding non-specific mechanisms, we were unable to find between-group differences in expectation towards the neurofeedback training and with respect to the evaluation of the training. Motivation slightly decreased in the upregulation group, although it remained at a high level. The decrease in motivation might be explained by the lower level of difficulty of the upregulation training compared to the downregulation training, which was experienced to be more challenging. Participants in the downregulation group also showed lower control beliefs than the upregulation group at the beginning of the training, but this difference disappeared over the course of the training once participants in the downregulation group were regulating more successfully. At the end of the 36 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 training, we found a significant group difference in the amount of the monetary rewards received, which occurred due to the higher regulation success in the upregulation group. Unfortunately, subjective reward experience was not assessed in this study. It is possible, however, that higher reward experience in the upregulation group may have contributed to the differential behavioral effects in the reorienting of attention task. Indeed, we found a small, albeit insignificant, correlation of reward with improvements in this task. It is worth noting, however, that such a difference in reward experience should have affected the perspective-taking task as well, and differences in mood states, motivation, evaluation of the training, or control beliefs were not found. In summary, these findings indicate a low influence of non-specific psychological mechanisms such as reward, treatment expectations, motivation, and control beliefs (Ros et al., 2020) and further support a neurophysiologically specific mechanism of rTPJ regulation on stimulus-driven attention. 4.4 Predicting neurofeedback success The finding that improvements in stimulus-driven attention were predicted by lower baseline performance is promising for clinical translation. Clinical populations characterized by difficulties in stimulus-driven attention, for example ASD (Kana et al., 2014; Landry & Parker, 2013), may benefit more from the training than our healthy sample. In particular, while these findings are promising from a clinical translation perspective, we acknowledge that conclusions are limited to a healthy population. Nevertheless, these exploratory findings allow us to hypothesize that measures of empathy, as well as the baseline task performance of the outcome measures, have a predictive value for the behavioral effects of neurofeedback training of the rTPJ. In this context, it is also noteworthy that comorbid impulsivity symptoms may moderate the effects of a neurofeedback intervention in ASD and should therefore also be assessed in future studies (Prillinger et al., 2022). Testing these hypotheses in confirmatory study designs including clinical samples will allow to identify and select responders of a neurofeedback intervention, which is important when it comes to the clinical translation of personalized TPJ neurofeedback protocols. 4.5 Limitations and future directions This study has some limitations that are worth discussing. Since this was the first study investigating the efficacy of fNIRS neurofeedback of the rTPJ, potential effect sizes were unknown and therefore the sample size was not determined based on an a-priori statistical power analysis. 37 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 While this may have resulted in insufficient statistical power to detect small effect sizes such as the hypothesized group x time interaction effect in regulation performance, it is worth underlining the large sample size in our study compared to the current state of the fNIRS neurofeedback field (Kohl et al., 2020). Secondly, both groups showed high activation from the beginning of the training and no learning effect in the upregulation group was observed, which may lead to the conclusion that mere mental rehearsal and stimulation through social stimuli is sufficient, and that neurofeedback is not necessary to regulate rTPJ activity. In subsequent studies, it could prove advantageous to implement longer training regimes to possibly foster a learning effect in the upregulation group as well. Additionally, employing more neutral stimuli, like thermometer images, and refraining from suggesting example strategies, as well as implementing controls for the activation of a coincident neurofeedback control network, could assist in isolating a specific mechanism of rTPJ upregulation. Finally, the inclusion of extra control groups, such as a mental rehearsal group or a sham feedback group, would aid in affirming a specific mechanism as observed in the attention task. Thirdly, this study did not involve short-distance measurement or other recommended measures of systemic physiology (Yücel et al., 2021). With the increasing availability of state-of-the art online artifact control measures and signal processing methods (Klein and Kranczioch, 2019; Lühmann et al., 2020; Klein et al., 2022; Schroeder et al., 2023), as well as hardware featuring an expanded channel count, spatial coverage and the inclusion of short-distance channels, future studies will be able to better control for systemic physiology but also for signals from irrelevant brain regions (spatial specificity). However, we took care to keep the contribution of systemic physiological changes in our design to a minimum and assessed the robustness of the online analysis through an additional offline analysis using more stringent preprocessing methods Furthermore, care should be taken when setting and adapting feedback thresholds, particularly when differences in target regulation difficulty can be expected. Feedback thresholds were based on online assessments of rTPJ activity during the tasks before the training. We found a large variation in the assessments (range: 0.03 – 6.92), which may have been the result of suboptimal online processing methods and artifact control and may have made the training too easy or too difficult for some of the participants. Future studies can use better online processing methods and thus exclude extreme values that are likely the result of noisy measures. To avoid differences in rewards, the thresholds should be adapted more carefully throughout the training and take into account differences in regulation difficulty, as present in our bidirectional control group design, for example the downregulation group should start with lower and smaller increases of feedback thresholds. 38 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 In addition, we had some issues with blinding the participants. Participants were informed about the bidirectional control group approach and the debriefing revealed that some participants associated “downregulation” with being more difficult or less successful in the training, while “upregulation” was associated with the opposite. This might explain why more than 80% in the upregulation group guessed the group assignment correctly, although none of the participants were confident about their judgement. Notably, this lack of blinding did not seem to have an influence on participants, as evidenced by the absence of group differences in motivation, control beliefs, expectation towards the training, and evaluation of the training. Future neurofeedback experiments employing a bidirectional control group approach should take care to avoid such associations when designing the instructions for the participants. If ethically justifiable, participants should not be informed about the existence of a control condition, or at least not be informed about a downregulation condition, but rather be instructed that there are two groups in which different patterns of brain activity are reinforced. 4.5 Conclusion In summary, this is the first study that demonstrated the feasibility and effectiveness of fNIRS-based neurofeedback training of the rTPJ. We present preliminary causal evidence that regulation of rTPJ activity affects stimulus-driven attention. However, it remains unclear if fNIRS-based neurofeedback can modulate social cognition. Future studies including longer training regimes and better controls are required to corroborate these initial findings in larger samples using state-of-the-art fNIRS methods. This study sets the ground for future investigations in clinical populations that are characterized by the aberrant functioning of the rTPJ or difficulties in stimulus-driven spatial attention. Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 39 Data availability statement Data from this study will be made available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/gbn2r/) after publication of the study, as far as data protection regulations permit. CRediT authorship contribution statement SHK: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing - Review & Editing, Visualization, Project administration PM: Investigation, Data curation JU: Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Data curation, Visualization ML: Methodology, Software, Writing – Review & Editing LB: Formal analysis, Writing – Review & Editing, Visualization DMAM: Writing – Review & Editing SRS: Writing – Review & Editing SV: Methodology, Writing – Review & Editing, Visualization KK: Conceptualization, Resources, Writing – Review & Editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition Declaration of competing interests SHK was an employee of MEDIACC GmbH, Berlin, an independent clinical research organization. SHK and DMAM received payments to consult with Mendi Innovations AB, Stockholm, Sweden. LB receives commissions for fNIRS visualizations. ML is an employee of the research company Brain Innovation B.V., Maastricht, the Netherlands. None of the above-mentioned companies were in relationship with or support of this work. The remaining authors declare no conflicts of interest. Acknowledgments This work was supported by the German Research Foundation’s (DFG) International Research Training Group “The Neuroscience of Modulating Aggression and Impulsivity in Psychopathology” (IRTG-2150). SHK was supported by a fellowship from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS). The purchase of the Hitachi fNIRS system for the University Hospital RWTH Aachen (Germany) was supported by funding from the German Research Foundation (DFG; INST 948/18-1 FUGG), awarded to KK. KK is further supported by the German Research Foundation – DFG: Project-ID 431549029 –SFB 1451. DMAM is supported by a Junior Principal Investigator (JPI) fellowship funded by the Excellence Strategy of the Federal 621 Government and the Laender (grant reference number: JPI074-21). SRS is supported by European Research Council (ERC) under the project NGBMI (759370) and TIMS (101081905), the Federal Ministry of Research and Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 40 Education (BMBF) under the projects SSMART (01DR21025A), NEO (13GW0483C), QHMI (03ZU1110DD) and QSHIFT (01UX2211), as well as the Einstein Foundation Berlin (A-2019-558). We would like to thank Dr. Iroise Dumontheil for providing the stimuli of the perspective-taking task and Dr. Simone Vossel for providing the stimuli of the reorienting of attention task. We would also like to thank Mert Asil Türeli for helping to program the experimental paradigms and Růžena Ceralová for her contribution to data management. Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 41 References Aasted, C. M., Yücel, M. A., Cooper, R. J., Dubb, J., Tsuzuki, D., Becerra, L., Petkov, M. P., Borsook, D., Dan, I., & Boas, D. A. (2015). Anatomical guidance for functional near-infrared spectroscopy: AtlasViewer tutorial. Neurophotonics, 2(2), 020801. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.nph.2.2.020801 Akaike, H. (1974). A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716–723. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705 Auer, T., Schweizer, R., & Frahm, J. (2015). Training efficiency and transfer success in an extended real-time functional MRI neurofeedback training of the somatomotor cortex of healthy subjects. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(OCT), 547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00547 Barman, A., Richter, S., Soch, J., Deibele, A., Richter, A., Assmann, A., Wüstenberg, T., Walter, H., Seidenbecher, C. I., & Schott, B. H. (2015). Gender-specific modulation of neural mechanisms underlying social reward processing by Autism Quotient. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(11), 1537–1547. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCAN/NSV044 Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 361–374. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1206 Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. In Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (Vol. 34, Issue 2, pp. 163–175). Kluwer Academic Publishers- Plenum Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00 Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism- Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Males and Females, Scientists and Mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005653411471 Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 Beauchamp, M. S. (2015). The social mysteries of the superior temporal sulcus. In Trends in Cognitive Sciences (Vol. 19, Issue 9, pp. 489–490). Elsevier Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.002 Bell, L., Scharke, W., Reindl, V., Fels, J., Neuschaefer-Rube, C., & Konrad, K. (2020). Auditory and visual response inhibition in children with bilateral hearing aids and children with adhd. Brain Sciences, 10(5), 307. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050307 Bölte, S. (2012). Brief report: The Social Responsiveness Scale for adults (SRS-A): Initial results in a German cohort. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(9), 1998–1999. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1424-5 Carter, R. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2013). A Nexus Model of the Temporal-Parietal Junction. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007 Cliffordson, C. (2001). Parent’s Judgments and Students’ Self-Judgments of Empathy: The Structure of Empathy and Agreement of Judgment Based on the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) Grade and grade assignment: Function and effects View project Self care and health among older people in Norway View project. Article in European Journal of Psychological Assessment. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.17.1.36 Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017 42 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 Cui, X., Bray, S., & Reiss, A. L. (2010). Functional near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) signal improvement based on negative correlation between oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin dynamics. NeuroImage, 49(4), 3039–3046. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.050 Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition. The Neuroscientist, 13(6), 580–593. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858407304654 Direito, B., Lima, J., Simões, M., Sayal, A., Sousa, T., Lührs, M., Ferreira, C., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2019). Targeting dynamic facial processing mechanisms in superior temporal sulcus using a novel fMRI neurofeedback target. Neuroscience, 406, 97–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.02.024 Direito, B., Mouga, S., Sayal, A., Simões, M., Quental, H., Bernardino, I., Playle, R., McNamara, R., Linden, D. E. J., Oliveira, G., & Castelo Branco, M. (2021). Training the social brain: Clinical and neural effects of an 8-week real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback Phase IIa Clinical Trial in Autism. Autism, 25(6), 1746–1760. https://doi.org/10.1177/13623613211002052 Donaldson, P. H., Kirkovski, M., Rinehart, N. J., & Enticott, P. G. (2018). Autism-relevant traits interact with temporoparietal junction stimulation effects on social cognition: a high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation and electroencephalography study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(6), 669–681. https://doi.org/10.1111/EJN.13675 Dumontheil, I., Küster, O., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Taking perspective into account in a communicative task. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1574–1583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.056 Emmert, K., Kopel, R., Sulzer, J., Brühl, A. B., Berman, B. D., Linden, D. E. J., Horovitz, S. G., Breimhorst, M., Caria, A., Frank, S., Johnston, S., Long, Z., Paret, C., Robineau, F., Veit, R., Bartsch, A., Beckmann, C. F., van de Ville, D., & Haller, S. (2016). Meta-analysis of real-time fMRI neurofeedback studies using individual participant data: How is brain regulation mediated? NeuroImage, 124, 806–812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.042 Esse Wilson, J., Trumbo, M. C., Wilson, J. K., & Tesche, C. D. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) for social cognition and social skills in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Neural Transmission 2018 125:12, 125(12), 1857–1866. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00702-018-1938-5 Geng, J. J., & Vossel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 2608–2620. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2013.08.010 Greer, S. M., Trujillo, A. J., Glover, G. H., & Knutson, B. (2014). Control of nucleus accumbens activity with neurofeedback. NeuroImage, 96, 237–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.073 Harmelech, T., Friedman, D., & Malach, R. (2015). Differential Magnetic Resonance Neurofeedback Modulations across Extrinsic (Visual) and Intrinsic (Default-Mode) Nodes of the Human Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(6), 2588–2595. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3098- 14.2015 Hartz, A., Guth, B., Jording, M., Vogeley, K., & Schulte-Rüther, M. (2021). Temporal Behavioral Parameters of On-Going Gaze Encounters in a Virtual Environment. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 3056. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2021.673982/BIBTEX Haugg, A., Renz, F. M., Nicholson, A. A., Lor, C., Götzendorfer, S. J., Sladky, R., Skouras, S., McDonald, A., Craddock, C., Hellrung, L., Kirschner, M., Herdener, M., Koush, Y., Papoutsi, 43 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 M., Keynan, J., Hendler, T., Cohen Kadosh, K., Zich, C., Kohl, S. H., … Steyrl, D. (2021). Predictors of real-time fMRI neurofeedback performance and improvement – A machine learning mega-analysis. NeuroImage, 237, 118207. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2021.118207 Hershaw, J. N., Hill-Pearson, C. A., Arango, J. I., Souvignier, A. R., & Pazdan, R. M. (2020). Semi- Automated Neurofeedback Therapy for Persistent Postconcussive Symptoms in a Military Clinical Setting: A Feasibility Study. Military Medicine, 185(3–4), e457–e465. https://doi.org/10.1093/MILMED/USZ335 Hudak, J., Rosenbaum, D., Barth, B., Fallgatter, A. J., & Ehlis, A. C. (2018). Functionally disconnected: A look at how study design influences neurofeedback data and mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One, 13(8), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200931 Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298. https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.48.00D280 Jasper, H. H. (1958). The Ten-Twenty Electrode System of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375. Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E., Hu, C. P., Deshpande, H. D., & Colburn, J. S. (2014). Functional Brain Networks and White Matter Underlying Theory-of-Mind in Autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 98. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCAN/NSS106 Kana, R. K., Maximo, J. O., Williams, D. L., Keller, T. A., Schipul, S. E., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2015). Aberrant functioning of the theory-of-mind network in children and adolescents with autism. Molecular Autism, 6(1), 59. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0052-x Kana, R. K., Patriquin, M. A., Black, B. S., Channell, M. M., & Wicker, B. (2016). Altered Medial Frontal and Superior Temporal Response to Implicit Processing of Emotions in Autism. Autism Research, 9(1), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1496 Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., Howes, M. J., Jin, R., Secnik, K., Spencer, T., Ustun, T. B., & Walters, E. E. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 245–256. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704002892 Kinoshita, A., Takizawa, R., Yahata, N., Homae, F., Hashimoto, R., Sakakibara, E., Kawasaki, S., Nishimura, Y., Koike, S., & Kasai, K. (2016). Development of a neurofeedback protocol targeting the frontal pole using near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 70(11), 507–516. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12427 Klein, F., Lührs, M., Benitez-Andonegui, A., Roehn, P., & Kranczioch, C. (2022). Performance comparison of systemic activity correction in functional near-infrared spectroscopy for methods with and without short distance channels. Https://Doi.Org/10.1117/1.NPh.10.1.013503, 10(1), 013503. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPH.10.1.013503 Ko, S., & Park, W. (2018). Effects of Quantitative Electroencephalography Based Neurofeedback Training on Autonomous Regulations in Patients with Alcohol Use Disorder. Asian Nursing Research, 12(2), 136–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ANR.2018.05.003 Kohl, S. H., Mehler, D. M. A., Lührs, M., Thibault, R. T., Konrad, K., & Sorger, B. (2020). The potential of functional near-infrared spectroscopy-based neurofeedback – a systematic review and recommendations for best practice. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 14, 594. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINS.2020.00594 Krall, S. C., Rottschy, C., Oberwelland, E., Bzdok, D., Fox, P. T., Eickhoff, S. B., Fink, G. R., & Konrad, K. (2015). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in attention and social 44 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 interaction as revealed by ALE meta-analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 220(2), 587–604. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0803-z Krall, S. C., Volz, L. J., Oberwelland, E., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., & Konrad, K. (2016). The right temporoparietal junction in attention and social interaction: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 796–807. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23068 Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models . Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13 Kvamme, T. L., Ros, T., & Overgaard, M. (2022). Can neurofeedback provide evidence of direct brain-behavior causality? NeuroImage, 258, 119400. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2022.119400 Landry, O., & Parker, A. (2013). A meta-analysis of visual orienting in autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 0, 833. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2013.00833 Mai, X., Zhang, W., Hu, X., Zhen, Z., Xu, Z., Zhang, J., & Liu, C. (2016). Using tDCS to Explore the Role of the Right Temporo-Parietal Junction in Theory of Mind and Cognitive Empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 0(MAR), 380. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2016.00380 Markiewicz, R., Markiewicz-Gospodarek, A., Dobrowolska, B., & Łoza, B. (2021). Improving Clinical, Cognitive, and Psychosocial Dysfunctions in Patients with Schizophrenia: A Neurofeedback Randomized Control Trial. Neural Plasticity, 2021. https://doi.org/10.1155/2021/4488664 Mars, R. B., Sallet, J., Schüffelgen, U., Jbabdi, S., Toni, I., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2012). Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right “temporoparietal junction area”: Evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cerebral Cortex, 22(8), 1894– 1903. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr268 Mathiak, K. A., Alawi, E. M., Koush, Y., Dyck, M., Cordes, J. S., Gaber, T. J., Zepf, F. D., Palomero-Gallagher, N., Sarkheil, P., Bergert, S., Zvyagintsev, M., & Mathiak, K. (2015). Social reward improves the voluntary control over localized brain activity in fMRI-based neurofeedback training. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 136. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00136 McNair, D., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. (1981). POMS Profile of Mood States. Ein Verfahren zur Messung von Stimmungszuständen. Internationale Skalen Für Psychiatrie. Mehler, D. M. A., Sokunbi, M. O., Habes, I., Barawi, K., Subramanian, L., Range, M., Evans, J., Hood, K., Lührs, M., Keedwell, P., Goebel, R., & Linden, D. E. J. (2018). Targeting the affective brain—a randomized controlled trial of real-time fMRI neurofeedback in patients with depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, November 2017, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386- 018-0126-5 Mengotti, P., Käsbauer, A.-S., Fink, G. R., & Vossel, S. (2022). Combined TMS-fMRI Reveals Behavior-Dependent Network Effects of Right Temporoparietal Junction Neurostimulation in an Attentional Belief Updating Task. Cerebral Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHAB511 Müller, V. I., Höhner, Y., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Influence of task instructions and stimuli on the neural network of face processing: An ALE meta-analysis. Cortex, 103, 240–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.011 Murphy, A. C., & Bassett, D. S. (2017). A network neuroscience of neurofeedback for clinical translation. Current Opinion in Biomedical Engineering, 1, 63–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobme.2017.03.003 Nobusako, S., Nishi, Y., Nishi, Y., Shuto, T., Asano, D., Osumi, M., & Morioka, S. (2017). Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation of the Temporoparietal Junction and Inferior Frontal Cortex Improves Imitation-Inhibition and Perspective-Taking with no Effect on the Autism- 45 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 Spectrum Quotient Score. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 84. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNBEH.2017.00084 Noguchi, K., Gel, Y. R., Brunner, E., & Konietschke, F. (2012). nparLD : An R Software Package for the Nonparametric Analysis of Longitudinal Data in Factorial Experiments. Journal of Statistical Software, 50(12). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v050.i12 Pamplona, G. S. P., Heldner, J., Langner, R., Koush, Y., Michels, L., Ionta, S., Scharnowski, F., & Salmon, C. E. G. (2020). Network-based fMRI-neurofeedback training of sustained attention. NeuroImage, 221, 117194. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2020.117194 Paret, C., Goldway, N., Zich, C., Keynan, J. N., Hendler, T., Linden, D., & Cohen Kadosh, K. (2019). Current progress in real-time functional magnetic resonance-based neurofeedback: Methodological challenges and achievements. NeuroImage, 202, 116107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116107 Paulus, C. (2012). Ist die Bildung eines Empathiescores in der deutschen Fassung des IRI sinnvoll? https://doi.org/doi:10.22028/D291-23347 Penner, J., Osuch, E. A., Schaefer, B., Théberge, J., Neufeld, R. W. J., Menon, R. S., Rajakumar, N., & Williamson, P. C. (2018). Temporoparietal Junction Functional Connectivity in Early Schizophrenia and Major Depressive Disorder. Chronic Stress, 2. https://doi.org/10.1177/2470547018815232 Pereira, H. C., Sousa, D., Simões, M., Martins, R., Amaral, C., Lopes, V., Crisóstomo, J., & Castelo- Branco, M. (2021). Effects of anodal multichannel transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on social-cognitive performance in healthy subjects: A randomized sham-controlled crossover pilot study. In Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 264, pp. 259–286). Elsevier B.V. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2021.04.004 Pinti, P., Scholkmann, F., Hamilton, A., Burgess, P., & Tachtsidis, I. (2019). Current Status and Issues Regarding Pre-processing of fNIRS Neuroimaging Data: An Investigation of Diverse Signal Filtering Methods Within a General Linear Model Framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 505. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00505 Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231 R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 510–532. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510 Ros, T., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Zotev, V., Young, K. D., Wood, G., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Wan, F., Vuilleumier, P., Vialatte, F., van de Ville, D., Todder, D., Surmeli, T., Sulzer, J. S., Strehl, U., Sterman, M. B., Steiner, N. J., Sorger, B., Soekadar, S. R., Sitaram, R., … Thibault, R. T. (2020). Consensus on the reporting and experimental design of clinical and cognitive- behavioural neurofeedback studies (CRED-nf checklist). Brain, 143(6), 1674–1685. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awaa009 Roy, L. B., Sparing, R., Fink, G. R., & Hesse, M. D. (2015). Modulation of attention functions by anodal tDCS on right PPC. Neuropsychologia, 74, 96–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.028 Salehinejad, M. A., Paknia, N., Hosseinpour, A. H., Yavari, F., Vicario, C. M., Nitsche, M. A., & Nejati, V. (2021). Contribution of the right temporoparietal junction and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to theory of mind in autism: A randomized, sham-controlled tDCS study. Autism Research, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/AUR.2538 Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2012). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Current Biology, 22(23), 2274–2277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018 46 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023 Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2015). Functional lateralization of temporoparietal junction - imitation inhibition, visual perspective-taking and theory of mind. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 42(8), 2527–2533. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13036 Schmidt, J., & Martin, A. (2016). Neurofeedback Against Binge Eating: A Randomized Controlled Trial in a Female Subclinical Threshold Sample. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2453 Schmidt, J., & Martin, A. (2020). The Influence of Physiological and Psychological Learning Mechanisms in Neurofeedback vs. Mental Imagery Against Binge Eating. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 45(4), 293–305. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10484-020-09486- 9/TABLES/4 Schönenberg, M., Weingärtner, A. L., Weimer, K., & Scheeff, J. (2021). Believing is achieving - On the role of treatment expectation in neurofeedback applications. Progress in Neuro- Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110129 Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE) (tabel) : In Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio. Causal and control beliefs. Generalised Self-Efficacy Scale1st Ed, J Weinman, S Wright, M Johnston. Nfer-Nelson, WindsorUK, 1995, 35–37. Sitaram, R., Ros, T., Stoeckel, L., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Lewis-Peacock, J., Weiskopf, N., Blefari, M. L., Rana, M., Oblak, E., Birbaumer, N., & Sulzer, J. (2017). Closed-loop brain training: the science of neurofeedback. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(2), 86–100. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.164 Soekadar, S. R., Kohl, S. H., Mihara, M., & von Lühmann, A. (2021). Optical brain imaging and its application to neurofeedback. NeuroImage: Clinical, 30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102577 Sorger, B., Scharnowski, F., Linden, D. E. J., Hampson, M., & Young, K. D. (2019). Control freaks: Towards optimal selection of control conditions for fMRI neurofeedback studies. NeuroImage, 186, 256–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.004 Symeonidou, I., Dumontheil, I., Chow, W.-Y., & Breheny, R. (2016). Development of online use of theory of mind during adolescence: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 81–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.007 van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 829–858. https://doi.org/10.1002/HBM.20547 Vossel, S., Weidner, R., Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2009). What is “Odd” in Posner’s Location- cueing Paradigm? Neural Responses to Unexpected Location and Feature Changes Compared. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 30–41. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21003 World Medical Association. (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. JAMA, 310(20), 2191–2194. https://doi.org/10.1001/JAMA.2013.281053 Yamashita, A., Hayasaka, S., Kawato, M., & Imamizu, H. (2017). Connectivity Neurofeedback Training Can Differentially Change Functional Connectivity and Cognitive Performance. Cerebral Cortex, 27(10), 4960–4970. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHX177 Yang, L.-Z., Zhang, W., Wang, W., Yang, Z., Wang, H., Deng, Z.-D., Li, C., Qiu, B., Zhang, D.-R., Kadosh, R. C., Li, H., & Zhang, X. (2020). Neural and Psychological Predictors of Cognitive Enhancement and Impairment from Neurostimulation. Advanced Science, 7(4), 1902863. https://doi.org/10.1002/ADVS.201902863 Yücel, M. A., Lühmann, A. v., Scholkmann, F., Gervain, J., Dan, I., Ayaz, H., Boas, D., Cooper, R. J., Culver, J., Elwell, C. E., Eggebrecht, A., Franceschini, M. A., Grova, C., Homae, F., Lesage, F., Obrig, H., Tachtsidis, I., Tak, S., Tong, Y., … Wolf, M. (2021). Best practices for fNIRS publications. Neurophotonics, 8(1), 012101. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPH.8.1.012101 47 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/imag/article-pdf/doi/10.1162/imag_a_00014/2154906/imag_a_00014.pdf by guest on 07 September 2023Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen
Kohl, S.H., Melies, PAG., Uttecht, J., Lührs, METRO., Campana, l., Mehler, D.M.A., Soekadar, S.R., Viswanathan, S., & imagen

Descargar PDF